Dreamlandic grammar: Difference between revisions

From FrathWiki
Jump to navigationJump to search
No edit summary
 
(35 intermediate revisions by 2 users not shown)
Line 19: Line 19:
The most prolific branch of the family was '''North Dreamlandic''', whose speakers split off from the others in the year 3370, and then followed the coast northward and reached 46°N within just a few hundred years.  Their northern fishing settlements were poorly defended and they came to live as minorities within other empires, but further south, the new cities of '''  Posensene    ''' and '''  Enoneta            ''' appeared and came to dominate the empire of Dreamland.   
The most prolific branch of the family was '''North Dreamlandic''', whose speakers split off from the others in the year 3370, and then followed the coast northward and reached 46°N within just a few hundred years.  Their northern fishing settlements were poorly defended and they came to live as minorities within other empires, but further south, the new cities of '''  Posensene    ''' and '''  Enoneta            ''' appeared and came to dominate the empire of Dreamland.   


==Minor North Dreamlandic languages==
==Sub-Dolphin languages==
These languages started with a syllable inventory of
Here are the languages for Lohi, Pupa, and Senampattore. Note that Pupa and Popa are named after the same geographic feature (probably a river), but are linguistically separate. The Valley of the Minds language may be the same as Senampattore, which would    place Senampattore with the Sepesian languages below. (In theory, it could also be an eastward migration from Pupa.)
  a    i    u    ya  wa  yi  wu
  pa  pi  pu  pya  pwa  pyi  pwu
  ma  mi  mu  mya  mwa  myi  mwu
  ta (ci  cutya      tyi
  na  ni  nu  nya      nyi
  sa  si  su  sya      syi
  la  li  lu  lya      lyi
  ra  ri  ru  rya      ryi
  ka  ki  ku        kwa      kwu
  ŋa  ŋi  ŋu        ŋwa      ŋwu
  ha  hi  hu        hwa      hwu
  fa  fi  fu
  mpa  mpi  mpu  mpya mpwa mpyi mpwu
nta (nsi  nsu  nsya      nsyi)
nka  nki  nku      nkwa      nkwu
ppa  ppi  ppu  ppya ppwa ppyi ppwu
kka  kki  kku  kkya kkwa kkyi kkwu


===Proto-Dreamlandic (1495) to Tasasina===
==Dolphin Rider language==
This language remains with three vowels longer than the others.  This is located immediately north of the Dolphin Rider birthplace and therefore may be intimately associated with the Dolphin Rider language, '''Neamaki'''.
===DPR Phonology===
:''For diachronics, see [[Lenian languages]].''


#The sequences ''kwa kwu ŋwa ŋwu'' shifted to '''pwa pwu mwa mwu'''.
#All long vowels became short.
#The sequences ''nsi nsya nsyi'' shifted to '''nyi nya nyi'''.
#The sequences ''si sya syi'' shifted to '''si sa si'''. 
#Then, the sequences ''ha hi hu hwa hwu''  became '''a yi bu bwa bwu'''.
#The sequences ''lya rya  '' merged as '''ya'''. Then ''lyi ryi  '' merged as '''yi'''. 
#The sequences ''nya nyi'' (including from earlier /nsya nsyi/) shifted to ''' na ni'''. 
#The sequences ''fa fi fu'' became '''ba bi bu'''. 
#Remaining ''h'' disappeared to '''Ø'''. 
#The sequences ''pw bw mw'' shifted to plain labials '''p b m'''.
#All remaining ''s'' shifted to '''h'''.
#The sequences ''pya pyi mya myi'' shifted to '''ta ti na ni'''.
#The sequences ''tya tyi kkya kkyi'' shifted to '''sa si ssa ssi'''.
#The sequences ''ci cu'' became '''si su'''.
#Remaining ''ns'' shifted to '''nz'''.


The syllable inventory at this point consisted of 61 glyphs:
Bilabials:       p  m          b 
Alveolars:          n  s  l  r 
Velars:          k 


  a   i   u      
Vowels are /a e i o u/ in both short and long forms.    
   wa  wi  wu
  pa  pi  pu     
  ba  bi  bu
  ma  mi  mu 
  ta  ti 
  sa  si  su
  na  ni  nu   
  la  li  lu   
  ra  ri  ru
  ya  yi  yu
  ka  ki  ku     
  ŋa  ŋi  ŋu     
  ha  hi  hu
mpa  mpi  mpu 
nta  nti     
nza      nzu
nka  nki  nku           
ppa  ppi  ppu 
tta  tti
ssa  ssi 
kka  kki  kku   


The velar stop /k/ is realized as [t] before any /e i/.  The phoneme inventory is the same as that of Baywatch, but the primary allophone of the dorsal stop is [k] in Dolphin Rider and [t] in Baywatch, leading to a different surface inventory.


The sequence /nzu/ was rare.  There was also a rare syllable-final /n/.  The consonant inventory can be analyzed as /p b m t s n l r k ŋ h/.
Syllable structure is strictly CV.


The sequences /ia ua/ had emerged from earlier /iha uha/, but there was no new bare /i/ or bare /u/.
===DPR verb markers===
====Tense markers====
*'''re''' past tense prefix


===Proto-Dreamlandic (1900) to Tata-A===
====Voice and aspect====
Makes long vowels from heavy syllables.
*'''mu''' reflexive verb prefix
===Proto-Dreamlandic (1900) to Tata-D===
:*Possibly retained as /bu/ in some fossils
Treats geminates as single consonants.
===Proto-Dreamlandic (1495) to Tata-B===
This may be spoken in Tata, but is too early to have been a [[Matrix]] language.


#The alveolar affricate ''č  '' shifted to '''š'''. 
===DPR particles===
#The labials ''pʲ mʲ fʲ f'' became '''h m h h'''.
*'''a''' "in, at" (locative)
#The labiovelars ''kʷ ŋʷ'' shifted to '''k ŋ'''.
*'''e''' "using" (instrumental)


==Sepesian languages==
These are descended from noun classifier prefixes.
These are the languages of '''Sepesi''' on the south coast of Dreamland.  The speakers here are in intimate contact with those of [[Baeba Swamp]] and parts of '''Kuhilani'''.


This branch might keep or even elaborate on the classifier prefix system, but probably still does not reach the levels of the Andanese or Subumpamese inventories.
:'''Nu erobi nē ''a mabo''.'''
::The lobster is ''in the ocean''.


===Proto-Dreamlandic (1495) to Suds (~3550) ===
Note that the copula is usually found before the object, creating an SVO sentence.  This sets Dolphin Rider apart from nearly all other languages on the planet.


===DPR definite articles===
These are also descended from noun classifier prefixes.  They mark both gender and noun case.  Since the plural of all animate genders had merged with the epicene by this time, and number had never been distinguished for inanimates, in a sense the articles also mark number.


#Labialization was defeated, leaving no effects on the vowels. 
Unlike those of Baywatch, the patient particles sometimes depend on the agent particles, and thus interactions work in both directions.
#The sequence ''ya'' shifted to '''ye'''.
#The sequence ''mp'' shifted to '''mm'''.
#Any singleton ''p '' shifted to ''' Ø'''.  (This includes /pu pi/ > /w y/.)  Any preceding vowels became long.  (But note that īy ūw were equivalent to ī ū.)
#The geminates ''pp čč kk'' shifted to '''p č k'''. 
#Before a vowel, the sequences ''ki ti pi hi fi'' merged as '''s'''.  Then ''tu'' shifted to '''su'''.  (This also implies /č/ > /š/ > /s/, as in Sessi.)
#Any remaining ''ki ti pi'' merged as the consonant geminator '''ʔ''', which shifted to /Ø/ in absolute final position; any remaining ''hi fi'' lengthened the preceding vowel (ː) and then disappeared to '''Ø'''. 
#Long vowels before a geminate consonant became short.
#The sequences ''ll rr'' shifted to '''l r'''.
#The sequences ''mi ni ŋi li ri'' shifted to '''n n n i i'''.  Sequences like /miwa/ > /nwa/ came to be spelled /nua/, etc.  This shift did not affect the long ī, which was equivalent to /iy/.
#:A yer-like process must appear here to prevent the syllables from bunching up on each other.  Since ī was rarely word-final, it is likely that word final -i will survive here and that therefore /mimi/ > /mmi/, etc. Word-initial geminates had already existed in the parent language. 
#The sequences ''lu ru'' shifted to '''u'''.
#The sequences ''nfa nfu nha nhu'' changed to '''mpa mpu na nu'''.
#The sequences ''fa fu'' shifted to '''ha hu'''.
#The sequences ''kī tī pī'' merged as '''si'''.  Then ''mī nī ŋī'' merged as '''ni'''.  Bare ''ī'' shifted to '''i'''. 
#The sequences ''ui ūi'' merged as '''i'''.
#:This means old kui, pui, kūi, etc all become consonant + /i/. 
#The sequences ''ūa ūu '' became '''ua uu '''. This also caused /uwa/ > /ua/.
#:Note that nearly all /ū/ had been before another vowel even in proto-Dreamlandic. 
#The sequences ''yi wu'' shifted to '''i u'''. (The surface realization of the two merged, and either can be thought of as the basic form.)


[[Baeba Swamp]] crushed PDP territory here and the population switched to speaking [[Khulls]].
====Intransitive====
;Feminine
*'''mi''' female experiencer


;Masculine
*'''si''' male experiencer


At this point the alphabet consisted of a very small number of glyphs:
;Maiden
*'''ni''' maiden experiencer


  a    i    u
;Neuter
  wa       
*'''nu''' neuter experiencer<ref>the /ne/ is from /nu-i/</ref>
  pa  pi  pu
  ma  mi  mu
  ta     
  na  ni  nu   ne
  sa  si  su    se
  la   
  ra   
  ya              ye
  ka  ki  ku
  ŋa  ŋi  ŋu
  ha  hi  hu


However, unlike neighboring languages, syllables could end in /n t/, which assimilated Palli-style to a following consonant.
;Plural
*Possibly '''pu'''


Common vowel nuclei were /a i u ā ai au āi āu ua uu ia ii iu/. There were also /e ei eu/, but these could only appear after /n s y/ and never in bare form. Also, there was no */ea/.
====DPR Agentives====
;Feminine
*'''mi''' female agent (if patient is male, maiden, or neuter)
:*''mi ... e'' female ---> male
:*''mi ... ni'' female ---> maiden
:*''mi ... Ø'' female ---> neuter
:*''mi ... pu'' female ---> plural
*'''e''' female agent (if patient is female)
:*''e ... bi'' female ---> female


The sequences /ia ua/ can be considered equivalent to the bare syllables /ya wa/ because /ya wa/ never occurred after consonants.
;Plural/epicene
*'''u''' plural agent
:*''u ... bi '' plural ---> female
:*''u ... e'' plural ---> male (PROBABLY)
:*''u ... ni'' plural ---> maiden
:*''u ... Ø'' plural ---> neuter
:*''u ... pu'' plural ---> plural


The alveolars /t l r/ only occurred before /a/, but this /a/ could precede /i/ or /u/, and also could be long, so a five-way contrast of /a ai au āi āu/ was maintained.
;Maiden
*'''ni''' maiden agent


====Distribution====
;Masculine
This was the language of the [[ppot#PDP|Soap]] party, also known as Suds. They were pacifists who lost a war against the Crystals and therefore their language became a substratum of the Crystal language. The Crystals opposed slavery, and therefore the Suds were allowed to move out, but many stayed and resigned themselves to living as an underclass in Baeba Swamp.  This was because, as pacifists, they opposed both sides of the war that had cost them their homeland, and thus considered themselves equally at home among their enemies as among their former allies.
*Probably '''si'''


Because the language lost most /p/ early on and also lost some /m/, it does not have the same character as the northern languages, DPR and BaywatchBut the restricted distribution of the coronals /t l r/ made the language stand out from all of its neighbors even so, and when the Crystals took control of the Soapies' homeland, the Soap language rapidly evolved a more phonologically stable inventory.
;Neuter
*'''no''' neuter agent (if patient is female)
:*''no ... Ø'' neuter ---> female
*'''me'''  neuter agent (if patient is  male) 
:*''me ... Ø''  neuter ---> male
*'''nu''' neuter agent (if patient is neuter)
:*''nu ... Ø''  neuter ---> neuter
:*'''ne''' variant neuter agent (if patient is  male or neuter)


===Proto-Dreamlandic (1495) to Sesesi (~2700) ===
;Miscellaneous
The end date might be earlier, because this originally contained additional sound changes under influence from [[Baeba Swamp]].
*'''su''' various inanimate objects when promoted to agents with human patients


#Labialization was defeated, leaving no effects on the vowels. 
====Patientive====
#The sequence ''ya'' shifted to '''ye'''.
*'''bi''' female patient
#Before a vowel, the voiceless sequences ''pu p pi'' shifted to '''w Ø y'''.  (This includes mp > mm > m).  Any preceding vowels became long.  (But note that īy ūw were equivalent to ī ū.)
:*'''e bi''' variant female patient (if agent is male)
#The geminates ''pp čč kk'' shifted to '''p č k'''.
*'''no''' female patient (if agent is neuter and sentence is OSV or OVS)
#The sequences ''hi fi ti tu'' shifted to '''si si si su'''.  Then all ''č'' became '''š'''. 
#Any remaining ''t'' shifted to '''r'''.
#The  fricatives ''f š'' shifted to '''h  s'''.
#Before a vowel, the sequences  ''pi  mi  ni li ri '' (including from earlier /mbi/) shifted to '''s n n l r  '''.   
#:added /ni/ because there was no /ñ/ in the target phonology.
#The prenasalized sequences ''  nr ns ŋk'' shifted to '''  n s k'''. Adds vowel length?
#The sequences ''aw ew iw uw'' shifted to '''ō ō ū ū'''. Thus a new vowel, /ō/, was created; however there was no short counterpart.  This shift also included tautosyllabic /au/, /eu/, etc.
#The sequences ''ay ey iy uy'' shifted to '''ē ē ī ī'''.
#The double vowel sequence ''aa'' shifted to '''ā'''.
#Long vowels followed by another vowel became short. Thus the new sequences like /ōa/ became /oa/, and the reflexes of earlier /ipia/ merged with /ipa/, and so on.


Thus the alphabet consisted of 39 syllables:


  a    i    u   
*'''ni''' maiden patient
  wa        wu 
  pa  pi  pu       
  ma  mi  mu     
  na  ni   nu  ne
  sa  si  su  se
  la  li  lu  le 
  ra  ri  ru  re 
  ya  yi        ye
  ka  ki  ku       
  ŋa  ŋi  ŋu   
  ha  hi  hu 




The short vowels were /a e i u/ and the long vowels were /ā ē ī ō ū/.
*'''e''' male patient
*'''se''' male patient (if agent is male)
*'''me''' male patient (if agent is neuter and sentence is OSV or OVS)


This branch divides less rapidly than the others.  There will be /s h/ > /0/, and may be /uk ku/ > /up pu/ and /ik ki/ > /it ti/ (through k > ć > ṭ), but only if these consonants can be separated from this context by later vowel shifts.


===Sesesi to Valley of the Minds===
*'''pu''' plural patient
This language is spoken in the northern part of [[Dreamland]], by people who made it to the north before the speakers of North Dreamlandic did. It may have survived in a valley with one major city.  Also, these sound changes might apply to other languages of the family as well, since VoM did not break off from near the root.


#When not before a vowel, the sequences ''se ne le re'' shifted to the consonants '''s n l r'''. Thus closed syllables were created.
#The long vowels ''ē ō'' decomposed to '''ay aw'''.


==Western languages==
*'''Ø''' neuter patient
The western branch broke off around 1300 AD, and then lost contact with the others.
*'''nu''' neuter patient (if agent is neuter and sentence is OSV or OVS)
====Changes unique to Western Fojy====
#The voiceless non-sibilant fricatives ''   f  fʲ  h hʷ'' shifted to '''  Ø Ø Ø w'''
#The palatalized labials ''pʲ  mʲ'' shifted to '''p  m'''.
#The bare vowel ''u'' shifted to '''ʉ'''. Then ''wa'' shifted to '''wo'''. Neither of these shifts were phonemic.


The language at this stage had exactly 100 syllables, if the clusters '''pp kk mp nt nk''' are counted as single onsets. This makes WF one of two related languages to evolve a 100-character syllabary early in their history, the other being the very early stages of the [[Gold language]].
In the formal register, masculine nouns are prohibited from being the agents of verbs with female patients, and thus must take an additional /e/ before the patient's article. This can be analyzed as either creating a null feminine agent or masculinizing the patient, since the female agent prefix and the male patient prefix in this narrow context are both /e/.


However, the 100 syllable analysis ignores the rare independent final nasal /n/. There were many vowel sequences, such as /ie/, as well as inherited long vowels which were now written as doubles.
This trait of granting female agents a slightly higher status on the animacy hierarchy is common to many [[languages of Teppala]]. But note that unlike, for example, the [[Proto-Moonshine culture|Moonshines]], the [[ppot#DPR|Dolphin Riders]] were not particularly feministic or pacifistic in cultural traits.


The syllabary consisted of signs for
===Gender and syntax===
Syntactically inanimate objects are mostly found in the neuter gender, but mass nouns and some others are in the plural gender, which is also known as epicene.


  a   i    ʉ    ya  wo  yi  wu  ye
====Feminine gender====
  pa  pi  pʉ  __  pwo  __  pwu  pe
Contains words for women of marriageable age, and a few words for girls.
  ma  mi  mʉ        mwo      mwu  me
====Maiden gender====
  ta  (ci  cʉ)  tya      tyi      tye
Contains words for girls and a few words for women.
  na  ni  nʉ  nya      nyi      nye
====Masculine gender====
  sa  si  sʉ  sya      syi      sye
Contains words for men and boys, and masculine property.
  la  li  lʉ  lya      lyi      lye
====Neuter gender====
  ra  ri  rʉ  rya      ryi      rye
This is DPR's counterpart of DRM's child gender. It contains some words for small children, but most neuter words are for animals and objects. DPR places more higher animals, such as birds, in the neuter gender than does DRM.
  ka  ki  kʉ  kya kwo kyi kwu kye
 
  ŋa  ŋi  ŋʉ  ŋya ŋwo  ŋyi ŋwu ŋye
Unlike the other genders, the agent prefix for the neuter gender is dependent on the gender of the patient. Thus all sentences with neuter agents can be considered passive.
ppa  ppi  ppʉ      ppwo      ppwu  ppe
 
kka  kki  kkʉ  kkya kkwo kkyi kkwu kkye
Sentences with neuter agents are often OSV or OVS. When this happens, the neuter agent prefix remains at the beginning of the clause, since it can also serve to mark the patient. Thus just as one says
mpa  mpi  mpʉ      mpwo      mpwu  mpe
 
nta (nci  ncʉ) ntya      ntyi      ntye
:''' ''Mi bupue'' bolimo rerasi.'''
ŋka  ŋki  ŋkʉ  ŋkya ŋkwo ŋkyi ŋkwu ŋkye
::''The girl'' caught an octopus.
 
One would also say


In the year 3958, one of the languages of Western Fojy (here "WF", or Wildfire) took over all of Dreamland, only to be swept out by the Dolphin Rider language in 4108.
:'''  No bupue'' bolimo'' rerasi.'''
::The girl was caught ''by an octopus.''


====Proto-Western Fojy (1900) to Wildfire I (3958)====
Or
This language loses its /ʉ/, creating an unbalanced setup where most of the consonants cannot occur before a back vowel. 


#The weak vowel ''ʉ'' disappeared to '''Ø''', creating closed syllables.   
: ''' ''No bolimo '' bupue rerasi. '''
#Triple consonant sequences became double.
::''The octopus'' caught a girl.
 
If both the patient and the agent are neuter, then the sentence is ambiguous, and context is necessary.  The normal word orders are SOV and SVO, but in a connected narrative, a subject may be the patient.  Thus one can say
 
:''' Nu bopusepu ''siselepi'' repobo.'''
::The eagle bit ''a rabbit''.
 
But in a story about a rabbit, the opposite wording could be used:
 
:''' ''Nu siselepi'' bopusepu repobo.'''
::''The rabbit'' was bitten by an eagle.
 
===Combining forms===
Possibly fuse definite articles and other morphemes to their roots, such as ''nu erobi'' > '''narobi'''. This would require analogy with the way words had been spelled 4,000 years earlier, but the analogy could have been made in proto-Dreamlandic and then gradually lost from some branches and not othersIt could also be argued that this would be a retention of the classifier system even after the alliterative concord was lost.
 
NOTE: it isnt clear how /ue/  could produce /a/; this may have been a mistake.  neither would /eu/ work.


==Baywatch language==
==Baywatch language==
Line 263: Line 201:


*'''nē''' verb copula
*'''nē''' verb copula
====Conjunctions and other particles====
*'''-o pe-''' "to transform [X] into [Y]", where the X precedes the conjunction and the Y follows.  The prefix '''pe-''' absorbs the instrumental prefix /e-/; this may or may not also fall away when that instrumental prefix itself mutates.


===DRM verb prefixes===
===DRM verb prefixes===
Line 432: Line 373:
Baywatch requires the use of many short words that would in other languages be either null morphemes or fusional inflections. Notably, the use of tools is denoted by a construction that behaves like a serial verb. For example, one says
Baywatch requires the use of many short words that would in other languages be either null morphemes or fusional inflections. Notably, the use of tools is denoted by a construction that behaves like a serial verb. For example, one says


: '''  Si pe ''ppu nō pi''  pu mpi'''.
: '''  Si pe ''ppu nōpi''  pu mpi'''.
::The man is ''slicing'' the apple (with a knife).
::The man is ''slicing'' the apple (with a knife).


Rather than having a dedicated word that means slice, Baywatch instead specifies that the man is holding (''ppu'') a knife (''nō'') and that the knife goes across  (''pi'') something. Thus there is no word for slicing that does not contain a morpheme indicating a cutting implement. Even so, the phrase can be Romanized as a single word /nōpi/, as though it were a single morpheme after all; the native syllabary is ambiguous as to word spacing.
Rather than having a dedicated word that means slice, Baywatch instead specifies that the man is holding (''ppu'') a knife (''nō'') and that the knife goes across  (''pi'') something. Thus there is no word for slicing that does not contain a morpheme indicating a cutting implement. Even this word can be Romanized as /nō pi/; the native syllabary is ambiguous as to word spacing.


;SOV analysis
;SOV analysis
Line 453: Line 394:
::The girl ''bit'' the strawberries.
::The girl ''bit'' the strawberries.


:''' Ne ī    ''se pompo''  mpu.'''
:''' Ne ī    ''sōmpupi''  mpu.'''
::The bird ''bit'' the girl.
::The bird ''bit'' the girl.


:'''Ne pase ''se lime''. '''
:'''Ne pase ''se lime'' ntu'''.
::The dog ''bit me''.
::The dog ''bit  '' the boy.
 
====Syntactic evolution====
In some cases, the word for an abstract concept has lost its independent use.  This most often happens when a compound of two monosyllables fuses in one or both directions with surrounding morphemes. For example, the basic root word for law is '''posu''', originally "bundle of arrows", but the most common use of this word in Baywatch is '''poso''',<ref>This /po/ is an independent morpheme, but the shift of /pu a/ > /po/ is so common that speakers construed it as plural early on.</ref> where a previously existing suffix has fused with the root. This once meant "to show or spread a cloud of arrows", referring to the threat of enforcing the law by showing one's weapons, but most students learn /poso/ before they ever hear of /posu/, and picture in their minds anything from a weapon to a book as the root has no use outside this phrase.
 
==Dolphin Rider language==
===DPR Phonology===
:''For diachronics, see [[Lenian languages]].''


:'''Ne pompi ''se tabu''. '''
::The beetle ''bit me''.


Bilabials:      p  m          b 
These four words exist because the four animals involved have greatly different mouth anatomy.    
Alveolars:          n  s  l  r 
Velars:          k    


Vowels are /a e i o u/ in both short and long forms.  Syllable structure is CVC, but words can begin with geminates and thus the structure could be analyzed as CCVC.  All clusters are homorganic, and the only consonant that can occur in absolute final position is /n/.
====Syntactic evolution====
 
In some cases, the word for an abstract concept has lost its independent use.  This most often happens when a compound of two monosyllables fuses in one or both directions with surrounding morphemes. For example, the basic root word for law is '''posu''',<ref>This /po/ is an independent morpheme, but the shift of /pu a/ > /po/ is so common that speakers construed it as plural early on.</reforiginally "bundle of arrows", but the most common use of this word in Baywatch is '''poso''',where a previously existing suffix has fused with the root. This once meant "to show or spread a bundle of arrows", referring to the threat of enforcing the law by showing one's weapons, but most students learn /poso/ before they ever hear of /posu/, and picture in their minds anything from a weapon to a book as the root has no use outside this phrase.
The velar stop /k/ is realized as [t] before any /e i/.  The phoneme inventory is the same as that of Baywatch, but the primary allophone of the dorsal stop is [k] in Dolphin Rider and [t] in Baywatch, leading to a different surface inventory.
 
Syllable structure is strictly CV.
 
===DPR verb markers===
====Tense markers====
*'''re''' past tense prefix
 
====Voice and aspect====
*'''mu''' reflexive verb prefix
:*Possibly retained as /bu/ in some fossils
 
===DPR particles===
*'''a''' "in, at" (locative)
*'''e''' "using" (instrumental)
 
These are descended from noun classifier prefixes.
 
:'''Nu erobi nē ''a mabo''.'''
::The lobster is ''in the ocean''.
 
Note that the copula is usually found before the object, creating an SVO sentence.  This sets Dolphin Rider apart from nearly all other languages on the planet.
 
===DPR definite articles===
These are also descended from noun classifier prefixes.  They mark both gender and noun case.  Since the plural of all animate genders had merged with the epicene by this time, and number had never been distinguished for inanimates, in a sense the articles also mark number.
 
Unlike those of Baywatch, the patient particles sometimes depend on the agent particles, and thus interactions work in both directions.
 
====Intransitive====
;Feminine
*'''mi''' female experiencer
 
;Masculine
*'''si''' male experiencer
 
;Maiden
*'''ni''' maiden experiencer
 
;Neuter
*'''nu''' neuter experiencer<ref>the /ne/ is from /nu-i/</ref>
 
;Plural
*Possibly '''pu'''
 
====DPR Agentives====
;Feminine
*'''mi''' female agent (if patient is male, maiden, or neuter)
:*''mi ... e'' female ---> male
:*''mi ... ni'' female ---> maiden
:*''mi ... Ø'' female ---> neuter
:*''mi ... pu'' female ---> plural
*'''e''' female agent (if patient is female)
:*''e ... bi'' female ---> female
 
;Plural/epicene
*'''u''' plural agent
:*''u ... bi '' plural ---> female
:*''u ... e'' plural ---> male (PROBABLY)
:*''u ... ni'' plural ---> maiden
:*''u ... Ø'' plural ---> neuter
:*''u ... pu'' plural ---> plural
 
;Maiden
*'''ni''' maiden agent
 
;Masculine
*Probably '''si'''
 
;Neuter
*'''no''' neuter agent (if patient is female)
:*''no ... Ø'' neuter ---> female
*'''me'''  neuter agent (if patient is male) 
:*''me ... Ø''  neuter ---> male
*'''nu'''  neuter agent (if patient is neuter)
:*''nu ... Ø''  neuter ---> neuter
:*'''ne''' variant neuter agent (if patient is male or neuter)
 
====Patientive====
*'''bi''' female patient
:*'''e bi''' variant female patient (if agent is male)
*'''no''' female patient (if agent is neuter and sentence is OSV or OVS)
 
 
*'''ni''' maiden patient
 
 
*'''e''' male patient
*'''se''' male patient (if agent is male)
*'''me''' male patient (if agent is neuter and sentence is OSV or OVS)
 
 
*'''pu''' plural patient
 
 
*'''Ø''' neuter patient
*'''nu''' neuter patient (if agent is neuter and sentence is OSV or OVS)
 
In the formal register, masculine nouns are prohibited from being the agents of verbs with female patients, and thus must take an additional /e/ before the patient's article. This can be analyzed as either creating a null feminine agent or masculinizing the patient, since the female agent prefix and the male patient prefix in this narrow context are both /e/.
 
===Gender and syntax===
Syntactically inanimate objects are mostly found in the child gender, but mass nouns and some others are in the plural gender, which is also known as epicene.
 
====Feminine gender====
Contains words for women of marriageable age, and a few words for girls.
====Maiden gender====
Contains words for girls and a few words for women.
====Masculine gender====
Contains words for men and boys, and masculine property.
====Neuter gender====
This is DPR's counterpart of DRM's child gender.  It contains some words for small children, but most neuter words are for  animals and  objects.  DPR places more higher animals, such as birds, in the neuter gender than does DRM.
 
Unlike the other genders, the agent prefix for the neuter gender is dependent on the gender of the patient.  Thus all sentences with neuter agents can be considered passive.
 
Sentences with neuter agents are often OSV or OVS.  When this happens, the neuter agent prefix remains at the beginning of the clause, since it can also serve to mark the patient.  Thus just as one says
 
:''' ''Mi bupue'' bolimo rerasi.'''
::''The girl'' caught an octopus.
 
One would also say
 
:'''  No bupue'' bolimo'' rerasi.'''
::The girl was caught ''by an octopus.''
 
Or
 
: ''' ''No bolimo '' bupue rerasi. '''
::''The octopus'' caught a girl.
 
If both the patient and the agent are neuter, then the sentence is ambiguous, and context is necessary.  The normal word orders are SOV and SVO, but in a connected narrative, a subject may be the patient.  Thus one can say
 
:''' Nu bopusepu ''siselepi'' repobo.'''
::The eagle bit ''a rabbit''.
 
But in a story about a rabbit, the opposite wording could be used:
 
:''' ''Nu siselepi'' bopusepu repobo.'''
::''The rabbit'' was bitten by an eagle.
 
===Combining forms===
Possibly fuse definite articles and other morphemes to their roots, such as ''nu erobi'' > '''narobi'''. This would require analogy with the way words had been spelled 4,000 years earlier, but the analogy could have been made in proto-Dreamlandic and then gradually lost from some branches and not others.


==Notes==
==Notes==
[[Category:Languages of Teppala]]
[[Category:Languages of Teppala]]
[[Category:Language families of Teppala|P9]]

Latest revision as of 16:55, 26 December 2021

The empire of Dreamland spoke a large variety of languages due to the many small, isolated settlements along the coast whose people had little contact with other Dreamers. However, all of these languages were closely related and shared many characteristics. Most languages belonged to the Dreamlandic family, but there were some longstanding settlements in Dreamland populated by speakers of other language families such as Hipatal.


Background

The Dreamlandic languages are closely related to Gold and Andanese, but starkly contrast with the other two branches in many ways. Dreamlandic languages tend to use many short words where the other branches may use a single long word. This applies both to semantic derivation and inflection. Phonologically, Dreamlandic languages are simpler than most languages in the other two branches, but one Andanic language is simpler still.

Settlement history

Lenian people began settling what is now Dreamland from the west around the year 1320 AD. They founded a major colony at about 26°N, 25°W. From here, they quickly divided into two groups of people: those who lived along the coast and survived by fishing the sea, and those who moved inland and survived by hunting and gathering, as well as fishing in lakes and rivers. There was no middle ground and both groups were migratory. In this way, the early Dreamer state strongly resembled Paba.

Unlike Paba, however, the hunting tribes and the fishing tribes were both of the same ethnicity: the blonde blue-eyed Lenian people. Therefore, members of one group could quickly assimilate into the other group, and the tribes maintained friendly relations even as they became more physically isolated.

For the most part, the hunting tribes found success in the hot, humid climates of the southern side of the peninsula, and formed the Sepesi branch of the family as they moved eastward along the coast towards the ancient city of Baeba Swamp. Soon, they signed a pact with Baeba Swamp and promised to pursue pacifism.

The fishing tribes lived in smaller, more concentrated colonies, and spent less time outside their territory. Therefore they divided into many small branches early on, with each one later becoming just a single state in the empire of Dreamland. However, the people furthest to the west did not join Dreamland and in later times came to consider themselves superior to the Dreamers.

Major languages

See Lenian languages for sound changes.

The most prolific branch of the family was North Dreamlandic, whose speakers split off from the others in the year 3370, and then followed the coast northward and reached 46°N within just a few hundred years. Their northern fishing settlements were poorly defended and they came to live as minorities within other empires, but further south, the new cities of Posensene and Enoneta appeared and came to dominate the empire of Dreamland.

Sub-Dolphin languages

Here are the languages for Lohi, Pupa, and Senampattore. Note that Pupa and Popa are named after the same geographic feature (probably a river), but are linguistically separate. The Valley of the Minds language may be the same as Senampattore, which would place Senampattore with the Sepesian languages below. (In theory, it could also be an eastward migration from Pupa.)

Dolphin Rider language

DPR Phonology

For diachronics, see Lenian languages.


Bilabials:       p   m           b   
Alveolars:           n   s   l   r  
Velars:          k   

Vowels are /a e i o u/ in both short and long forms.

The velar stop /k/ is realized as [t] before any /e i/. The phoneme inventory is the same as that of Baywatch, but the primary allophone of the dorsal stop is [k] in Dolphin Rider and [t] in Baywatch, leading to a different surface inventory.

Syllable structure is strictly CV.

DPR verb markers

Tense markers

  • re past tense prefix

Voice and aspect

  • mu reflexive verb prefix
  • Possibly retained as /bu/ in some fossils

DPR particles

  • a "in, at" (locative)
  • e "using" (instrumental)

These are descended from noun classifier prefixes.

Nu erobi nē a mabo.
The lobster is in the ocean.

Note that the copula is usually found before the object, creating an SVO sentence. This sets Dolphin Rider apart from nearly all other languages on the planet.

DPR definite articles

These are also descended from noun classifier prefixes. They mark both gender and noun case. Since the plural of all animate genders had merged with the epicene by this time, and number had never been distinguished for inanimates, in a sense the articles also mark number.

Unlike those of Baywatch, the patient particles sometimes depend on the agent particles, and thus interactions work in both directions.

Intransitive

Feminine
  • mi female experiencer
Masculine
  • si male experiencer
Maiden
  • ni maiden experiencer
Neuter
  • nu neuter experiencer[1]
Plural
  • Possibly pu

DPR Agentives

Feminine
  • mi female agent (if patient is male, maiden, or neuter)
  • mi ... e female ---> male
  • mi ... ni female ---> maiden
  • mi ... Ø female ---> neuter
  • mi ... pu female ---> plural
  • e female agent (if patient is female)
  • e ... bi female ---> female
Plural/epicene
  • u plural agent
  • u ... bi plural ---> female
  • u ... e plural ---> male (PROBABLY)
  • u ... ni plural ---> maiden
  • u ... Ø plural ---> neuter
  • u ... pu plural ---> plural
Maiden
  • ni maiden agent
Masculine
  • Probably si
Neuter
  • no neuter agent (if patient is female)
  • no ... Ø neuter ---> female
  • me neuter agent (if patient is male)
  • me ... Ø neuter ---> male
  • nu neuter agent (if patient is neuter)
  • nu ... Ø neuter ---> neuter
  • ne variant neuter agent (if patient is male or neuter)
Miscellaneous
  • su various inanimate objects when promoted to agents with human patients

Patientive

  • bi female patient
  • e bi variant female patient (if agent is male)
  • no female patient (if agent is neuter and sentence is OSV or OVS)


  • ni maiden patient


  • e male patient
  • se male patient (if agent is male)
  • me male patient (if agent is neuter and sentence is OSV or OVS)


  • pu plural patient


  • Ø neuter patient
  • nu neuter patient (if agent is neuter and sentence is OSV or OVS)

In the formal register, masculine nouns are prohibited from being the agents of verbs with female patients, and thus must take an additional /e/ before the patient's article. This can be analyzed as either creating a null feminine agent or masculinizing the patient, since the female agent prefix and the male patient prefix in this narrow context are both /e/.

This trait of granting female agents a slightly higher status on the animacy hierarchy is common to many languages of Teppala. But note that unlike, for example, the Moonshines, the Dolphin Riders were not particularly feministic or pacifistic in cultural traits.

Gender and syntax

Syntactically inanimate objects are mostly found in the neuter gender, but mass nouns and some others are in the plural gender, which is also known as epicene.

Feminine gender

Contains words for women of marriageable age, and a few words for girls.

Maiden gender

Contains words for girls and a few words for women.

Masculine gender

Contains words for men and boys, and masculine property.

Neuter gender

This is DPR's counterpart of DRM's child gender. It contains some words for small children, but most neuter words are for animals and objects. DPR places more higher animals, such as birds, in the neuter gender than does DRM.

Unlike the other genders, the agent prefix for the neuter gender is dependent on the gender of the patient. Thus all sentences with neuter agents can be considered passive.

Sentences with neuter agents are often OSV or OVS. When this happens, the neuter agent prefix remains at the beginning of the clause, since it can also serve to mark the patient. Thus just as one says

Mi bupue bolimo rerasi.
The girl caught an octopus.

One would also say

No bupue bolimo rerasi.
The girl was caught by an octopus.

Or

No bolimo bupue rerasi.
The octopus caught a girl.

If both the patient and the agent are neuter, then the sentence is ambiguous, and context is necessary. The normal word orders are SOV and SVO, but in a connected narrative, a subject may be the patient. Thus one can say

Nu bopusepu siselepi repobo.
The eagle bit a rabbit.

But in a story about a rabbit, the opposite wording could be used:

Nu siselepi bopusepu repobo.
The rabbit was bitten by an eagle.

Combining forms

Possibly fuse definite articles and other morphemes to their roots, such as nu erobi > narobi. This would require analogy with the way words had been spelled 4,000 years earlier, but the analogy could have been made in proto-Dreamlandic and then gradually lost from some branches and not others. It could also be argued that this would be a retention of the classifier system even after the alliterative concord was lost.

NOTE: it isnt clear how /ue/ could produce /a/; this may have been a mistake. neither would /eu/ work.

Baywatch language

Phonology

For diachronics, see Lenian languages.
Bilabials:       p   m           b   
Alveolars:       t   n   s   l   r     

Vowels are /a e i o u/ in both short and long forms. Syllable structure is CVC, but words can begin with geminates and thus the structure could be analyzed as CCVC. All clusters are homorganic, and the only consonant that can occur in absolute final position is /n/.

There are no dorsal consonants; instead, the coronals /t n/ are realized as [k ŋ] before any /u/, and for some speakers, also before any /o/.

DRM particles

  • a "in, at" (locative)
  • e "using" (instrumental). Mutates to o if the following noun begins with /o/ or /u/

The particles a and e can be thought of as verbs, in the sense that they must immediately precede a noun, and take verb-like inflections. But the copula must come after this if there is no main verb.

See below at #ppu for an explanation of how the particles could be mingled with the verbs and all distinctions lost.

  • verb copula

Conjunctions and other particles

  • -o pe- "to transform [X] into [Y]", where the X precedes the conjunction and the Y follows. The prefix pe- absorbs the instrumental prefix /e-/; this may or may not also fall away when that instrumental prefix itself mutates.

DRM verb prefixes

Because SOV and SVO word orders were both legal, the verb prefix often alliterated with the object prefix, and this led to analogical removal of whichever occurred last in a given sentence.

This list is arranged in the order the morphemes appear in the sentence.

DRM tense markers

  • se past tense marker. Merges with following vowels, such that se-o > , etc. Because many verb stems begin with vowels, this mutation is common. Because the tense marker comes before the case particles as well, these case particles merge.

The list of mergers is:

  1. e-a > ē, as in sē bama "went inside"
  2. e-e > ē, as in sē tare "planned"
  3. e-i > ē, as in sēsēmpo "made peace"
  4. e-o > ō, as in sōpu "sat on"
  5. e-u > ō, as in sōbi "felt"
  6. e-ā > ē, as in (no examples)
  7. e-ē > ē, as in (no examples)

This morpheme originally began with /ns/, but the /n/ was lost at a stage when the child agentive prefix n- still echoed before the verb.

Person markers

  • ne 1st or 3rd person human patients
  • mpu reflexive verb prefix
  • isolated forms with /mu/ may also exist

Number markers

  • bere plural patient marker
  • mesi plural for epicene gender
  • nsisse plural for neuter gender

It is not clear where these go or how they are used; perhaps they appear between the tense prefix and the verb stem.


DRM definite articles

These are also descended from noun classifier prefixes. They mark both gender and noun case. Since the plural of all animate genders had merged with the epicene by this time, and number had never been distinguished for inanimates, in a sense the articles also mark number.

Intransitive

  • mi female experiencer
  • si male experiencer
  • ni maiden experiencer
  • n child experiencer

These are also used for possession of objects.

Agentive

  • mi female agent (if patient is male, maiden, or child)
  • si male agent (if patient is female, maiden, child, or plural)
  • ni maiden agent
  • n child agent

The forms above are the same as the intransitives. However, there are more:

  • i female agent (if patient is female)
  • pi female agent (if patient is plural)
  • se male agent (if patient is male). Mutates to so if the following noun begins with /o/ or /u/
  • ne variant child agent (if patient is female, male, or child). Mutates to no if the following noun begins with /o/ or /u/
  • This is probably also the form used if the patient is 1st person.
  • me variant child agent (if patient is male). Mutates to mo if the following noun begins with /o/ or /u/


  • bu plural agent. Shifts to pu if the following noun begins with /p/ or /t/

Patientive

  • ni maiden patient
  • n child patient

The forms above are the same as the intransitives. However, there are more:

  • ne 1st person patient
  • mpi female patient
  • e male patient. Mutates to o if the following noun begins with /o/ or /u/
    In an early draft, this became Ø if the instrumental particle /e/ appeared anywhere earlier in the sentence.

  • ppu plural patient. Shifts to pu if the following noun contains another historical consonant cluster (not always transparent due to subsequent shifts)
  • This word united with one of the verbs for grasping, /pu/, and thus came to be generalized as an instrumental prefix for handheld objects. This is why tools are mostly in the epicene ("plural") gender instead of the inherited neuter. Note that the /pu/ that was analyzed was a verb, not a noun, even though /pu/ as a noun can also mean the palm of the hand. Note also that this word was once /ppu/, so even the alternation would be preserved.
  • Furthermore, this could open the way for many other monosyllables to also appear in this position, making the inherited case markers /a/ and /e/ no different than any of many verbs such as /ppu/ "grasp", /bi/ "feel", /o/ "expose", and so on. But note that in all cases, this verb is semantically tied to the word immediately afterwards, so e.g. one would not use /pi/ "cut" before the word for knife, because humans dont cut knives, humans use knives to cut other things.


Those agentive prefixes that also uniquely specify the gender of the patient require the speaker to omit the patientive prefixes. Thus one can say

Me mposu pempure nsebise.
The duck followed the soldier.

For emphasis, a prefix can be used, but it must be a repetition of the agent's prefix rather than a retention of the patient's, and the emphasis is not on the patient but on the agent's control over the patient. Thus the above sentence can be reworded as

Me mposu me pempure nsebise.
The duck followed the soldier.

The repetition of the prefix implies that the soldier may be an unwilling party whereas in the former sentence there is no such implication. The otherwise expected formula, Me mposu *e pempure nsebise, is not used.

Note that the prefixes for children and maidens are the same in all cases.

Note: try to get out of the habit of using SOV.

Use of noun prefixes

The noun prefix is mandatory in sentence-initial position, and therefore there is no distinction between definite and indefinite subjects. This is common in related languages, even those with very different grammars. Thus one must say

Si peno pu pempā nsōnse.
The (male) goat ate the grass.
A (male) goat ate the grass.

Gender and syntax

Gender marking on humans is strictly syntactic, and becomes more grammatical (non-literal) as the objects described become less animate. The maiden and child genders are retained from the proto-language and remain fully functional, but in some areas of the grammar they merge with each other.

Feminine gender

The feminine gender includes words for women and women's property. It also includes words for girls, but younger girls more often take words belonging to the maiden gender. Higher animals are also described according to literal gender. There are also many inanimate objects in the feminine gender, all deriving from various processes of analogy.

Maiden gender

The maiden gender includes words for girls of pre-marriageable age. It also includes many words for immature female animals, and for females of some small animals regardless of maturity. There are many inanimate objects as well.

Child gender

Sometimes called neuter, the child gender includes words for very young children not capable of living independently. In the high register of the language, some verbs are ungrammatical if the agent is a child, but this rule is ignored in colloquial speech. The child gender also includes many words for animals and inanimate objects, and can be called the neuter gender in this context.

Because the prefix for the child gender in most environments is a simple n-, it cannot occur before a word beginning in a cluster or one of l- r-. Such words thus must use one of the other animate genders even when describing small children and other neuter objects. For example, a student might expect the words liri "goldfish" and mpoli "octopus" to be neuter like other sea life, but in fact they are found in the epicene gender.

This phonological restriction also prohibits the neuter possession marker from occurring before the names of objects beginning with these consonants. Thus, for example, it is impossible to say *n mpempe "the baby's rock", because this would be pronounced with /mmp/, which is illegal. The solution here is to use one of the other animate genders, corresponding to the child's closest semantic gender. This is maidens for girls and men for boys. Because gender is inherited from the possessor, the object then becomes this gender itself even though its possessor is a baby.

Note that classifier prefixes are repeated before the objects, so this can cause the initial word to also change. This causes babies to gain semantic gender and requires the speaker to know the syntactic gender of the baby being referenced. Thus, though Baywatchers are accustomed to saying things such as

N pose n pepepo.
The baby's diaper.

To reference "the baby's clothes", one would need to pick a gender and either say

Si pose si ppe.
The boy baby's clothes.

Or

Ni pose ni ppe.
The girl baby's clothes.

Masculine gender

The masculine gender includes words for men and boys, and for masculine property. This includes many words for tools and weapons. Males of higher animals are also assigned the masculine gender. There are relatively few words in the masculine gender that are not tied to men and men's habits.

Plural gender

Also called epicene, the plural gender encompasses all words for groups, even if the group is internally homogeneous. It also includes all mass nouns and some inanimate objects perceived as indifferent to number.

Pu pperi o ontu nsōnē.
The rain fell on the boy.
Si nano pu mpobe nsenessi.
The king spoke to the people.

Dynamic gender assignment

As in most related languages, inanimate objects take on the gender of their owners when they are used in a possessive construction. Thus one can say

Mi pempi.
Her umbrella.
Si pempi.
His umbrella.
Ni pempi.
Her umbrella. (Owner is a young girl)
N pempi. (pronounced /mpempi/)
The child's umbrella.

Repetition of classifier prefixes for possessed objects can cause mutation of the agent marker. Thus one says

Si ontu.
The boy,

But

So ontu so pensipe ēponsi.
The boy is wearing his mask.

Here, si shifts to so because the sentence is transitive.

Additional notes on word placement and syntax

Baywatch requires the use of many short words that would in other languages be either null morphemes or fusional inflections. Notably, the use of tools is denoted by a construction that behaves like a serial verb. For example, one says

Si pe ppu nōpi pu mpi.
The man is slicing the apple (with a knife).

Rather than having a dedicated word that means slice, Baywatch instead specifies that the man is holding (ppu) a knife () and that the knife goes across (pi) something. Thus there is no word for slicing that does not contain a morpheme indicating a cutting implement. Even this word can be Romanized as /nō pi/; the native syllabary is ambiguous as to word spacing.

SOV analysis

Lastly, also note that the morpheme /ppu/ can be also analyzed not as "hold" but as the epicene patient marker, but in this analysis, there is no verb, because /pi/ cannot be the verb whose patientive argument is the knife since the knife is not what is being crossed. Thus one would need to create a second /pi/ that has a meaning such as "use ___ to go across something with". And this would be SOV rather than SVO.

SVO analysis

However, the dominant word order is SVO. In this analysis, /ppu/ can be interpreted as a merger of the verb for "hold" with the epicene patient marker, and /nō pi/~/nōpi/ as a verbal noun meaning "slice", whether or not it is broken into its two constituent morphemes of "knife" and "go across".

WHERE DOES THE TENSE MARKER GO?

DRM Vocabulary

Baywatch is rich in terms for hand and body movements, mostly compounds such as opo "to smear with the fingertips", sepo "to spread the palms", upu "to squeeze with both hands", and so on. There are comparatively few words for abstract concepts, and the above words are used metaphorically for concepts that in related languages have dedicated roots.

Baywatch shares the trait of having different words for biting and other body motions, but for a different reason: in Baywatch these words are historically compounds, whereas in many other related languages the different words exist merely by tradition.

Pi mpu se punepu pu ppe.
The girl bit the strawberries.
Ne ī sōmpupi mpu.
The bird bit the girl.
Ne pase se lime ntu.
The dog bit the boy.
Ne pompi se tabu.
The beetle bit me.

These four words exist because the four animals involved have greatly different mouth anatomy.

Syntactic evolution

In some cases, the word for an abstract concept has lost its independent use. This most often happens when a compound of two monosyllables fuses in one or both directions with surrounding morphemes. For example, the basic root word for law is posu,[2] originally "bundle of arrows", but the most common use of this word in Baywatch is poso,where a previously existing suffix has fused with the root. This once meant "to show or spread a bundle of arrows", referring to the threat of enforcing the law by showing one's weapons, but most students learn /poso/ before they ever hear of /posu/, and picture in their minds anything from a weapon to a book as the root has no use outside this phrase.

Notes

  1. the /ne/ is from /nu-i/
  2. This /po/ is an independent morpheme, but the shift of /pu a/ > /po/ is so common that speakers construed it as plural early on.