Proto-Uralic: Difference between revisions

From FrathWiki
Jump to navigationJump to search
 
(12 intermediate revisions by the same user not shown)
Line 4: Line 4:
'''B.''' = Baltic, '''Cf.''' = 'compare', '''En''' = Enets, '''Er.''' = Erźa, '''Es.''' = Estonian, '''F.''' = Finnish, '''Gmc''' = Germanic, '''H.''' = [[Hungarian]], '''Hi.''' = Hill Mari, '''IA''' = Indo-Aryan, '''IE''' = Indo-European, '''II''' = Indo-Iranian, '''K.''' = Komi, '''Ka.''' = Kamass, '''Kh.''' = [[Khanty]], '''Li.''' = Livonian, '''Ma.''' = [[Mari]], '''Me.''' = Meadow Mari, '''Mk.''' = Mokša, '''Mo.''' = [[Mordvinic]], '''Ms.''' = [[Mansi]], '''N''' = North, '''Ne.''' = Nenets, '''Ng.''' = Nganasan, '''P.''' = [[Permic]], '''PU''' = Proto-Uralic, '''S.''' = Samic / South, '''Se.''' = Selkup, '''Smy.''' = Samoyedic, '''U.''' = Udmurt, '''Ve.''' = Veps, '''Võ.''' =
'''B.''' = Baltic, '''Cf.''' = 'compare', '''En''' = Enets, '''Er.''' = Erźa, '''Es.''' = Estonian, '''F.''' = Finnish, '''Gmc''' = Germanic, '''H.''' = [[Hungarian]], '''Hi.''' = Hill Mari, '''IA''' = Indo-Aryan, '''IE''' = Indo-European, '''II''' = Indo-Iranian, '''K.''' = Komi, '''Ka.''' = Kamass, '''Kh.''' = [[Khanty]], '''Li.''' = Livonian, '''Ma.''' = [[Mari]], '''Me.''' = Meadow Mari, '''Mk.''' = Mokša, '''Mo.''' = [[Mordvinic]], '''Ms.''' = [[Mansi]], '''N''' = North, '''Ne.''' = Nenets, '''Ng.''' = Nganasan, '''P.''' = [[Permic]], '''PU''' = Proto-Uralic, '''S.''' = Samic / South, '''Se.''' = Selkup, '''Smy.''' = Samoyedic, '''U.''' = Udmurt, '''Ve.''' = Veps, '''Võ.''' =
South Estonian (Võro)
South Estonian (Võro)
'''[http://protouralic.wordpress.com/ Now with a blog!]''' <span style="color:red">NB: New URL<span>


__NOTOC__
__NOTOC__
==Development==
==Development==
* Sound changes to [[Finnish]]
* Sound changes to [[Finnish]]
* Sound changes to [[Mordvinic]]
* Sound changes to [[Proto-Samic]]
* Sound changes to [[Proto-Samic]]
* Sound changes to [[Proto-Samoyedic]]
* Sound changes to [[Proto-Samoyedic]]
Line 30: Line 33:
* [[Proto-Uralic/-ma|-mA]]
* [[Proto-Uralic/-ma|-mA]]
* [[Proto-Uralic/-ta|-tA]]
* [[Proto-Uralic/-ta|-tA]]
[[Proto-Uralic/derived|Potential derivativs]] • [[Proto-Uralic/CC|Cluster issues]] • [[Proto-Uralic/harmony|Co-occurrence of coronals]]
[[Proto-Uralic/derived|Potential derivativs]] • [[Proto-Uralic/CC|Cluster issues]] • [[Proto-Uralic/harmony|Co-occurrence of coronals]] • [[Proto-Uralic/WE|West-East discrepancies]]


==Reconstructed phoneme inventory==
==Reconstructed phoneme inventory==


;Vowels:
;Vowels:
<nowiki>*/i ü u e ë o ä a/</nowiki> in the initial syllable. Only a two-way height-based contrast */I A/ is normally reconstructed in later syllables, which may have been realized as [i æ] after front vowels and [ɯ ɑ] after back vowels (ie. with vowel harmony); or as unalternating [ə a]. (That *-i fails to cause palatalization anywhere outside of Finnic seems like a good argument against an original close front value.) These pages will use the notation *a~*ä, *ə. A couple family terms suggest different vowels, including *nato "brother's wife", *kälü "spouse's sister", *wäŋü/*wiŋü "son-in-law".
<nowiki>*/i ü u e ë o ä a/</nowiki> in the initial syllable. Only a two-way height-based contrast */I A/ is normally reconstructed in later syllables, which may have been realized as [i æ] after front vowels and [ɯ ɑ] after back vowels (ie. with vowel harmony); or as unalternating [ə a]. These pages will use the notation *a~*ä, *ə. A couple family terms suggest different vowels, including #nato "brother's wife", #kälü "spouse's sister", #wäŋü/#wiŋü "son-in-law".


It is furthermore unclear if (1st-syllable) *ë was [ɯ] or [ɤ], and *a [ɑ] or [ɒ], but substitution of Indo-Iranian *a by *ë in loans suggests the latter values (unless these particular words are newer loans.)
Opinions vary on if (1st-syllable) *ë was [ɯ] or [ɤ], and *a [ɑ] or [ɒ]. As of July 2014 I ([[User:Tropylium|Tropylium]]) support [ɤ] for the former, as suggested by e.g. the substitution of Indo-Iranian *a by *ë in loans (unless these words are post-PU in date.) The latter seems like an open question; I am investigating the possibility of [ɒ] as the default value, [ɑ] as a positional allophone.


Two "reduced" or "semi-rounded" vowels */ê ô/ have been proposed recently.
Several amendments have been proposed at times:
* Long vowels or equivalent entities have been a popular proposal, but the main evidence, from Finnic, has recently been adequately explained otherwise.
* A number of studies have proposed, on the basis of the Ugric evidence, to reinterpret several quality contrasts as quantity contrasts instead.<sup>[citation needed]</sup> No wholly systemic account of this idea seems to have been presented to this day.
* Korhonen (1988) proposes an *ï, which would have split at an early date to to front and back allophones, the former then developing into standard *ü.
* This *ï has been reshuffled into two "reduced" or "semi-rounded" vowels *ê *ô ([ɪ ʊ]?) by Häkkinen (2007), which also aim to explain some apparent exceptions in the development of *e and *o. In light of some recently identified conditional sound laws, this may not be a necessary hypothesis. (I still suspect that at least a raised allophone of *e might have existed though.)


;Consonants:
;Consonants
Nasals */m n ń ŋ/, voiceless stops/affricates */p t ć č k/, voiceless sibilants */s ś š/, a "laryngeal" *x (likely a voiceless velar fricativ & a recent pre-Uralic split from *k), two "spirants" */ð ðʲ/, two liquids */l r/ and two semivowels */w j/.
Nasals */m n ń ŋ/, voiceless stops/affricates */p t ć č k/, voiceless sibilants */s ś š/, a "laryngeal" */x/ (a voiceless velar fricativ [x] seems like the most plausible sound value, though voiced [ɣ] is not ruled out), two "spirants" */d₁ d₂/ (traditionally interpreted as [ð] and [ðʲ] respectivly, though this is far from certain), two liquids */l r/ and two semivowels */w j/.


<nowiki></nowiki> (as distinct from ?) and *š (as original Proto-Uralic?) are reconstructed less securely than the other consonants. A palatal liquid *ĺ is also found in old reconstructions, but the etyma involved do not really behave (they may be late inter-branch loans). The "palatal spirant" may be the actual palatal liquid; obstruent reflexes are limited to western branches, and external comparisions generally involve laterals. The dental spirant, while also merging with *l in most branches, is however certainly distinct.
The existence of a distinction between and is not certain. *ć can be securely established only in the clusters *ćć, *ńć, where *ś is not found. The Proto-Uralic status of *š is also uncertain: most words with the consonant are not found in languages east to Permic, and frequently moreover probable Indo-European loanwords.


A notable distributional feature was that *ŋ, *x and probably also , *r could not occur word-initially.
A palatal liquid *ĺ is also commonly found in old reconstructions (e.g. in the ''Uralisches Etymologisches Wörterbuch''), but many of the etyma involved do not really behave. Some may be late inter-branch loans. Most others have been by now accounted for by a widespread development *l > *ĺ in Samoyedic. The "palatal spirant" *d₂ may thus have been the actual palatal liquid; obstruent reflexes are limited to western branches, and external comparisions generally involve laterals. (The "dental spirant" also develops to /l/ widely, but was certainly distinct from *l.)


Roots generally had the form (C)V(C)C{A I}, with initial stress; in pronouns and prepositions and the copula also CV; and a single lone-V root, the negativ verb *e-
Three further consonants of dubious nature were supported until the mid-20th century: the palatal shibilant *š́ (distinguished from regular *ś only in Mansi), and the retroflex sonorants *ṇ, *ḷ (distinguished from regular *n, *l only in Khanty).
 
;Phonotactics
A notable distributional feature was that *ŋ, *x and probably also *d₁, *r could not occur word-initially.
 
Roots generally had the form (C)V(C)C{A I}, with initial stress. A couple longer stems of the shape (C)V(C)C{A I}C(I) are also attested. Shorter CV roots occurred in grammatical words: pronouns, prepositions and the copula also CV; there was also a single lone-V root, the negativ verb *e-.
 
See below for allowed consonant clusters.


Basic consonant correspondences (gradation not included in Finno-Samic, asterisks for Mari and Mordvinic largely superfluous):
Basic consonant correspondences (gradation not included in Finno-Samic, asterisks for Mari and Mordvinic largely superfluous):
Line 116: Line 130:
| rowspan="3"| *d, *dʲ || *ð || ∅ || z
| rowspan="3"| *d, *dʲ || *ð || ∅ || z
|-
|-
! *ðʲ
! *d₂
| rowspan="2"| *ð || *l, *-ð- || *ĺ || ɟ || *ĺ || *j || *j
| rowspan="2"| *ð || *l, *-ð- || *ĺ || ɟ || *ĺ || *j || *j
|-
|-
! *ð
! *-d₁-
| *l, *-∅- || rowspan="2"| *l || rowspan="2"| l || rowspan="2"| *l || rowspan="2"| *l, *-ɬ- || *r
| ∅ || *l, *-∅- || rowspan="2"| l || rowspan="2"| *l || rowspan="2"| *l, *-ɬ- || *r
| lost in Permic only intervocally, not in clusters
|-
|-
! *l
! *l
| *l || *l || *l || *l || *l, *j
| *l || *l || *l || *l || *l || *l, *j
| In Kh. also irregularly *l → *ɭ
| In Kh. also irregularly *l → *ɭ
|-
|-
Line 132: Line 147:
| *r || *r || *r || *r || *r || r || *r || *r || *r
| *r || *r || *r || *r || *r || r || *r || *r || *r
<!--
<!--
|-
! *NP
| *NP || *NB || *NB || *NB || *B || B || *NP || *NP || *NP
|-
! *nč
| *nt || *nc || *nž || *č || *dž, *ž || ?? || *nš || *nč || *nč
|-
! *nś
| *s || *nč || *ź ? || *nć, *ž || *dź || gy || *nć || *nć || *ns
|-
|-
! *-kt-
! *-kt-
Line 152: Line 158:
| * || * ||  || * || * ||  || * || * || *-->
| * || * ||  || * || * ||  || * || * || *-->
|}
|}
Major consonant developments involving specific '''vowel environments''' include:
* the widespread loss of *w before non-open labial vowels, *j before non-open front vowels
** best retained in Permic and Khanty
* the labialization of *k, *ŋ, *x in Mansi to *kʷ, *ŋʷ, *w before and after labial vowels
* the assibilation of *t in Finnic (of any origin, i.e. < *t, *č, *d₁, *d₂) to *c before *i
Major soundlaws affecting '''consonant clusters''' include:
* the widespread loss or vocalization of *j and *w
** only Samic consistently retains these, though Finnic in most cases as well and Samoyedic also frequently
** the cluster *lj has widely coalesced to /lʲ/; in Mordvinic all j-clusters yield palatalized consonants
** Mansi and Khanty have retained a few direct traces of *w (generally as delabialized *ɣ)
** indirect traces of such clusters are widely found in the development of vowels
* the shortening of geminates everywhere except in Samic and Finnic
** generally leading to new medial voiceless stops/affricates, after the lenition of the original ones
** geminates other than *kk are merged with the corresponding singletons in Mari, Mansi, Khanty and Samoyedic
* the assimilation of *mt to *nt in Finnic, Mordvinic, Mansi, and probably Permic & Hungarian
* the denasalization of nasal + stop/affricate clusters to voiced stops/affricates in Permic and Hungarian
** the pre-Hungarian voiced affricates have lost their sibilancy: *nč *ńć > *[ʤ ʥ] > /r ɟ/
* the metathesis of *k in Mansi and Khanty, when following a heterorganic consonant
* the loss of *k in Samoyedic, under the same conditions
* after the loss of final vowels, the breaking of most heterorganic consonant clusters by an epenthetic schwa in Hungarian, Mansi and Khanty
** entirely general in Khanty, somewhat limited in Hungarian and Mansi; in many cases consonant clusters are still attested in Old Hungarian
** some traces of a similar process are found in Mari (for *ŋ + sibilant) and Finnic (for *kś) as well
* the loss of *p before other obstruents in Mansi
* the "palatality metathesis" (*Ć-lk, *d₂k, *d₂w >) *ĺɣ > /lɟ/ in Hungarian
* the assimilation of *lm (incl. earlier *d₁m) to *nm in Permic
* the merger and lenition of *pt *kt to *ht in Finnic, *ft in Mordvinic
* the simplification of *čč, *kš to *h in Finnic (via *th, *kh?)


==Medial consonant clusters==
==Medial consonant clusters==


Words included chiefly from appendix from this: http://urn.fi/URN:NBN:fi-fe20071746 (see comments in table code)
Includes chiefly the words from the appendix in Häkkinen (2007).


CF: http://books.google.fi/books?id=TM2NQ78dP2wC&pg=PA492&dq=phonotactics+of+PFU
Cf. Sammallahti (1988): http://books.google.fi/books?id=TM2NQ78dP2wC&pg=PA492&dq=phonotactics+of+PFU


{|
{|
! colspan="2"|2nd →<br/>1st ↓
! colspan="2"|2nd →<br/>1st ↓
! p !! t !! č !! k !! s !! ś !! š !! ð !! l !! r !! w !! j !! m !! n !! ŋ
! p !! t !! č !! k !! s !! ś !! š !! d₁ !! l !! r !! w !! j !! m !! n !! ŋ
! ∑
! ∑
! width="25"| !! Notes !! width="25"| !! Frequency color code
! width="25"| !! Notes !! width="25"| !! Frequency color code
Line 167: Line 202:
! rowspan="3" valign="center" style="background:#f0f0f0"| N
! rowspan="3" valign="center" style="background:#f0f0f0"| N
! m
! m
| mp <!--lämpe-->|| style="background:#fff0c0"| mt <!--ëmta- omte tumte---> || … ||
| style="background:#ffffe0"| mp <!--kompa lämpe-->|| style="background:#fff0c0"| mt <!--ëmta- omte tumte---> || … ||
| ms <!--pImseme-->|| style="background:#ffffe0"| mś <!--n'ims'a kums'e (= Häkk. kuxs'e)-->||
| ms <!--pImseme-->|| style="background:#ffffe0"| mś <!--n'ims'a kums'e (= Häkk. kuxs'e)-->||
| colspan="3" rowspan="3" align="center" valign="center" style="background:#f0f0f0"| N/A
| colspan="3" rowspan="3" align="center" valign="center" style="background:#f0f0f0"| N/A
| colspan="5" rowspan="11" align="center" valign="center" style="background:#f0f0f0"| N/A
| colspan="5" rowspan="11" align="center" valign="center" style="background:#f0f0f0"| N/A
| 7
| 8
| || mostly i-stems except *ńimśa
| || mostly i-stems except *kompa *ńimśa
| || single root
| || single root
|-
|-
Line 192: Line 227:
! p
! p
| pp <!--säppa-->|| style="background:#ffffe0"| pt <!--jupta- ëpte-->|| ||
| pp <!--säppa-->|| style="background:#ffffe0"| pt <!--jupta- ëpte-->|| ||
| style="background:#ffffe0"| ps <!--ipse kupsa--->|| pś <!--lëps'e-->|| … || <!--däpdä-->
| style="background:#ffffe0"| ps <!--ipse kupsa--->|| pś <!--lëps'e-->|| … || pd₁ <!--l/däpdä-->
| … ||
| … ||
| 7
| 7
Line 222: Line 257:
! k
! k
| || style="background:#ffc8c0"| kt <!--kakta tUkta tUkte ïkta täkta-->|| … || kk <!--lükka-->
| || style="background:#ffc8c0"| kt <!--kakta tUkta tUkte ïkta täkta-->|| … || kk <!--lükka-->
| style="background:#fff0c0"| ks <!--peksa- sukse mïksa-->|| style="background:#fff0c0"| kś <!--s'üks'e n'uks'e n'ëks'eme-->|| () <!--mIkSe-->||
| style="background:#fff0c0"| ks <!--peksa- sukse mïksa-->|| style="background:#fff0c0"| kś <!--s'üks'e n'uks'e n'ëks'eme-->|| style="background:#ffffe0"| kš <!--mIkSe wokSe-->||
| … || …
| … || …
| 12
| 13
| || *probably separate loans in FP and H.<!-- since reflected differently from *ks in the latter-->
| || but *mekšə "bee" probably separate loans in FP and H.<!-- since reflected differently from *ks in the latter-->
|-
|-
! rowspan="3" valign="center" style="background:#f0f0f0"| S
! rowspan="3" valign="center" style="background:#f0f0f0"| S
Line 253: Line 288:
! r
! r
| style="background:#ffffe0"| rp <!--korpi tUrpa--->|| style="background:#ffffe0"| rt <!--pärta sarta-->|| … || style="background:#fff0c0"| rk <!--sarka kurke purke n'ërke-->
| style="background:#ffffe0"| rp <!--korpi tUrpa--->|| style="background:#ffffe0"| rt <!--pärta sarta-->|| … || style="background:#fff0c0"| rk <!--sarka kurke purke n'ërke-->
| rw <!--kIrwa---> || …
| style="background:#ffffe0"| rw <!--jerwV- kIrwa---> || …
| rm <!--n'urme--> || … || …
| rm <!--n'urme--> || … || …
| 10
| 11
| || mostly back-harmonic
| || mostly back-harmonic
|-
|-
| align="center" valign="center" | (?)
| align="center" valign="center" | (?)
! ð<sup>(</sup>ʲ<sup>)</sup>
! d₁/d₂
| || || || ð<sup>(</sup>ʲ<sup>)</sup>k <!--s'ëd(')ka-->
| || || || d₁k <!--s'ëdka-->
| ðʲw <!--käd'wa-->||
| d₂w <!--käd'wa-->||
| || ||
| dₓm <!--s'eDmä--> || ||
| 2
| 3
|-
|-
! rowspan="2" valign="center" style="background:#f0f0f0"| sV
! rowspan="2" valign="center" style="background:#f0f0f0"| sV
! w
! w
| || … || colspan="5" rowspan="2" align="center" valign="center" style="background:#f0f0f0"| N/A?
| || … || ||
| style="background:#ffffe0"| <!--käwde täwde--->
| ws <!--kowse--> || ||
| style="background:#fff0c0"| wd₁ <!--owdem käwde täwde--->
| wl <!--lewle-->|| || || wj <!--jewje-->
| wl <!--lewle-->|| || || wj <!--jewje-->
| || wn <!--säwnV--> || wŋ <!--s'awNa-->
| || wn <!--säwnV--> || style="background:#ffffe0"| wŋ <!--owNe s'awNa-->
| 6
| 9
| || after e ä a only
| || after e ä a o only
|-
|-
! j
! j
| … || … ||
| … || … || || ||
| || ||
| || … || style="background:#fff0c0"| jw <!--päjwa kajwa ojwa--> || …
| || … || style="background:#fff0c0"| jw <!--päjwa kajwa ojwa--> || …
| jm <!--äjmä-->|| … || style="background:#ffffe0"| jŋ <!--wajNe ajNe-->
| jm <!--äjmä-->|| … || style="background:#ffffe0"| jŋ <!--wajNe ajNe-->
Line 282: Line 319:
|-
|-
! colspan="2" align="center"| ∑
! colspan="2" align="center"| ∑
| 4 || 19 || 7 || 37
| 5 || 19 || 7 || 37
| 7 || 10 || (1) || 3
| 8 || 10 || 2 || 4
| 1 || 0 || 6 || 5
| 1 || 0 || 9 || 5
| 7 || 1 || 4
| 8 || 1 || 5
|}
|}


Not all blank'd cells were necessarily impossible: some roots of limited distribution have examples of *kč, *pš, *kš, *pl, *ćl, *kl, *kr, *čt, *tt, *st, *śt, *št, *šk, *ćk, *nš, *ŋš, *mč, *lp, *lč, *ln, *rč, *rj, *rn, *rŋ, *ðm, *jp, *jt, *jr, *jj, *jń, *wt (mark'd with an ellipsis in the table).
Not all blank'd cells were necessarily impossible: some roots of limited distribution have examples of *kč, *pš, *kš, *pl, *ćl, *kl, *kr, *čt, *tt, *st, *śt, *št, *šk, *ćk, *nš, *ŋš, *mč, *lp, *lč, *ln, *rč, *rj, *rn, *rŋ, *jp, *jt, *jr, *jj, *jń, *wt (mark'd with an ellipsis in the table).
 
==Literature==
;Current standard literature
* Janhunen, Juha (1981): Uralilaisen kantakielen sanastosta
* Janhunen, Juha (1982): On the structure of Proto-Uralic
* Sammallahti, Pekka (1988): Historical Phonology of the Uralic languages
 
; Nonstandard proposals
* Häkkinen, Jaakko (2007): ''Kantauralin murteutuminen vokaalivastaavuuksien valossa''. Master's thesis. http://urn.fi/URN:NBN:fi-fe20071746
* Korhonen, Mikko (1988): Uralilaisten kielten jälkitavujen vokaaliston historiaa
 
;Dated reconstructions
* Collinder, Björn (1960): ''Comparative Grammar of the Uralic languages''
* Itkonen, Erkki (1946): Zur Frage nach der Entwicklung des Vokalismus der ersten Silbe in den finnisch-ugrischen Sprachen, insbesondere in Mordwinischen. ''Finnisch-Ugrische Forschungen'' 29.
* Steinitz, Wolfgang (1944): ''Geschichte des finnisch-ugrischen Vokalismus''. Stockholm.
* Sammallahti, Pekka (1979): Über die Laut- und Morphemstruktur der uralischen Grundsprache. ''Finnisch-Ugrische Forschungen'' 43.


{{Natlangs}}
{{Natlangs}}
[[Category:Reconstructed languages]]
[[Category:Reconstructed languages]]

Latest revision as of 12:59, 3 July 2014

Fully a work in progress. Mistakes may occur.

Abbreviations used on these pages: B. = Baltic, Cf. = 'compare', En = Enets, Er. = Erźa, Es. = Estonian, F. = Finnish, Gmc = Germanic, H. = Hungarian, Hi. = Hill Mari, IA = Indo-Aryan, IE = Indo-European, II = Indo-Iranian, K. = Komi, Ka. = Kamass, Kh. = Khanty, Li. = Livonian, Ma. = Mari, Me. = Meadow Mari, Mk. = Mokša, Mo. = Mordvinic, Ms. = Mansi, N = North, Ne. = Nenets, Ng. = Nganasan, P. = Permic, PU = Proto-Uralic, S. = Samic / South, Se. = Selkup, Smy. = Samoyedic, U. = Udmurt, Ve. = Veps, Võ. = South Estonian (Võro)

Now with a blog! NB: New URL


Development

Data subpages

In the vowel tables, bold marks vocalic irregularities, italic uncertainties in what the regular vocalic reflex is, red consonantal irregularities.

Close *i *ï?*u
Mid *e ~ *ê ~ *E *o ~ *ô ~ *O
Open *a, *ë

Known derivativs with

Potential derivativsCluster issuesCo-occurrence of coronalsWest-East discrepancies

Reconstructed phoneme inventory

Vowels

*/i ü u e ë o ä a/ in the initial syllable. Only a two-way height-based contrast */I A/ is normally reconstructed in later syllables, which may have been realized as [i æ] after front vowels and [ɯ ɑ] after back vowels (ie. with vowel harmony); or as unalternating [ə a]. These pages will use the notation *a~*ä, *ə. A couple family terms suggest different vowels, including #nato "brother's wife", #kälü "spouse's sister", #wäŋü/#wiŋü "son-in-law".

Opinions vary on if (1st-syllable) *ë was [ɯ] or [ɤ], and *a [ɑ] or [ɒ]. As of July 2014 I (Tropylium) support [ɤ] for the former, as suggested by e.g. the substitution of Indo-Iranian *a by *ë in loans (unless these words are post-PU in date.) The latter seems like an open question; I am investigating the possibility of [ɒ] as the default value, [ɑ] as a positional allophone.

Several amendments have been proposed at times:

  • Long vowels or equivalent entities have been a popular proposal, but the main evidence, from Finnic, has recently been adequately explained otherwise.
  • A number of studies have proposed, on the basis of the Ugric evidence, to reinterpret several quality contrasts as quantity contrasts instead.[citation needed] No wholly systemic account of this idea seems to have been presented to this day.
  • Korhonen (1988) proposes an *ï, which would have split at an early date to to front and back allophones, the former then developing into standard *ü.
  • This *ï has been reshuffled into two "reduced" or "semi-rounded" vowels *ê *ô ([ɪ ʊ]?) by Häkkinen (2007), which also aim to explain some apparent exceptions in the development of *e and *o. In light of some recently identified conditional sound laws, this may not be a necessary hypothesis. (I still suspect that at least a raised allophone of *e might have existed though.)
Consonants

Nasals */m n ń ŋ/, voiceless stops/affricates */p t ć č k/, voiceless sibilants */s ś š/, a "laryngeal" */x/ (a voiceless velar fricativ [x] seems like the most plausible sound value, though voiced [ɣ] is not ruled out), two "spirants" */d₁ d₂/ (traditionally interpreted as [ð] and [ðʲ] respectivly, though this is far from certain), two liquids */l r/ and two semivowels */w j/.

The existence of a distinction between *ć and *ś is not certain. *ć can be securely established only in the clusters *ćć, *ńć, where *ś is not found. The Proto-Uralic status of *š is also uncertain: most words with the consonant are not found in languages east to Permic, and frequently moreover probable Indo-European loanwords.

A palatal liquid *ĺ is also commonly found in old reconstructions (e.g. in the Uralisches Etymologisches Wörterbuch), but many of the etyma involved do not really behave. Some may be late inter-branch loans. Most others have been by now accounted for by a widespread development *l > *ĺ in Samoyedic. The "palatal spirant" *d₂ may thus have been the actual palatal liquid; obstruent reflexes are limited to western branches, and external comparisions generally involve laterals. (The "dental spirant" also develops to /l/ widely, but was certainly distinct from *l.)

Three further consonants of dubious nature were supported until the mid-20th century: the palatal shibilant *š́ (distinguished from regular *ś only in Mansi), and the retroflex sonorants *ṇ, *ḷ (distinguished from regular *n, *l only in Khanty).

Phonotactics

A notable distributional feature was that *ŋ, *x and probably also *d₁, *r could not occur word-initially.

Roots generally had the form (C)V(C)C{A I}, with initial stress. A couple longer stems of the shape (C)V(C)C{A I}C(I) are also attested. Shorter CV roots occurred in grammatical words: pronouns, prepositions and the copula also CV; there was also a single lone-V root, the negativ verb *e-.

See below for allowed consonant clusters.

Basic consonant correspondences (gradation not included in Finno-Samic, asterisks for Mari and Mordvinic largely superfluous):

C Finnic S. Mordv. Mari Permic Hung. Ms. Kh. Smy. Comments
*m *m *m *m, *v *m *m m, -v- *m *m *m Sporadic lenition in Mo, H. Regular in suffixes in H.
*n *n *n *n, *ń- / _F *n,
*-ń- / F_F
*n n *n *n *n
*ń,
*n- /#_B
ń
*-ŋ *v, *ː *j / F_,
*v / B_
ń / F_,
n / C_,
m / B_
g *ŋk *ŋk Irregularly split in ObU (the more general development is *ŋk)

Retained in some Erzya & Udmurt dialects

*w *v *v *w *v v *w *w *w
*-w- *j / F_,
∅ / B_
*v → -ː-
*-x- *k *j
*-k- *k *j, *v Mo. split by vowel backness/frontness
*k k *k *k, *g k, h /_B *k *k *k Stop voicing irregularly split in P.
*p *p *p *p, *-v- *p, *-w- *p, *b f, -v- *p *p *p
*t, *h *c *č, *dž č, š
*s *ś, *-ć- *ć, *-ź- *ć, *dź č, s *ć, *s *ć, *s *s
ś, -ź- *š, -ž- *ś, *-ź- *s, *š s *s
*s *s *s, *-z- *s, *-z- *t *t
*h *š, *-ž- *š, *-ž-
*t *t *t *t, *tʲ *t *t, *d t *t
*-t- *d, *dʲ z
*d₂ *l, *-ð- ɟ *j *j
*-d₁- *l, *-∅- l *l *l, *-ɬ- *r lost in Permic only intervocally, not in clusters
*l *l *l *l *l *l *l, *j In Kh. also irregularly *l → *ɭ
*j *j, *ː *j *j *j *j j, ɟ *j *j *j
*r *r *r *r *r *r r *r *r *r

Major consonant developments involving specific vowel environments include:

  • the widespread loss of *w before non-open labial vowels, *j before non-open front vowels
    • best retained in Permic and Khanty
  • the labialization of *k, *ŋ, *x in Mansi to *kʷ, *ŋʷ, *w before and after labial vowels
  • the assibilation of *t in Finnic (of any origin, i.e. < *t, *č, *d₁, *d₂) to *c before *i

Major soundlaws affecting consonant clusters include:

  • the widespread loss or vocalization of *j and *w
    • only Samic consistently retains these, though Finnic in most cases as well and Samoyedic also frequently
    • the cluster *lj has widely coalesced to /lʲ/; in Mordvinic all j-clusters yield palatalized consonants
    • Mansi and Khanty have retained a few direct traces of *w (generally as delabialized *ɣ)
    • indirect traces of such clusters are widely found in the development of vowels
  • the shortening of geminates everywhere except in Samic and Finnic
    • generally leading to new medial voiceless stops/affricates, after the lenition of the original ones
    • geminates other than *kk are merged with the corresponding singletons in Mari, Mansi, Khanty and Samoyedic
  • the assimilation of *mt to *nt in Finnic, Mordvinic, Mansi, and probably Permic & Hungarian
  • the denasalization of nasal + stop/affricate clusters to voiced stops/affricates in Permic and Hungarian
    • the pre-Hungarian voiced affricates have lost their sibilancy: *nč *ńć > *[ʤ ʥ] > /r ɟ/
  • the metathesis of *k in Mansi and Khanty, when following a heterorganic consonant
  • the loss of *k in Samoyedic, under the same conditions
  • after the loss of final vowels, the breaking of most heterorganic consonant clusters by an epenthetic schwa in Hungarian, Mansi and Khanty
    • entirely general in Khanty, somewhat limited in Hungarian and Mansi; in many cases consonant clusters are still attested in Old Hungarian
    • some traces of a similar process are found in Mari (for *ŋ + sibilant) and Finnic (for *kś) as well
  • the loss of *p before other obstruents in Mansi
  • the "palatality metathesis" (*Ć-lk, *d₂k, *d₂w >) *ĺɣ > /lɟ/ in Hungarian
  • the assimilation of *lm (incl. earlier *d₁m) to *nm in Permic
  • the merger and lenition of *pt *kt to *ht in Finnic, *ft in Mordvinic
  • the simplification of *čč, *kš to *h in Finnic (via *th, *kh?)

Medial consonant clusters

Includes chiefly the words from the appendix in Häkkinen (2007).

Cf. Sammallahti (1988): http://books.google.fi/books?id=TM2NQ78dP2wC&pg=PA492&dq=phonotactics+of+PFU

2nd →
1st ↓
p t č k s ś š d₁ l r w j m n ŋ Notes Frequency color code
N m mp mt ms N/A N/A 8 mostly i-stems except *kompa *ńimśa single root
n nt 16 mostly back-harmonic a-stems; *ns → *nč? two roots
ŋ ŋt ŋk ŋs 9 back-harmonic or *ä 3-4 roots
P p pp pt ps pd₁ 7 5-6 roots
t tk 3 all front-harmonic ə-stems 10+ roots
č čč čk 3 Most suspiciously none
ć
k kt kk ks 13 but *mekšə "bee" probably separate loans in FP and H.
S s sk N/A 2 both o_ə
ś śk 5 mostly ə-stems + *wäśka
š
L l lt lk lw lj lm 25 *lw *lj only a-stems; *lt may be derived ← *-lk-t-
r rp rt rk rw rm 11 mostly back-harmonic
(?) d₁/d₂ d₁k d₂w dₓm 3
sV w ws wd₁ wl wj wn 9 after e ä a o only
j jw jm 6 after ä a o only
5 19  37 8 10 2 4 1 0 9 5 8 1 5

Not all blank'd cells were necessarily impossible: some roots of limited distribution have examples of *kč, *pš, *kš, *pl, *ćl, *kl, *kr, *čt, *tt, *st, *śt, *št, *šk, *ćk, *nš, *ŋš, *mč, *lp, *lč, *ln, *rč, *rj, *rn, *rŋ, *jp, *jt, *jr, *jj, *jń, *wt (mark'd with an ellipsis in the table).

Literature

Current standard literature
  • Janhunen, Juha (1981): Uralilaisen kantakielen sanastosta
  • Janhunen, Juha (1982): On the structure of Proto-Uralic
  • Sammallahti, Pekka (1988): Historical Phonology of the Uralic languages
Nonstandard proposals
  • Häkkinen, Jaakko (2007): Kantauralin murteutuminen vokaalivastaavuuksien valossa. Master's thesis. http://urn.fi/URN:NBN:fi-fe20071746
  • Korhonen, Mikko (1988): Uralilaisten kielten jälkitavujen vokaaliston historiaa
Dated reconstructions
  • Collinder, Björn (1960): Comparative Grammar of the Uralic languages
  • Itkonen, Erkki (1946): Zur Frage nach der Entwicklung des Vokalismus der ersten Silbe in den finnisch-ugrischen Sprachen, insbesondere in Mordwinischen. Finnisch-Ugrische Forschungen 29.
  • Steinitz, Wolfgang (1944): Geschichte des finnisch-ugrischen Vokalismus. Stockholm.
  • Sammallahti, Pekka (1979): Über die Laut- und Morphemstruktur der uralischen Grundsprache. Finnisch-Ugrische Forschungen 43.
This article is one of quite a few pages about Natlangs.

Indo-european natlangs:

Balto-Slavic Natlangs: Czech * Russian
Celtic Natlangs: Revived Middle Cornish * Pictish
Germanic Natlangs:
North Germanic Natlangs: Norwegian
West Germanic Natlangs: Anglo-Saxon * Dutch * English (Old English * Middle English * Modern English * Scots) * German (High German * Low German)
Indo-Iranian Natlangs: Pahlavi
Italic Natlangs: French * Italian * Latin * Spanish
Debated: Cimmerian

Uralic Natlangs: Finnish * Khanty * Mansi * Mordvinic * Proto-Uralic
Altaic (controversial): Japanese
Sino-Tibetan Natlangs:
Uto-Aztecan Natlangs: Nahuatl

-

Isolate Natlangs: Basque * *
Hypothetical/debated Natlangs and Natlang families: Danubian * Europic (obsolete)