User:Masako/pataka: Difference between revisions

From FrathWiki
Jump to navigationJump to search
Line 1: Line 1:
== Content words ==
== Content words ==
=== Verbs ===
'''Kala''' relies on analytic serial verb constructions, and can therefore get by with very little verbal morphology. Each verb has at most two possible forms: the [[wp:Dynamic_verb|''active'']] and the [[wp:Stative_verb|''stative'']]. Passivity is marked on the subject thus verbs are unmarked and must be analyzed based on surrounding morphology. Active verbs solely denote actions and occurrences and never states in Kala. Stative verbs are the words that modify nouns in an attributive and often adjectival way. They often express a state like a quality or result.
==== Passivity ====
The passive voice is formed by prefixing '''e'''- ([[wp:Patient_(grammar)|patient marker]]) to pronouns, and the object marker '''ke''' for nouns, or '''nya''' "for, by, via".
{{col-begin}}
{{col-break}}
* '''ha yatsi'''
: <small>3sg bite</small>
: ''He bites.''
* '''ha yapo'''
: <small>3sg build</small>
: ''He builds.''
{{col-break}}
* '''eha yatsi'''
: <small>P.3sg bite</small>
: ''He is bitten.''
* '''etla nya ha yapo'''
: <small>P.4sg by 3sg build</small>
: ''It is built by him.''
{{col-end}}
==== Tense ====
Kala has three simple tenses; past, present, and future. Present tense is unmarked. However, past ('''-ye''') and future ('''-tli''') tenses can be modified to include immediate future ("''is about to''..."), distant future ("''will...in a long while''"), recent past ("''just'' ..."), and remote past ("...''a long while ago''"). These distinctions are made with the augmentative and diminutive endings '''-ha''' and '''-hi'''.
* The present tense can show immediacy by using the adverb '''ima''', "now; at this time":
:'''ima mita ina''' - <small>now dog eat</small> - ''The dog is eating right now.''
* If a temporal adverb is used, the tense suffix may be omitted:
:'''yomaye nam ina''' - <small>yesterday 1pl eat</small> - We ate yesterday.
{| class="wikitable" style="text-align: center; width: 800px;"
|+ eme
|-
!
! Kala
! gloss
! English
|-
! Present
| '''mita ina''' || <small>dog eat</small> || ''The dog eats.''
|-
! Past
| '''mita inaye''' || <small>dog eat-PST</small> || ''The dog ate.''
|-
! Recent Past
| '''mita inayehi'''<br>or '''-hye''' || <small>dog eat-REC</small> || ''The dog just ate.''<br>(action just finished)
|-
! Remote Past
| '''mita inayeha''' || <small>dog eat-REM</small> || ''The dog ate long ago.''<br>(before the lifetime of the speaker)
|-
! Future
| '''mita inatli''' || <small>dog eat-FUT</small> || ''The dog will eat.''
|-
! Immediate Future
| '''mita inatlihi'''<br>or '''-tlai''' || <small>dog eat-IMM</small> || ''The dog will eat soon.''<br>(within the day)
|-
! Distant Future
| '''mita inatliha''' || <small>dog eat-DIS</small> || ''The dog will eat a long while from now.''<br>(months from now)
|}
==== Aspect ====
There are four aspects in Kala. The progressive, also called the [[Wikipedia:Continuous_and_progressive_aspects|continuous]] ['''CONT'''], this is used to express an incomplete action or state in progress at a specific time. It is marked with '''-nko''', from '''nkoso''' - "to continue; proceed; progress". The [[Wikipedia:Perfective_aspect|perfective]] aspect indicates that an action is completed ['''PFV''']. It is often translated by the English present perfect (''have done some-thing''). It is marked with '''-pua''', from '''opua''' - "to end; finish; complete". The [[Wikipedia:Inchoative_aspect|inchoative]] aspect refers to the beginning of a state ['''INCH''']. It is marked with '''-mu''', from '''mula''' - "to begin; start; initiate". The [[Wikipedia:Frequentative|frequentative]]  aspect refers to a repeated action ['''FREQ''']. It is marked with '''-nua''', from '''nua''' - "frequent; often; regular".
{| class="wikitable" style="text-align: center; width: 600px;"
|+ ti'a
|-
!
! Kala
! gloss
! English
|-
! Progressive
| '''mita inanko''' || <small>dog eat-CONT</small> || ''The dog is eating.''
|-
! Perfective
| '''mita inapua''' || <small>dog eat-PFV</small> || ''The dog has eaten.''
|-
! Inchoative
| '''mita inamu''' || <small>dog eat-INCH</small> || ''The dog begins to eat.''
|-
! Frequentative
| '''mita inanua''' || <small>dog eat-FREQ</small> || ''The dog eats often.''
|}
==== Mood ====
The negative mood (always marked finally) is indicated by the suffix –'''k''' or '''–nke''' (when the last syllable contains /k/).
* '''mita inayek''' - <small>dog eat-PST-NEG</small> - ''The dog did not eat.''
* '''mita mokunke''' - <small>dog sleep-NEG</small> - ''The dog does not sleep.''
==== Affix Ordering ====
Verbs can be marked with several suffixes to add or change meaning. The modals and tense affixes can be added in different order to a verb to create a new meaning…their placement is not always fixed. The negative, adjectival, and plural ending are always final, while other affixes can be varied, but in general they should be ordered:
<tt>STEM-(SIZE/IMPORTANCE)-(MOOD)-(ASPECT)-(TENSE)-(NEGATIVE)</tt>
Example:
{| class="wikitable" style="text-align: center; width: 600px;"
|+
|-
! Verb Stem
! Size/Importance
! Mood
! Aspect
! Tense
! Negative
|-
| '''empa''' || '''-hi''' || '''-pa'''  || '''-nko''' || '''-ye''' || '''-k'''
|-
| run || DIM || ABIL || PROG  || PST || NEG
|}
* '''na empahipankoyek'''
: <small>1SG run-DIM-able-PROG-PST-NEG</small>
: ''I was not able to keep jogging.''


=== adjectives ===
=== adjectives ===

Revision as of 04:54, 6 January 2017

Content words

adjectives

Kala does not have adjectives as a distinct part of speech. Instead, many intransitive verbs can be used as adjectives. This leaves open to interpretation many phrases.

  • mita inya - dog be.hungry
    • The dog hungers.
    • The dog is hungry.
    • The hungry dog.
    • A hungry dog.
  • tsaka ketlahi - house be.red-DIM
    • The house is a little red.
    • The light-red house.
    • A pale red house.
  • taki saua - coat be.wet
    • The coat is wet.
    • The wet coat.
    • A wet coat.
  • umalo tahaku - horse-PL be.big-extreme
    • The horses are extremely large.
    • The very big horses.

comparison

In Kala the concepts of comparative and superlative degree of an adjective (verb) are merged into a single form, the elative. How this form is understood or translated depends upon context and definiteness. In the absence of comparison, the elative conveys the notion of “greatest”, “supreme.”

  • tsaka hayo ke nayo tahaka
house 3sg.POSS O 1sg.POSS big-AUG
His house is bigger than mine.
  • ke mauam tayo yanaha
O flower.PL 2sg.POSS yellow-AUG
Your flowers are the most yellow.
  • iyapo ke tsaka tayo pakoha
PROX-building O home 2sg new-AUG
This building is newer than your home.

relative

In a relative clause, the verb has the suffix -tle (or -le if the final syllable contains /tl/) added to it. The order of the words in relative clauses remains the same as in regular clauses. The use of participles in Kala is rather different than in English and at first sight is difficult to understand. This is mainly due to the fact that the relative pronouns who, what, which, where are not used in Kala as in English.

  • yalapa - to be able to walk produces: yalapatle - who/which/that can walk
  • yalapak - to not be able to walk produces: yalapanketle - who/which/that can't walk

This nominalizes the verb in some cases, and makes it possible for it to be either the subject or the object.

  • na ke tlaka nya inama talatle unya
1sg O man for eat-time come-REL know
I know the man who is coming to lunch.
  • ke naka patlole pako
O woman sweep-REL young
The woman who is sweeping is young.

The relative suffix is most often in the final position. In some cases, it may be followed by the negative -k.

  • itsaka na sutahuetle
PROX-house 1sg reside-LOC-REL
This is the house in which I live.
  • itsaka na sutahueyetlek
PROX-house 1sg reside-LOC-PST-REL-NEG
This is the house in which I did not live.


adpositionals

Kala does not have prepositions (or postpositions) as a distinct part of speech. Instead, many locative verbs can be used as adpositionals, in which case they precede the noun they modify. There is one general locative (-hue) which is affixed to nouns (and occasionally verbs) to indicate the sense of “at; in; on”. Here are some common verbs used as adpositions:

  • pahe - against; touching
  • pa'e - apart from; other than; except for
  • paye - beyond; exceeding; farther than
  • pue - after; back; behind; rear
  • tahe - below; beneath; under
  • ka'e - to; towards; at [moving toward]
  • kaye - around; encircling; surrounding
  • mahe - around; approximate; close to
  • ma'a - with [accompanied by / furnished with]
  • ma'e - before; in front
  • maye - between; among
  • nahe - in [located inside of]; internal
  • nyaue - outside of; exterior to
  • sahe - across; opposite; other side
  • saye - along; following [a line]
  • hue / -hue - at [in the same location as] [LOC]
  • tsa'e - across; through
  • ua'e - above; over / on
  • uaye - from [moving out of or away from]
  • ya'e - near; close to
  • yomo - to the right of
  • yoso - to the left of


Numbers

Kala number English Kala number English Kala number English
ye'o 0 zero tsa'o 6 six nya'o 500 five hundred
na'o 1 one ka'o 7 seven tle'o 103 (one) thousand
ta'o 2 two pa'o 8 eight mue'o 104 ten thousand
ha'o 3 three sa'o 9 nine kye'o 105 (one) hundred thousand
ma'o 4 four ue'o 10 ten nte'o 106 (one) million
ya'o 5 five nye'o 100 (one) hundred hue'o 109 (one) billion

Forming Larger Numbers

  • uena'o - eleven / 11
  • taue'o - twenty / 20
  • nyeka'o - one hundred seven / 107
  • hanyetauetsa'o (long form) / hatatsa'o (short form) - three hundred twenty six / 326
  • tsatletauema'o - six thousand and twenty four / 6024

Other Number Forms

Kala number English ordinal multiple fractional
na'o 1 one kina'o
first
tina'o
once
-
ueta'o 12 twelve kiueta'o
twelfth
tiueta'o
twelve times
iueta'o
a twelfth
yauema'o
(yama'o)
54 fifty four kiyama'o
fifty fourth
tiyama'o
54 times
iyama'o
a fifty fourth
nyetsa'o 106 one hundred (and) six kinyetsa'o
106th
tinyetsa'o
106 times
inyetsa'o
a 106th
katle'o 7000 seven thousand kikatle'o
seven thousandth
tikatle'o
7000 times
ikatle'o
1/7000

Math Operations

  • ha'o ma ya'o ke pa'o a
3 and 5 O 8 COP
Three plus five is eight.
  • tsa'o ma ya'ok ke na'o a
6 and 5-NEG O 1 COP
Six and five-less is one.
  • ha'o ma tima'o ke ueta'o a
3 and multiple-4 O 12 COP
Three times four is twelve.
  • tama'o ma ha'o ke pa'o yeka
24 and 3 O 8 division
Twenty-four divided by three is eight.

Function words

Function words serve only grammatical functions. They have no meaning by themselves. Function words have to be used together with Content Words to form a meaningful sentence or phrase. Function words are a much smaller category in number but more frequently used. Kala function words are divided into a few subcategories; particles, conjunctions, and interjections.

Particles

ke

The direct object particle. It can often be omitted, especially in simple phrases, when the object is understood.
  • na mita anya
1sg dog see
I see the dog.
  • na tahe yempa ke mita anyaye
1sg be.under table O dog see-PST
I saw the dog under the table.

Conjunctions

There are three coordinating conjunctions in Kala and three correlative conjunctions:

coordinating

  • ma - and; also
mita ina ma moku
dog eat and sleep
The dog eats and sleeps.
  • ua - or
mita ina ua moku
dog eat or sleep
The dog eats or sleeps.
  • ehe (me) - but; yet
mita ina me mokunke
dog eat but sleep-NEG
The dog eats but does not sleep.

correlative

  • yema - both X and Y
mita ina yema empa
dog eat both.X.and.Y run
The dog both eats and runs.
The dog is eating and running simultaneously.
  • ue - either X or Y
mita ina ue empa
dog eat either.X.or.Y run
The dog is either eating or running.
  • uenke (uek) - neither X nor Y
mita ina uenke empa
dog eat neither.X.nor.Y run
The dog is neither eating nor running.


interjections

The most common interjections in Kala are as follows;

  • kya - imperative particle
  • kyo - imperative particle
  • kyo'a - imperative particle
  • nka - emphatic negative
  • ya - vocative particle
  • a - affirmative
  • aya - expresses strong emotions such as surprise ('ah', 'argh') and pain ('ow')

Interjections

Word formation

Compounding

Derivation

Verbalization

Nominalization

Basic syntax

The basic structure of a Kala sentence is:

AGENT--PATIENT--VERB (or SOV)

The agent is the person or thing doing the action described by the verb; The patient is the recipient of that action. The importance of word order can be seen by comparing the following sentences:

  • mita tlaka anya
dog man see
The dog sees the man.
  • tlaka mita anya
man dog see
The man sees the dog.

In both sentences, the words are identical, the only way to know who is seeing whom is by the order of the words in the sentence.

Simple sentences

Intransitive clauses

Intransitive clauses in Kala minimally consist of a subject followed by an intransitive verb, giving SV word order.

  • nta'i moku
baby sleep
The baby sleeps.
  • mita ina
dog eat
The dog eats.
  • sama nala
sun shine
The sun shines.
  • kamahi ke naha ya'e
town-DIM O river be.near
There is a village near the river.
  • ke apua muyapua
O song do-PFV
The song has been sung.

Transitive clauses

Clauses with transitive verbs follow a SOV pattern.

  • ona ke matla kuha
mother O stew cook
The mother is cooking stew.
  • tasako ke masala yake
hunt-AG O deer-INDEF chase
The hunters are chasing some deer.
  • kyali ke itohuatla peha
spear O tree-oak pierce
The spear pierces the oak tree.
  • tasako ke mitla hita ma ne masa mata
hunt-AG O arrow cast and DO deer kill
The hunter shoots an arrow and kills the deer.

Predication

Nominal predicates are formed with the copula a, using SOV word order. However, more common is the idiomatic omission of the copula and object marker.

  • tsola ke haya a
fox O animal COP
The fox is an animal. (grammatical)
  • tsola haya
fox animal
The fox is an animal. (idiomatic)
  • itlaka ke taya nayo a
PROX-man O husband 1sg.POSS COP
This man is my husband. (grammatical)
  • itlaka ke taya nayo
PROX-man O husband 1sg.POSS
This man is my husband. (idiomatic)

Oblique participants

Kala verb phrases have only a single object slot. As a result, the patient of a ditransitive clause needs to be introduced with the help of a preposition.

Dative and benefactive

Dative participants are marked with the preposition nya ‘for, by, via’.

  • ntahi ke nyotlomi nya kinti yeta
child O nut-PAUC BEN squirrel give
The child gives a few nuts to the squirrel.
  • ikema nya na tlahi
PROX-task BEN 1sg be.easy
This task is easy for me.

Benefactive participants are also marked with the preposition nya ‘for, by, via’.

  • mekatlo nya ntakum ke tsani yomu
holy-AG BEN sibling-PL O story recite
The shaman recites a story for the siblings.

Antibenefactive participants are marked like ordinary datives using nya:

  • tekim nya kamahi namyo tanyaye
enemy-PL BEN town-DIM 1pl.POSS destroy-PST
The enemies destroyed our village.

Instrumental

Comitative

Locative

Negation

Negation, both of noun phrases and of clauses, is made with the negating suffix -k (or -nke), which affixes to the negated element. Kala utilizes multiple negation, like tlok ak (AG-NEG COP-NEG) - (there isn't anyone / there is no-one).

  • intahi ke nok onyotlik
PROX-child O thing-NEG learn-FUT-NEG
This child will learn nothing.
  • mita inyak
dog hunger-NEG
The dog is not hungry.

The suffix -nke also marks the abessive, meaning without, or lacking.

  • ha ke’e hatsanke nya potsi hayo kayoye
3SG so luck-ABE for wallet 3SG.POSS lose-PST
He was unlucky enough to lose his wallet.

Interrogatives

There are two types of questions: Polar, those which may be answered "yes" or "no," and those which require explanations as answers.

Polar questions

Any statement can become a polar question by adding the interrogative particle ka at the end of the sentence.

  • mita ina
dog eat
The dog eats.
  • nta'i moku
baby sleep
The baby is sleeping. / The baby sleeps.
  • ta ke tlo'o anyaye
2SG O elephant see-PST
You saw the elephant.
  • tekatlo eta ke ya'a yetaye
heal-AG P.2SG O medicine give-PST
The doctor gave you the medicine.
  • mita ina ka
dog eat Q
Does the dog eat?
  • nta'i moku ka
baby sleep Q
Is the baby sleeping?
  • ta ke tlo'o anyaye ka
2SG O elephant see-PST Q
Did you see the elephant?
  • tekatlo eta ke ya'a yetaye ka
heal-AG P.2SG O medicine give-PST Q
Did the doctor give you the medicine?


Content questions

Questions that give a list of possible answers are formed like polar questions, with the conjunction ue ‘or’ introducing each alternative (which must appear in the form of a noun phrase).

  • ta ke nkapa ue maya inuue ka
2SG O beer or.EXCL water drink-VOL Q
Do you want to drink beer or water?
  • uala ta ke sinka mataye ue empa ma koma ka
truly 2sg O lion kill-PST or.EXCL flee CONJ hide Q
Did you really kill the lion, or did you run away and hide?

Open content questions are most easily formed with the correlatives, such as ko ‘person’, mo ‘place’, to ‘manner’, etc. These correlatives always appear clause-initially:

  • ko ta ka
person 2sg Q
Who are you?
  • itla ka
this Q
What is this?
  • to kihu ka
manner weather Q
What's the weather like?

The other type contains a question word and is followed by ka:

kanyo
Kala gloss English
object ke mita ina ka O dog eat Q What does the dog eat?
person ko ina ka person eat Q Who eats?
possession koyo mita ina ka person-POSS dog eat Q Whose dog eats?
manner to mita ina ka manner dog eat Q How does the dog eat?
place mo mita ina ka place dog eat Q Where does the dog eat?
reason nye mita ina ka reason dog eat Q Why does the dog eat?
time ama mita ina ka time dog eat Q When does the dog eat?
amount uku mita ina ka amount dog eat Q How much/many does the dog eat?
which ula mita ina ka any dog eat Q Which dog eats?

Passives

Reflexives and reciprocals

Complex sentences

Clause coordination

Coordination of noun phrases

Complement clauses

Relative clauses

Adverbial clauses

Serial verb construction

Sequential events

Lexicalized sequential serials

Grammatical use of serial verbs

Causatives

Comparison

Motion verbs

Direction and deixis

Manner of motion

Source and target

Posture and orientation

Aspect