Languages of Teppala: Difference between revisions

From FrathWiki
Jump to navigationJump to search
 
(360 intermediate revisions by 2 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
 
*PLEASE PARDON THE MESS
*PAGE UNDER CONSTRUCTION


Humans on the planet [[Teppala]] are confined to a single continent, '''Rilola''', and its offshore islands.  Thus all human languages can be traced back to a single starting point, and have many traits in common.   
Humans on the planet [[Teppala]] are confined to a single continent, '''Rilola''', and its offshore islands.  Thus all human languages can be traced back to a single starting point, and have many traits in common.   
Line 18: Line 16:
==Traits common to all Teppalan languages==
==Traits common to all Teppalan languages==
===Phonology===
===Phonology===
*Bilabial consonants are very common, especially the stops ''p b'' and the nasal ''m''.  The commonest stop in a language is usually /p/.  However, in some languages, this is because /p/ stands alone whereas stops further back in the mouth are divided into several articulation types.  For example, a language may have /p/ as its only bilabial stop but also have a distinction between a plain velar stop /k/ and an ejective /ḳ/; or there may be co-articulations such as palatalization or labialization associated with dorsal stops but not with bilabials.
#Bilabial consonants are very common, especially the stops ''p b'' and the nasal ''m''.  
*The commonest syllable shape is always CV, even in languages that allow dense consonant clusters and/or diphthongs and triphthongs.
#The commonest syllable shape is always CV, even in languages that allow dense consonant clusters and/or diphthongs and triphthongs.
*If a language has only one series of coarticulated consonants, they are labialized consonants.
#If a language has only one series of coarticulated consonants, they are labialized consonants.
#No language has more than six vowels.  All vowels are one of these inventories:
#::/a i u/
#::/a ə i u/
#::/a e i o u/
#::/a e i o u ɨ/
#::/a ə ɨ/
#:Some differences in Romanization may appear such as writing a schwa as ''e'' or the high central vowel as schwa.
#Any consonant that can occur in syllable-final position can also occur in syllable-initial position.
#Languages are ''consonant-strong'': consonants have greater effect on surrounding vowels than vowels have on consonants.  For example, if all vowels are inherently unrounded, rounded allophones appear adjacent to labialized consonants.


===Grammar===
===Grammar===
Line 29: Line 36:
==Traits common to most Teppalan languages==
==Traits common to most Teppalan languages==
===Phonology===
===Phonology===
*Very few Teppalan languages have at any time in history ever gone beyond six vowels, and when there are six vowels, it is always /a e i o u ə/, where the /ə/ vowel may have considerable allophony.
#The commonest stop in a language is usually /p/.  However, in some languages, this is because /p/ stands alone whereas stops further back in the mouth are divided into several articulation types.  For example, a language may have /p/ as its only bilabial stop but also have a distinction between a plain velar stop /k/ and an ejective //; or there may be co-articulations such as palatalization or labialization associated with dorsal stops but not with bilabials.
*Languages with five or more phonemic vowels often do not permit diphthongs; those that do have a very small set. [[Thaoa]] is an outlier in that it has six phonemic vowels and several diphthongs.  Note that rising diphthongs are generally parsed as a sequence of consonant + vowel since, in almost all Teppalan languages, there is no restriction on which vowels can follow an onset of [j] or [w].
#Languages with five or more phonemic vowels often do not permit diphthongs; those that do have a very small set. [[Thaoa]] is an outlier in that it has six phonemic vowels and several diphthongs.  Note that rising diphthongs are generally parsed as a sequence of consonant + vowel since, in almost all Teppalan languages, there is no restriction on which vowels can follow an onset of [j] or [w].
*Dense consonant clusters do not appear, except in some languages where a certain vowel, usually /a/ or /ə/, is not distinguished at the phonemic level from silence.  That is, some languages may always pronounce /tk/ as [tək], without the schwa actually being present as a phoneme.
#Dense consonant clusters do not appear, except in some languages where a certain vowel, usually /a/ or /ə/, is not distinguished at the phonemic level from silence.  That is, some languages may always pronounce /tk/ as [tək], without the schwa actually being present as a phoneme.
*Syllables are commonly front-loaded, such that a sequence like /papsa/ is more likely to be pronounced ''[pa.psa]'' rather than ''*[pap.sa]''.  This happens most often when the first element of a cluster is lower on the sonority hierarchy than any following consonants, but in many languages, nasal-stop clusters such as /mp nt ŋk/ will also be front-loaded.
#Syllables are commonly front-loaded, such that a sequence like /papsa/ is more likely to be pronounced ''[pa.psa]'' rather than ''*[pap.sa]''.  This happens most often when the first element of a cluster is lower on the sonority hierarchy than any following consonants, but in many languages, nasal-stop clusters such as /mp nt ŋk/ will also be front-loaded.
*On the continent of Rilola, the voiced velar stop /ġ/ (IPA /g/) is rarely used even in languages with a robust series of voiced stops.  This is a long-standing trait and crosses language family boundaries.  On the islands of Laba, however, /ġ/ is common.
#On the continent of Rilola, the voiced velar stop /ġ/ (IPA /g/) is rarely used even in languages with a robust series of voiced stops.  This is a long-standing trait and crosses language family boundaries.  On the islands of Laba, however, /ġ/ is common.
*There are no minimal pairs between a diphthong and a sequence of the same two vowels. Thus diphthongs can be analyzed as allophones of vowel sequences.
#There are no minimal pairs between a diphthong and a sequence of the same two vowels. Thus diphthongs can be analyzed as allophones of vowel sequences.
*Voiceless obstruents occur more frequently than voiced ones. In some languages, /b/ or /d/ is the only voiced stop.  In others, there are no voiced stops at all but the voiced velar fricative /g/ (IPA /ɣ/) takes on a stop allophone after a nasal or a high tone.
#Voiceless obstruents occur more frequently than voiced ones. In some languages, /b/ or /d/ is the only voiced stop.  In others, there are no voiced stops at all but the voiced velar fricative /g/ (IPA /ɣ/) takes on a stop allophone after a nasal or a high tone.
*There are often marginal consonant phonemes. These mostly arise from previously existing consonant clusters that were worn down. However, some marginal phonemes arise from sound changes affecting consonants that previously were more common, which survived in only a few phonemic environments. For example, in Khulls voiced stops survived a lenition shift only after a nasal. Later, the nasal sometimes disappeared, meaning that the voiced stops could no longer be analyzed as allophones of voiced fricatives. But they remained rare.
#There are often marginal consonant phonemes. These mostly arise from previously existing consonant clusters that were worn down. However, some marginal phonemes arise from sound changes affecting consonants that previously were more common, which survived in only a few phonemic environments. For example, in Khulls voiced stops survived a lenition shift only after a nasal. Later, the nasal sometimes disappeared, meaning that the voiced stops could no longer be analyzed as allophones of voiced fricatives. But they remained rare.
*It is common to have restrictions forbidding certain consonants to appear in certain parts of a word; for example, in Khulls /r/ cannot begin a word. Most languages allow only a small subset of their consonants to appear at the end of a word
#It is common to have restrictions forbidding certain consonants to appear in certain parts of a word; for example, in Khulls /r/ cannot begin a word. Most languages allow only a small subset of their consonants to appear at the end of a word


===Grammar===
===Grammar===
Line 58: Line 65:
*Person markers on nouns, denoting their possessor, are common. Languages that lose this system often redevelop it from grammatically unrelated words later on.
*Person markers on nouns, denoting their possessor, are common. Languages that lose this system often redevelop it from grammatically unrelated words later on.


==List of sound changes==
==List of very early sound changes==
 
Many minor languages have been wiped from this list; some are recorded on their own articles, but others are only in the edit history.
   
===Pre-Mapi changes===
===Pre-Mapi changes===
====Proto-Greater-Laban (18343 BC) to Mapi (year 14000 BC)====
====Proto-Greater-Laban (18343 BC) to Mapi (year 14000 BC)====
<br><br>
The Pre-Proto-Macro-Hyper-Greater-Laban (PGL) language had a consonant inventory of /pʷ p mʷ m hʷ w t c s č š j k kʷ ŋ ŋʷ h l r ř/ and a vowel inventory of /a i u ə/.  Schwa is usually spelled "e".  /ŋ/ was allophonically [g] and the other nasals could become stops occasionally in word-initial position.  At first, "c" was homophonous with the cluster /ts/, but later /ts/ was dragged down to dental and /c/ remained alveolar.
The Proto-Greater-Laban (PGL) language had a consonant inventory of /pʷ p mʷ m hʷ w t c s č š j k kʷ ŋ ŋʷ h l r ř/ and a vowel inventory of /a i u ə/.  Schwa is usually spelled "e".  /ŋ/ was allophonically [g] and the other nasals could become stops occasionally in word-initial position.  At first, "c" was homophonous with the cluster /ts/, but later /ts/ was dragged down to dental and /c/ remained alveolar.


See [[Primordial scratchpad]] for details on the language.


#The primordial final nasal -/<b>n</b>/ disappeared, but lengthened any preceding vowel (including schwa).
#The primordial final nasal -/<b>n</b>/ disappeared, but lengthened any preceding vowel (including schwa).
Line 91: Line 97:
#The labialized fricative sequences ''sw šw xw hw'' changed to '''h'''.  Note that there was still an /x/, from earlier /kh/. THus there was a contrast between /h/ vs /x/, but neither of them had labialized forms.
#The labialized fricative sequences ''sw šw xw hw'' changed to '''h'''.  Note that there was still an /x/, from earlier /kh/. THus there was a contrast between /h/ vs /x/, but neither of them had labialized forms.
#The short vowels <b>e o</b> merged as <em>ə</em> (usually spelled "e").  The long vowels <b>ē ō</b> merged as <em>ə̄</em> (usually spelled "ē").
#The short vowels <b>e o</b> merged as <em>ə</em> (usually spelled "e").  The long vowels <b>ē ō</b> merged as <em>ə̄</em> (usually spelled "ē").
===Pre-Tapilula changes===
 
====Mapi (14000 BC) to Southeast Laban (8000 BC)====
===Pre-Tapilula changes===  
;NOTE: Some languages in this section are reconstructed internally, meaning that they have the smallest possible phonology capable of developing into the phonologies of its immediate daughter languages.  Marginal phonemes can thus be inserted arbitrarily into any language provided that they merge into one of the phonemes of the daughter language.
;NOTE: Some languages in this section are reconstructed internally, meaning that they have the smallest possible phonology capable of developing into the phonologies of its immediate daughter languages.  Marginal phonemes can thus be inserted arbitrarily into any language provided that they merge into one of the phonemes of the daughter language.


Line 100: Line 105:
There were 3 central vowels: /a ɜ ɨ/.  Further back in time, each of these three could be preceded by the palatal glide /y/, even after a consonant, but later the palatal glide disappeared after labials & labialized consonants, and merged with the other consonants to form ''palataloids''. This happened without creating any new vowels.   
There were 3 central vowels: /a ɜ ɨ/.  Further back in time, each of these three could be preceded by the palatal glide /y/, even after a consonant, but later the palatal glide disappeared after labials & labialized consonants, and merged with the other consonants to form ''palataloids''. This happened without creating any new vowels.   


The Mapi language started out with 13 "core" consonants: /p m t n s l r k ŋ x h w y/, from which can be derived all of the consonants of the daughter languages '''Silatibarra''' (8000 BC; sometimes just called "Southeast Laban"), '''proto-paleo-Andanese''' (PPAnd), and '''proto-macro-Haswarabic''' (PMH). However the phonology also had additional consonants which merged with these core consonants in various ways.  The total consonant inventory thus was:
The   daughter languages are '''Silatibarra''' (8000 BC; sometimes just called "Southeast Laban"), '''proto-paleo-Andanese''' (PPAnd), and '''proto-macro-Haswarabic''' (PMH).    


Rounded labials:      pʷ   w
All of the labialized consonants were "stops" (nasal or oral). The labialized nasals /ŋʷ/ may have been pronounced as voiced stops (as in Khulls). The lack of the expected */xʷ hʷ/ was due to a late change in the parent language, and in many ways the /h/ patterned as if it were the labialized counterpart of /x/.  
Plain bilabials:      p  m
Alveolars:            t  n  s  l  r
Postalveolars:        č  ň  š  ł  ř
Palatals:            ć  ń  ś 
Velars:              k  ŋ  x
Labiovelars:          kʷ ŋʷ
  Glottals:                  h


Thus all of the labialized consonants were "stops" (nasal or oral).  The lack of the expected */xʷ hʷ/ was due to a late change in the parent language, and in many ways the /h/ patterned as if it were the labialized counterpart of /x/. 
Syllables were CVC at maximum, and there were limits on the coda ... a preference for nasal codas if there is one.  PoA was not contrastive before another consonant.  The final stops allowed were /t, kw/ and possibly /ć/. There were thus no syllables like /kʷya/ or even /pya/.  
 
Syllables were CVC at maximum, and there were limits on the coda ... a preference for nasal codas if there is one.  PoA was not contrastive before another consonant.  . There were thus no syllables like /kʷya/ or even /pya/.  


Note that Mapi is the parent language of the '''Paleo-Andanese''' languages, so called because they are the ancestral languages of the people who, 14000 years later, began to speak Tapilula and later Andanese.
Note that Mapi is the parent language of the '''Paleo-Andanese''' languages, so called because they are the ancestral languages of the people who, 14000 years later, began to speak Tapilula and later Andanese.
Line 119: Line 115:
Stops & frics early on came to be voiced when occurring after a nasal.
Stops & frics early on came to be voiced when occurring after a nasal.


--------
===Mapi (14000 BC) to Primordial MRCA (11000 BC)===


#Fricatives before a nasal or stop changed to the PoA of the stop.  They did not assimilate in labialization, however.
The grammar of this language may have had infixes, because they are present in both '''Owl''' and Tapilula.  The MRCA of those is in the middle of this chart, ~11000 BCNonetheless, Tapilula's infixes can be "recovered" (biological sense) from suffixes and therefore Mapi can have essentially any grammar.
#The glottal fricatve ''h'' disappeared unconditionally.  This created vowel sequences of /aa aɜ aɨ ɜa ɜɜ ɜɨ ɨa ɨɜ ɨɨ/ most of which occurred as the only vocoid in a word since most roots had been bisyllabic. 
#:The diphthongs with  high vowels came to have a true /i/ or true /u/ depending on the adjacent consonant.  There was no neutral.
#:The double sequences changed to long vowels. Thus the nine sequences changed as ''/aa aɜ aɨ/ /ɜa ɜɜ ɜɨ/ /ɨa ɨɜ ɨɨ/'' > '''a: a: ai~au/ /ya~wa ə: əi~əu/ /ya~wa yə~wə yɨ~wɨ/.'''  Thus the language now could be analyzed with 4 vowels or 3. 
#The new vocoid sequences  attached to consonants.  Thus new "mismatched" palatalized ('''pʲ mʲ ''') and labialized ('''tʷ nʷ sʷ lʷ rʷ xʷ hʷ''') consonants were created, and they were a fullsetHowever, the palatalized ones are merged before /i/, and the labialized ones are merged before /u/, so the setup is not far from still being of a vertical inventory, but with changed consonants.
#The front allophones of the diphthongs ''aɨ əɨ'' became '''a: ə:''' and palatalized any following consonant (it would not have ever been labialized).
#Fricatives became weakened between vowels: ''s š ś x'' changed to '''h ś ś h'''. 
#''ś'' may have become '''y'''.
#:''NOTE ON POLITICS: PALEO-ZENITH SPLITS OFF HERE.''
#Fricatives in syllable-final position changed also to one of '''ś h hʷ'''.  Thus the fricvative /x/ occurred only before vowels.
#The velar fricative ''x'' came to be seen as an allophone of '''h'''.
#Other consonant cluster reduction:  
#:''hr rh'' > '''hh''' (can be labialized from either original enemenet.)
#:Possibly ''ts č ć'' > '''s š ś'''? Remember that stops did not become voiced, but may have instead tended towards frication to replace the missing voiced fricatibes.
#:''lʷ'' > '''ḷ''' (dental).  In fact, probably all of the labialized alveolars become dentals.  This would help explain why they are never palatalized.
#The labial consonants ''p pʷ pʲ'' became '''h hʷ ś''' between vowels. /hʷ/ is sometimes spelled "f" because its roundedness was more malleable than that of /xʷ/. 
#The geminate stops ''ppʷ pp tt čč ćć kk kkʷ'' became single: '''pʷ p t č ć k kʷ'''.
#Fricatives behind voiceless stops disappeared.
#Most postalveolar consonants  trapped in final positions become plain alveolars: <b>č  ñ ł</b> become <em>t  n l</em>.  Note that there were no longer any palatals in the language.
#All final fricatives disappeared.


init:


True /i/ & /u/ will appear from lenited fricatives in the syllable coda, as in the "pineapple " language.
Rounded labials:      pʷ mʷ    w
Plain bilabials:      p  m
Alveolars:            t  n  s  l  r
Postalveolars:        č  ň  š  ł  ř
Palatals:            ć  ń  ś 
Velars:              k  ŋ  x
Labiovelars:          kʷ ŋʷ
Glottals:                  h


===Southeast Laban (8000 BC) to Pre-Proto-Tapilula (3770 BC)===
#č > š (retroflex, not postalveolar)
#(pre-Silatibarra changes: t`a &gt; t`e &gt; te, etc)
#retroflex <b>r`</b> changed to <em>g</em> (a fricatve) before a vowel.
#retroflex <b>r`</b> disappears, changing to a schwa vowel <em>e</em> if after a vowel.
#Geminate nasals <b>mm ṇṇ nn n`n` ŋŋ</b> became nasal + stop: <em>mb ṇḍ nd n`d` ŋġ</em>. '''NOTE: the nasal + stop sequence *already* was voiced from before.  later /mp/, etc is from cmpinds.'''
# Voiceless stop + nasal became a simple voiced stop: <b>pm ṭṇ tn t`n` kŋ</b> into <em>b ḍ d d` ġ</em>.
#Word-initial <b>ř</b> became  <em>d</em>.  Word-initial <b>w</b> became <em>b</em>. 
#Adjacent to a retroflex consonant, in either direction, the vowel <b>a</b> changed into <em>e</em> ( a schwa).
#The voiceless stops <b>p t k </b> between vowels became <em>b d ġ</em>. Intervocalic fricatives became voiced.
#The sequence <b>gi</b> before a vowel became <em>ž</em> (even if accented, due to analogy).
#i....u &gt; i...e; u...i &gt; u....e .
#Final schwa was deleted.
#A few sequences, like abi&gt; au, occcuirred here. (only if the /a/ was stressed).
#The voiceless fricative ''s'' (never between vowels) became '''h''' unconditionally.
#Voiced fricatives before a vowel were smeared out: ''v'' changed all preceding vowels into /u/; ''ž'' changed only /e/ &gt; /i/; ''g z'' disappeared with no effect on the preceding vowel.
#Gemionates weree simplified.  Thus voiceless interovolcalic stops were created.


===Pre-Proto-Tapilula (3770 BC) to Tapilula (500 AD)===
#The fricatives ''š ś h'' shifted to '''y y Ø ''' unconditionally .  This created vowel sequences of /aa aɜ aɨ ɜa ɜɜ ɜɨ ɨa ɨɜ ɨɨ/ most of which occurred as the only vocoid in a word since most roots had been bisyllabic. 
#The sequences ''ɨa ɨɜ ɨɨ'' shifted to '''ʕa ʕɜ ʕɨ''', where /ʕ/ is a voiced stop after a nasal and otherwise a fricative(?). Thus prenasalized stops were created.  rule3 might not be needed if we keep h
#The sequences ''ɨwa ɨwɜ ɨwɨ'' shifted to ''ʷa ʷɜ ʷɨ''. 
#The sequences ''ɨya ɨyɜ  ɨyɨ'' shifted to '''ʲa ʲɜ ʲɨ        '''. Thus new "mismatched" palatalized ('''pʲ mʲ ''') and labialized ('''tʷ nʷ sʷ lʷ rʷ xʷ  ''') consonants were created, and they were a full set.
#The sequences ''aa aɜ ɜa ɜɜ'' shifted to '''a: a: ɜ: ɜ:'''. ''aɨ ɜɨ'' shifted to '''  ɜ: ɜ:''' as well. 
#''mʷ nʷ ŋʷ'' > '''mbʷ nd ŋġʷ'''.  These may have also been the reflexes of /mw nw ŋw/, but the syllable boundaries would have kept them apart even so.  That is, a syllable-straddling /m-w/ would turn into /m-mbʷ/. 
#Fricatives became weakened between vowels, and in syllable-final position: ''s  x sʷ xʷ'' changed to '''h  h hʷ hʷ'''.
#The cluster ''ts'' shifted to '''s'''.
#The labial consonants ''p pʷ  pʲ'' became '''B w  y'''.


The original dialect of Pre-Tapilula (so called because it had no other descendants), spoken around 3770 BC, had 18 consonants (<em>  p b m w ṭ ḍ ṇ ḷ t d n l r j k ġ ŋ h</em>&nbsp;&nbsp; ) and 4 vowels, <em>a i u ə</em> .


#Note that far back in the past, nn &gt; nd, hence explaining why there is no /nn/. Also tn &gt; d. In a few compounds these rulse still apply by analogy.
And the vowel inventory was probably  
# Word-initial ''ř'' became  '''d'''.
#Before a vowel, the sequences ''əi əu'' changed into '''i u'''.
#The vowel ''ə'' became '''u''' before a labial in a closed syllable.
#Palatalized alveolars change: ''tʲ dʲ nʲ lʲ rʲ'' into '''š ž ň ź ź'''.  Note that this did not affect dentals.
#Palatalized velars change: ''kʲ ġʲ ŋʲ hʲ'' into '''š ž ň š'''. 
#The palatalized dentals  ''ḍʲ ṇʲ'' changed into '''ž ň'''.  This did not affect the voiceless form. 
#The sequences ''iŋ uŋ'', in syllable-final position, became lowered (allophonically at first) to '''ɛŋ  oŋ'''.  This /ɛ/ is the true /e/, not a schwa. This vowel was allophonically nasalized, but could not carry an additional coda, and therefore patterned like the other nasal codas.
#Syllable-final stops ''p ṭ t k'' before a "strong consonant" changed into the glottal stop '''ʔ''', which itself was generally silent except for changing the tone of the syllable to high.  However, clusters like /pl/ instead changed to /kl/.
#Syllabic nasals were created: ''im əm um'' > '''ṁ''', ''ən'' > '''ṅ''', and ''əŋ'' > '''ŋ̇'''.
#:<s>The sequences /wi/ and /ju/, before another a vowel, became /w/.  (when???)</s>
#Syllable-initial clusters such as ''kl'' changed into pharyngealized consonants such as '''ḳ'''. <--- note that a previous shift (not listed here) did things like pl>xl>kl, thus this shift produces mostly the velar ejective /ḳ/, not the others.
#DEPALATALIZATION!!!!!!!!!!!!!!! ''j'' was changed to '''ʕ''' in all positions.  Between vowels, this became silent but used to show diphthongs' separation.  Thus the palatalized consonants ''pʲ bʲ mʲ ṭʲ'' became the pharyngealized  clusters '''pʕ bʕ mʕ ṭʕ'''.   
#:''NOTE: there is *no* ṇʕ''.
#The postalveolar nasal ''ň'' (from earlier /ṇʲ nʲ ŋʲ/) changed to '''ŋ'''.
#The voiced sounds ''ž ź r'' changed to '''y'''  .  ''lʲ'' probably also to '''y''', and maybe even ''ḷʲ''.
#The vowel sequences <b>aə əa</b> changed to <em>au ua</em>.  <b>əi</b> became <em>ui</em> (/wi/).
#<s>An <b>h</b> after an accented vowel metathesized and created an aspirated consonant at the beginning of the word, and leaving a gap in between the other two vowels.  Note that voiceless stops were always aspirated, so this shift did not change them.</s>
#''h'' was removed to ''' Ø''' in all positions (?), only to reappear later. 
#The sequence ''ua'' changed to '''ʷo'''. ''yʷ'' > '''y''', thus the word for hand is just "yò". 
#The vowel sequences ''ai au'' changed to '''ɛ o''' unconditionally. 
#A schwa in an unaccented syllable after an accented /ɛ/ or /o/ changed to match that vowel.  Some double schwa words also changed, e.g. ''məhə'' > '''moho''' "fire". 
#The "clear" labialized consonants ''ṭʷ ḍʷ ṇʷ kʷ ŋʷ'' became the rounded bilabials '''pʷ bʷ mʷ pʷ mʷ'''.
#The pharyngealized  labialized consonants ''ṭʕʷ ṇʕʷ kʕʷ'' became '''pʕʷ mʕʷ pʕʷ'''.  hʷ became fʷ, but the orthography was retained.
#:''NOTE!! '' this accounts for most of what was once marked "dʷ".
#:<s>Nasalized vowels caused the stops perceiving them to become deaspirated.  This affects /m/ as well.</s>
#:<s>Aspirated voiced stops became voiceless fricatives.  </s>
#The plain voiced stops ''b bʕ ḍ d ġ'' and the voiced fricatives '' (?)'' all shifted  to '''g''', except after a nasal.
#:Note, since the language already has pharyngealized nasals, it is very likely that theses stops will merge.  e.g. /mʕ/ = /mg/, which is [mb].
#The labialized voiced stops ''bʷ dʷ'' became '''w''', except after a nasal.
#The voiceless fricatives ''s š'' (and any others that might exist) merged as '''h'''.
#:possibly /hy/ > /y/.  It would be a rare sequence in any case. 
#Syllables *preceding* heavy syllables became '''LOW ''' tone.
#Other final nasals also change into nasal vowel markers, thus changing syllable weight.
#The  nasals ''m mʷ'' became '''mh mhʷ''' before a vowel facing another labial.  This did not affect the pharyngealized versions.  (The other nasals may have participated in this shift, but only appeared in the "Fojy" branch.)


Thus the final phonology was:
SHORT VOWELS


  Rounded bilabials:    pʷ  pʕʷ mʷ  mbʷ~mʕʷ mhʷ~hʷ  w
  a   ɜ   ɨ
Spread bilabials:      p   pʕ  m   mb ~mʕ  mh ~f  (Ø)
Dentals:              ṭ  ṭʕ  ṇ  ṇḍ              ḷ
Alveolars:            t      n  nd              l
Rounded alveolars:    tʷ      nʷ  ndʷ       
Velars:                k  ḳ  ŋ  ŋġ          h  g


The (f) was pronounced [mʰ] in some dialects.
LONG VOWELS AND FALLING DIPHTHONGS


====Nuclear Tapilula====
ā  ai  au
''NOTE'': the following changes apply only to the Andanese/Gold branch of the family (which also includes minor families like [[Subumpamese]]). 
  ɜ̄  ɜi ɜu
#The aspirated nasals ''mhʷ mh'' become '''hʷ f''', where /f/ patterns as a spread version of /hʷ/. (The other aspirated nasals either never arose, or deaspirated.)
ɨ̄  ī   ū
#The prenasalized voiced stops ''mb ṇḍ nd ndʷ ŋġ'' changed to plain voiced stops '''b ḍ d dʷ ġ'''.
#The glottalized stops <b>pʕ pʕʷ ṭʕ kʕ</b> changed to <em>b bʷ ḍ ḳ</em> unconditionally.
#The rounded labials ''pʷ bʷ mʷ mʕʷ'' became plain labials '''p b m mʕ'''.    


See [[Repilian languages]].


;use triple coin flips to determine which high tone syllables previously ended in a nasal.
===Primordial (11000 BC) to Southeast Laban (8000 BC)===
 
#The fricatives ''h hʷ'' (including the newly generated ones) in syllable-final position voiced to  ''' Ø w'''.  ś had been shifted to /y/ earlier.  Thuis frics no longer occured at end of tillable.
===Tapilula to Gold (year 1900)===
#'' rh'' > ''' h'''.  
Alternate names: Ukieipi, G̣ʷidiʕìləs
#''   mʲ'' > ''' mb'''.  
 
#Possibly, also hʷ h ś > vless stops when after a nasal, but they later  become voiced in Tapilula.
''Important note '':  dictionary '''' should be broken into '''b''' (from //) and '''ndʷ''' (true dʷ in gold branch, variable elsewhere).
#The voiceless stops ''č ć'' shifted to '''s₂ s₃ '''   (cover symbol: '''$''') between vowels.
 
#Most postalveolar consonants  trapped in final positions become plain alveolars: <b>č  ñ ł</b> become <em>t  n l</em>.  Note that there were no longer any palatals in the language.
'''Note on politics: It is not clear whether the Tapilula language split apart in 0 AD or in 500 AD.  If 500, the various Subumpamese languages are almnost independent branches since they begin diverging not long aftre 600 AD.  However, Sub and Gold share much more in common than either of htem does with Andanese, despite being supposedly only 100 years closer together. So perhaps the true date is 0 after all.'''
#''ć ń ś'' >  '''s₄ ň s₅'''.   
 
#Probably ''lʷ'' > '''w'''; maybe ł > y.
====Pre-classical changes====
#''s₅ s'' > '''s₁'''.
These changes have already taken place as of the time of the dictionary of Tapilula.
#Probably ''tʷ'' > '''kʷ'''.  
#The dentals ''ṭ ḍ ṇ'' became the alveolars '''t d n'''.
#The sequences ''ɜ̄ ɜi  ɨ̄  ī    '' shifted to '''ē ē ī ī''' unconditionally.
 
#The high vowel ''ɨ'' shifted to '''ū''' before a labialized consonant in a closed syllable. (This is a dummy shift to make it easier to understand changes that happen in Tapilula, but it may be of use in other daughter languages as well.)
 
#Final ''-ɨ'' after a labialized consonant shifted to '''u'''.  Final ''-ɨ'' after a palatalized consonant shifted to '''i'''. These are both dummy shifts as well.
#The aspirated velar stop <b>k</b> became <em>č</em> before the vowel <em>i</em>.  If another vowel followed, the /i/ disappeared.  This happened even if the /i/ was accented ... e.g. takʰìa &gt; tačă.
#When a "velaroid" consonant (<b>k ŋ h g l</b>) followed an accented high tone vowel and the following vowel was the same, that following vowel disappeared, leaving a closed syllable. (Initial-stress diphthongs metathesized; e.g. <b>ūhi</b> became <em>ūih</em> and <b>āku</b> became <em>āuk</em>.)  If the next syllable had begun with a vowel, a <em>ʡ</em> (voiced pharyngeal fricative) was added there.  These new syllables were all high tone, and were the only closed syllables in the language. Thus the high tone came to be associated with closed syllables.  "l" had been considered a velar consonant because of certain distribution patterns dating from Standard Animal, even though it was not a velarized l.
#:'''NOTE ON POLITICS: SUBUMPAMESE CAN BE CONSIDERED TO BREAK OFF HERE, AS THIS IS THE LAST OF THE SHARED CHANGES. ''' (that is, there were only A FEW of them)
#effective unconditional merger of [a e o] > /a/, but see paper for details.
#In absolute final position, syllable-final ''ŋ'' changed to '''n'''.   
#Accented syllables gained a pharyngeal <em>ʕ</em> as an onset if a consonant was not already there.  If there was an /x/ (resulting from syllable-final /h/), it became <em>ĥ</em> (a "true" /h/, not like {h} which really is /x/).  Thus, all long vowels now had their stress on the first vowel and thus had a falling tone.
#After long vowels, all consonants became voiced. Also, consonants occurring after initial vowels also became voiced.  This created the new consonants <em>b v vʷ d z ġ dž</em> .  Thus, final <b>-s</b> in words like hʷīs became <em>-z</em>.  However, analogy made it so that the change was confined to open syllables in most words. (''This could also mean it didnt affect clusters.'')
#After initial unstressed /u/, all consonants became labializedThus uġau "heart" &gt; uġʷau.  This change extended even to clusters.
#Initial vowels were deleted unless an illegal consonant cluster would have resulted.  Sometimes initial vowels were retained due to classifier prefixes.
#All schwas and diphthongs became low tone.
#Labialized consonants lost their labialization when occuring after another labial or labialized consonant (used to explain bàigʷa &gt; bàiga).  This also affected labialized consonants occurring before syllabic ṁ ?  
#After a stressed syllable, intervocalic <b>ʕ ʕʷ</b> became <em>g gʷ</em>.
#:'''NOTE ON POLITICS''': ''THIS IS WHERE THAOA SPLITS OFF (YR 1085 AD)''
#The velar fricatives <b>h g</b> were rounded to <em>hʷ gʷ</em> before <em>u</em>.
#Some fricatives change: <b>f v fʲ vʲ fʷ vʷ</b> became <em>s z ħ ʕ ħʷ ʕʷ</em>.
#In absolute final position, syllable-final ''h'' changed to '''s'''. (This could be earlier since it is in Thaoa too.)
#The labialized coronals ''tʷ dʷ nʷ'' changed to '''tl dl nl'''.
 
   
   
Note: unlike the previous model, areal influences cannot be assumed here to keep the breakoff date of Thaoa at 1085 while having only a few changes not shared. This is because Thaoa and AlphaLEAP are no longer geographically contiguous. However,it had been a bad model anyway since it assumed friendly relations with Thaoa's enemy, [[Paba]].
The chronology of the changes is not a problem, however. At most, Thaoa will have to forgo the last four shifts, but since these are mostly unconditional, they can be shown in Thaoa's sound change list as reversals of the Gold ones, so that Thaoa words (mostly placenames) can be derived from Gold instead of going back to Tapilula or setting up a middle-stage language.


===Gold (1900) to Khulls (4700)===
And the vowel inventory may have been /a ɜ ɨ/ for short vowels and /ā ē ī ō ū/ for long vowels.
Alternate names: Kuroras


This language was originally spoken near [[Bābākiam]], in the city of [[Paba]].
The prenasals contrast with syllable-straddling sequences of nasal + *voiceless* stop, and with nasal + prenasal (that is, /mmb/ etc).


NOTE: Much of what is spelled "k" here should in fact be "ḳ".
==Southeast Laban (8000 BC) to Tapilula (500 BC)==


<p class="body">
Rounded labials:                  w      mbʷ
Initial phoneme inventory: /p b f v m w t d s z n l č ǯ j k g ŋ h ɣ r a i u ə/
Plain bilabials:         m             mb  ''B''                       
<br>Four tones: <em>ă</em> indicates stressed short vowels with no tone (i.e. a low tone), <em>à</em> indicates stressed short vowels with high tone, <em>ā</em> indicates stressed long vowels with a falling tone (seen as high + low), and <em>á</em> indicates stressed long vowels with a rising tone (seen as low + high). Two short stressed high vowels (e.g. àà) do not make a long vowel because there is an inherent glottal stop in the à tone whenever it comes before another vowel or at the end of a word. Thus the glottal stop could be considered marginally phonemic. Unstressed vowels can have tone in a shadowy wayno marker indicates a low tone, and a dot above (ȧ) indicates a high tone. However, these unstressed high tones are mostly swallowed except in long sequences of low tones.    
Alveolars:            t   n       l   r nd  ''$''
Rounded alveolars:                   ()
  Postalveolars:        č  ň      λ  ř  
  Palatals:                         y  
  Velars:              k  ŋ   h              ''G''
Labiovelars:          kʷ      hʷ        ŋġʷ


Tentative consonants are in parens. Note that /nʷ/ is not inherited from MRCA, so it's possible that /tʷ/ > /kʷ/ > /p/ and that all /tʷ/ in Tapilula is secondary just as all nʷ must be.


<br>
There may be a few other consonants not listed here resulting from the creation of new labialized and palatalized consonants in the parent language.
Voiced stops other than "d" are rare, and cannot occur after another consonant except in some compounds.  That is, there is no /mb/, /bb/, etc.  But /d/ is an exception to this rule because it arose from different environments.  /b/ and /g/ were marginal, in fact, and /g/ is generally spelled ğ or ġ which leaves {g} free for the much more common phoneme /ɣ/.  But this is not carried over to the phonemic listings below.


#The vowel <b>u</b>, in all tones and lengths, changed to <em>o</em> of the same tone and length.
The cover symbol ''B'' can be used for "a consonant that needs to disappear" to '''Ø''' since there is so much vowel hiatus required to get things the way they are in Tapilula. The cover symbol ''$'' can likewise be used for any consonant that ultimately ends up as /h/, since there is so much /h/ in Tapilula as wellLastly, ''G'' indicates any consonant that ends upo as /g/, though this might be adqeuately covered by grammatical reanalysis from hiatus and not need a new consonant.
#The diphthongs <b>əi ài</b>  changed to <em>ĕ ē</em>.  <b>əu àu</b> became <em>ū ō</em>.
#Fricatives preceded by ʕ became voiced.  e.g. <b>ʕs</b> ---&gt; <em>z</em>.
#The plain schwa (which was always low tone) changed to <em>ŭ</em>. 
#Sequences like <b>aʕa</b> became pharyngealized vowels; these could still have tones, but later all pharyngealized tones merged with each other except for sandhi effects.  Pharyngealized vowels are spelled <em>â</em>.
#Voiced fricatives  became <em>ʕ</em>, which was silent when unstressed. </li>
#The aspirated stop <b>dʰ</b> became <em>tʰ</em>.  (There was no bʰ).
#Clusters of /s/ + another consonant metathesized.  /s/ in this posiiton was actually [h] and this pronunciation was actually a retention of the original pronunc of ~4000 yrs earlier.  Thus ''sm sn sŋ'' became '''mh nh ŋh'''.
#Nasals followed by /h/ turned into nasal + stop clusters. THus ''mh nh ŋh'' became '''mp nt ŋk'''.  One excception to this shift was that if the following vowel was [i] (any length, any tone), the /nt/ would instead be ''''''.  Thus '''tanči''' "wine", not *''tanti''.</li>
#After the vowel [u] (any tone, any length), ''k ḳ'' in a syllable coda became '''p ṗ'''. They were actually coarticulated labial-velar stops for quite a long time before merging with the pure bilabials, but because this merger happened in both branches of the language, it is treated as if it were such from the beginning.
#The short vowel ''ŭ'' disappeared in all positions unless an illegal consonant cluster would be created.  It labialized any consonant that preceded it.  Thus labialized versions of ''all'' consonants were created.  If the syllable ended in a  consonant (which was particularly common after schwa), instead of becoming an "illegal" cluster, the consonant became syllabic.  Thus the syllabic consonants <em>ṡ p ṗ ż ḷ</em> were created, and syllabic nasals became more common.  The "syllabic" stops /p ṗ/ were indeed pronounced without an epenthetic vowel, creating words like '''sălṗ'''.  <b>ʕəs</b> became a syllabic <em>ż</em>.
#Where /ə/ collapsed, stress shifted syllables to the nearest adjacent one.  This tone was mid-tone (ă) unless there had been a "dot" tone previously.
#After a syllabic nasal, the final stops <b>p ṗ</b> (which was the only ones that did occur) changed to match the position of the nasal.  However, these were written with the letters for "p ṗ".
#Sequences like <b>hṁm</b> (in kahṁma "dark-skinned") changed to <em>ṁp</em>, and so on. 
#The consonant cluster <b>hʷg</b> became <em>kʷ</em> (not xʷ, even though g was a fricative).
#The voiceless fricatives <b>h hʷ </b> became <em>x xʷ</em>  in all positions.
#The voiced sounds <b>b bʷ v vʷ</b> changed to <em>ʢʷ</em><b>ġ ġʷ</b> changed to <em>g gʷ</em> (fricatives).
#The coronals <b>č ǯ</b> became <em>š ž</em> in all positions.
#The voiced stop <b>d</b> became <em>l</em> in initial position, but <em>r</em> (a flap) elsewhere.  Unlike the similar changesd above, this did *not* affect dʷ.
#Labialized coronals became velar. (Possibly misplaced, but it wont matter.)</li>
#Final <b>k</b> raised the preceding vowel to a high tone à (á if it was long) and then disappeared, though it left an allophonic glottal stop in some positions.
#The labialized fricative <b>šʷ</b> became <em>hʷ</em>.
#:'''NOTE ON POLITICS:''' ''The [[Proto-Moonshine language]] breaks off here.'' (Year 3958)
#<b>ya yo</b> (on all tones) &gt; <em>ye</em>.
#Nasal consonants followed by /y/ became voiced stops: <b>my ñy</b> became <em>by ǯy</em>.  (This includes the reflexes of /ny/ and /ŋy/.)
#The palatal glide /y/ was deleted when not before a high vowel (it was only /e i u/ by now anyway).
#The clusters <b>ml nl ŋl</b> changed to <em>bl dl ġl</em>, thus restoring voiced stops to a marginal phonemic position.  WHAT ABOUT PALATALS?
#The labialized consonants <b>mʷ  ŋʷ</b> changed to <em>bʷ  ġʷ</em>.
#The sound /l/ disappeared after a voiced stop: the clusters <b>bl dl ġl</b> changed to <em>b d ġ</em>.
#The voiceless labialized nasals <b>mʷʰ nʷʰ ŋʷʰ</b> changed to <em>mpʷ ntʷ ŋkʷʰ</em>.  Non-labialized aspirated nasals also changed.  In initial position, the nasal element dropped out.  Thus there was once again a phonemic /tʷ/ in the language.  However, there were some words such as kʷhʷnōn "beak" where there was an initial consonant as well, and in these cases the word became kʷhʷṅtōn.


Thus Gold's original four tone system was preserved unchanged in Khulls: <em>ă à ā á</em> (with unstressed variants <em>a ȧ</em>).  However, minimal pairs between ā and á were few, and the whole system could still be analyzed as a length contrast, with ă corresponding to bare short vowels, à to short vowels followed by a glottal stop, ā to long vowels, and á to sequences of two short vowels (or in some words, one long vowel followed by a glottal stop).  This is the method that was used for transcription of loanwords into Bābākiam.  <br><br>  The final phonology of Kuroras was  /p pʷ m mʷ w t n s l r š ž j k ŋ x ɣ kʷ ŋʷ xʷ ɣʷ ʔ h ʢ hʷ ʢʷ b d ġ/ for consonants and /a e i o u ə/ with four tones for vowels.  Dipthongs are not distinguished from vowel sequences and are mostly determined by accentuation patterns and tone.  Many of the consonants are very marginal: /b d ġ bʷ dʷ ġʷ h hʷ ṃ ʔ ʢ ɣʷ/ all occurred in very restricted environments. Most of the labialized consonants were not particularly common either.  Thus the "major" consonants were just /p m w t n s l r š ž j k kʷ ŋ x ɣ xʷ ʢʷ/.
The ''B'' consonant was most likely just /b/, but the capital letter spelling leaves open the possibility that it traces back to more than one phoneme and that these were still distinct at a fairly late stage of development, just as ''$'' corresponds to a set of phonemes that merged only very late.


<br><br>Kuroras is unusual in that it has only unconditional changes for its
The vowel inventory was
vowels, apart from a few changes in tone.&nbsp; The other languages that
descended from Ukieipi all had a few conditional changes that "filled out"
the vowel system.&nbsp; Thus, even though Kuroras has five vowels, the
vowels /e/ and /u/ are quite rare, because they descend from diphthongs that
were themselves rare.</pd></pr></pbody></pable>
<pable><pbody><pr><pd width="185"></pd><pd>


===Macro-Pabap languages===
a ɜ ɨ
{{:Babakiam/Sound_changes}}
ā ɜ̄  ē ī ō ū
===Bābākiam (4100) to Poswa (8700)===
Alternate names: Ižda Mir, Blop, Bloppabop
NOTE that this section is outdented for prominence. Poswa is a daughter language of Bābākiam, not of Gold.
 
#Long vowels in initial syllables became double: <b>ā ī ū</b> became <em>aa ii uu</em>. </li>
#The double vowels <b>ii uu</b> became <em>ʲi ʷu</em> in all positions.
#Between two consonants in a monosyllabic word, the diphthongs <b>au ai eu ei</b> changed to <em>o ae u e</em>. iu &gt; y, ui &gt; i/y, uu &gt; u, ii &gt; i (Note: eu &gt; y if unstressed, hence peum &gt; pym &gt; pum; but this is actually a later shift) But in longer words, or when a consonant cluster was adjacent, a <b>b</b> was inserted and the individual vowels were retained.  And in word-final position they remained as diphthongs (to undergo a slightly different shift later). 
#<b>ā</b> changed to <em>aba</em> in all positions.
#At the beginning of a syllable and after <b>p m</b>, the semivowels <b>w</b> and <b>j</b> shifted to <em>r</em> and <em>l</em> respectively.  At the end of a syllable, no shift took place, but the orthography was changed as if it had. 
#The voiced postalveolar fricative <b>ž</b> changed to the palatal approximant <em>j</em> in all positions. 
#The consonant <b>šʲ</b> became <em>ś</em>, a voiceless palatal approximant.  <b>fʷ</b> became a voiceless rounded labial approximant.
#The velar stops <b>k ŋ</b> changed to the labials <em>p m</em> in final position.
#The medial clusters <b>pt mt pn mn</b> shifted to <em>tt nt tn nn</em>.  Then <b>pk mk sk pŋ mŋ sŋ</b> became <em>pt mpt št pn mn šn</em>.  <b>ms mš</b> became <em>mps mpš</em>.
#The cluster <b>sf</b> changed to <em>ff</em>.
#The labialized consonants <b>sʷ tʷ nʷ</b> shifted to <em>ps pʷ mʷ</em> in word-initial position, and <em>ps pt bʷ</em> medially. 
#The new clusters <b>lw rw ww</b> merged as <em>w</em>.
#The consonant clusters <b>mʷ mr ml ŋʷ</b> shifted to <em>bʷ br bl gʷ</em> unconditionally. 
#Meanwhile <b>šʷ</b> changed to <em>pš</em>.
#:(***MOST KURORAS LOANS ENTER HERE***)&nbsp; year 4700 
#In initial position before a vowel, the voiceless labial fricative <b>f</b> changed to <em>w</em>.  It also happened often to a word-internal -f- preceded by a /w/ of any origin.  (e.g. bwafa "hug" &gt; bwawa)  This shift did not affect <b>fʲ</b>. 
#After a labialized consonant (except <em>w</em>), the schwa vowel <b>y</b> changed to <em>u</em>.
#Labialized consonants lost their labialization when they occurred before <em>u</em>.  (But later they were re-introduced through compounding.)
#The sequences <b>ow</b> and <b>uw</b> changed to <em>o</em> and <em>u</em> respectively.&nbsp;
#:This is the stage at which most "early Poswa" loans entered the Sak languages.
#The vowels <b>i</b> and <b>u</b> changed to <em>e</em> and <em>o</em> in closed syllables. (Note that verbs still had a weak vowel -u and thus did not participate in this change.&nbsp; For the same reason, many verbs did not participate in rule 2 either and thus did not have these vowels to begin with, even if the corresponding nouns did. Example, 'rum' (child) was still rimu here.&nbsp;  However, they were later analogized as if they had in fact dropped those vowels, hence rum instead of rim.) 
#The vowel <b>y</b> in closed syllables changed to either <em>i</em> or <em>u</em> depending on the other vowel in the root. The default choice was <em>u</em>, unless it followed a <b>w</b>.  <em>i</em> was chosen only when it followed a <b>w</b> or was in a word in which an <b>i</b> or <b>e</b> was in an adjacent syllable and that was the only other vowel in the word.
#The sequences <b>wu wo</b> changed to <em>wi we</em>.
#Then, <b>s</b> and <b>p</b> disappeared before nasals and sporadically in stem-final position due to back-formation from plurals (e.g. <em>pe</em> "island" from <em>pena</em>, earlier <b>pes</b>, <b>pesna</b>).  sb -&gt; b, not to be confused with later shifts in which it turned into žb (re-compounding of words ending in s with words beginning with b happened essentially continuously; this shift was one time only).  Also not to be confused with a much earlier shift of sb -&gt; s (persistent in verbs, but nowhere else).  Thus the plurals of words ending in s lost the s in their nominal but not their verbal forms.
#Unaccented <b>i</b> changed to <em>e</em> unless the accented syllable of the word contained an <em>i</em>.
#Then unaccented <b>y</b> changed to <em>i</em> unless the syllable ended in a labial or the accented syllable of the word contained a <em>y</em>. 
#:Politically, the proto-Poswobs became independent here (5547), but the language remained unified through physical contact for another 450 years or so.
#The consonant cluster <b>ŋʲ</b> assimilated to <em>nʲ</em>. 
#Then unaccented <b>u</b> changed to <em>y</em> except when the syllable ended in a labial or the accented syllable also contained a <b>u</b>. 
#<span style="font-color: #bc00ff;">Before a vowel, unaccented <b>y</b> and <b>yb</b> changed to <em>u</em>.</span>
#<span style="font-color: #bc00ff;">Before a vowel, unaccented <b>a</b> and <b>ab</b> changed to <em>i</em> ("the karaoke shift").</span>
#Next, <b>i</b> changed to <em>y</em> if the next syllable had <em>u</em>. 
#The palatalized consonants <b>pʲ mʲ sʲ lʲ rʲ</b>, which had been created mostly by rule 28, changed to <em>f v ś j b</em> before a vowel.  <b>bʲ</b> also changed to <em>b</em>. <b>šʲ žʲ</b> became <em>ś j</em>.
#:(year 6000; THIS IS WHERE PABAPPA BREAKS OFF)
#In stressed syllables, in only a few words, <b>ol ul or ur</b> changed to <em>we wi wa wa</em>.  The conditioning environment was that the syllables had to be unstressed and have only one consonant before them; in other words, they occured in compounds only, in a syllable which would be stressed if it weren't a compound.
#Simultaneously, syllable-final <b>r</b> in most words changed to <em>b</em>.  AQcutaly, it was f/v before coronals, b in absolute final position, stays r before šž + labials + velars (but really pronounced as /w/).  rl &gt; vl.  rr &gt; wr (spelled rr for now, but later as rw).  probably š ž before šž actually, but f before s changing to š.  Sometimes au+labial -&gt; o even so, no particular rule.  It always changed to /o/ in the initial sllable of a word, ignoring all these other rules.  thus pautu &gt; poty, not pafty.
#The surviving final  <b>r</b> changed to <em>vʷ fʷ</em> before labials.  <b>fʷ</b> became <em>w</em>.
#The palatal approximants <b>ś j</b> (including any /j/ taken from loans) changed to <em>š ž</em> in all positions.
#<b>fʷ</b> became <em>w</em> before a vowel (that is, everywhere except before a labial).
#In words not affected by the previous shift (mostly due to grammatical analogy), syllable-final <b>ar</b> and <b>yr</b> shifted to <em>o</em> and <b>er</b> and <b>ir</b> shifted to <em>u</em>.  However <b>ar</b> did not shift to <em>o</em> after a /w/, but rather became <em>abʷ</em> (by analogy). 
#In unstressed syllables before a nasal, the sequences <b>el il</b> merged as <em>i</em> and <b>or ur</b> merged as <em>u</em>. 
#Unstressed <b>ol</b> and <b>ul</b> became <em>e</em> before a consonant or at the end of a word. 
#<b>i</b> and <b>y</b> shifted to <em>u</em> before a labial in a closed syllable, or before a syllable beginning with a labial cluster (in this case, even /pʷ/ etc counted as "clusters" ... see the shift a few lines below).  Thus for example /tipwu/ > /tupwu/. However, other labialized consonants such /tʷ/ did not trigger the shift, so /titwu/ stays /titwu/.
#:NOTE ON POLITICS ... THIS IS WHERE TUPPY BREAKS OFF (year 6843)
#<b>ŋ</b> was denasalized to <em>g</em> in all positions.
#The palatalized velar consonants <b>kʲ gʲ</b> became the postalveolar affricates <em>tš dž</em>. In some dialects, including the one that produced Pabappa, this shift did not occur before back vowels, and instead the consonants were reduced to plain velars.
#The labialized velar consonants <b>kʷ gʷ pʷ bʷ</b> were decomposed to the clusters <em>kw gw pw bw</em>. 
#Final <b>y</b> in trisyllabic words disappeared.  Due to analogy, it disappeared in some shorter words as well.  However, consos that now occurred at the end of a word because of the dropped y became labialized, though this is not shown in Romanizaiton.  Thus minimal pairs such as <em>nap</em> "pyramid" versus <em>napʷ</em> "arrow" did exist.
#The remaining palatalized consonants became labiodental fricatives: <b>fʲ</b> and <b>tʲ</b> merged as <em>f</em>, and <b>nʲ</b> changed to <em>v</em>.  <b>pf</b> became <em>ff</em>.
#Medial vowels in trisyllabic words disappeared if the resulting consonant cluster was acceppable ("the Debra shift"). wr&gt;rw.  Here again, labialization hung around if the deleted vowel was o u or y.  Thus there were minimal pairs such as <em>puppa</em> "salamander" vs <em>pupʷpa</em> "kind, humanitarian".  This period (around the year 7300) is the beginning of what is often considered "Classical Poswa" (<i>Poswa Maswumbies</i>).
#:Poswobs invaded Pabappa territory beginning around 7414, and the language was essentially unchanged at that time.
#<b>š</b> before a nasal changed to <em>ž</em> and the nasal changed into a voiced stop.  At this time, the new sound <em>d</em> was spelled with the letter <b><font color="#000000">v</font></b>.
#Clusters in which the two elements were at different PoA's AND different classes resolved in the following ways:  <br> &nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp; pn bn &gt; vž
#šn sn &gt; žv
#tm km &gt; vb
#fm vm &gt; vb
#šm žm sm šb sb &gt; žb
#tp &gt; pp
#pm tn &gt; mm nn
#The clusters <b>pk</b> and <b>bg</b> became <em>pw</em> and <em>bi</em> respectively.  <b>mk</b> became <em>mw</em> (not mpw).&nbsp;
#:And likewise for other consonants.
#The voiced velar stop <b>g</b> was fronted to <em>dž</em> unless it occurred in a cluster after another consonant and before <b><font color="#000000">a o u</font></b>. 
#The clusters <b>šb</b> and <b>bš</b> were devoiced to <em>šp</em> and <em>pš</em> respectively.
#The clusters <b>žp</b> and <b>pž</b> became <em>žb</em> and <em>bž</em> respectively.
#The clusters <b>pl</b> and <b>bl</b> became <em>p</em> and <em>b</em> respectively when overlaying two unaccented syllables.  The same also happened for other stop + nonstop clusters such as <b>pr br pš ps bž pt pf bv</b>, althoiugh pš, pf, and ps survived as aspirates pʰ for long enough to survive a particular subsequent shift.  Clusters like mž and mdž &gt; mbž &gt; mb. 
#The sequences <b>yw ww wy</b> changed to <em>ʷu</em> in all positions (these three were all seen as the same thing anyway, w &amp; y are just Romanization.)
#wi+vowel &gt; i+vowel (wawia "sadism" &gt; waia but still spelled wawia).  Likewise, the recently created ʷu &gt; u before vowels.  Neither of these changes occurred in absolute initial position, however; <em>wialeb</em> "also" remained wialeb.
#iy &gt; ia (<em>platia</em> "record"); ii &gt; ie (<em>plopie</em> "grass").
#In wholly unstressed syllables, except when preceded by <em><font color="#000000">a</font></em>, the clusters <b>rl</b> and <b>lr</b> changed to <em>al</em> and <em>ar</em> respectively, and raised the preceding vowel. 
#The cluster <b>rgw</b> (occurring only in "wirgwep" and similar words) became <em>vbw</em>, with both consonants labialized.
#A velar-onset syllable preceded by another changed to alveolar if the vowel was a back vowel, but postalveolar if it was a front vowel. The affected consonants were <b>k g kw gw r</b>. 
#The inherited vowel <b>y</b> had lowered and weakened to a schwa in most positions.  Now the diphthong <b>al</b> (but not alʷ) went to <em>ae</em>  and <b>yl</b> to <em>e</em>.  <em>ae</em> was pronounced in some dialects as <em>ai</em> or <em>a</em>, with the <em>a</em> pronunciation winning out in all but a few words.
#In some dialects, the labiodental fricatives <b>f v</b> came to be pronounced as the dentals <em>ṣ ẓ</em> in all positions, but there was no change in the spelling.
#Geminate stops overlaying two unaccented syllables reduced to single if there was another geminate earlier in the word.  This affected only compounds such as "talap bavva", which spawned a new variant <em>talabbava</em> and then created the new word "bava", and <b>pam mepoppos</b> became <em>pam mepopos</em>.
#<b>pb</b> --&gt; <em>p</em> sporadically, especially overlaying two unaccented syllables.  Likewise also šb &gt; š, etc.
#<b>mp</b> --&gt; <em>mb</em> after a voiceless conso (e.g. pampa&gt;pamba), except if another voiceless conso followed 
#Initial <b>ps pš</b> --&gt; ph --&gt; <em>p</em>, also sometimes if overlaying two unaccented silabs just as pl bl had.  (Thus pampsa &gt; pampa, not pamba).


<br><br>ž&gt;j problems??
These are spelled inconsistently however, and there may have also been a fourth pair of long vowels for primordial /ai au/.
   
   
#Final short ''ɜ ɨ'' shifted to '''ɨ Ø''' unconditionally.  Here, "final" refers to polysyllabic words only.
#:It is possible that at this time, a plural '''-ɨw''' (later /u/) became generalized to all consonant stems, from original ɨ-stems.  It is not clear what the original plural would have been from the pre-existing consonant stems.
#In at least  some combinations,  word-internal ''ɨ'' also disappeared to '''Ø''', thus allowing the later creation of word-initial /ḳ/ and, through contraction of ''-ɨB-'', a source of new labialized consonants.  Alternatively, this shift could be united with the shift below in which /ɜy ɨy ɜw ɨw/ become /i i u u/.
#:This created new clusters, which in some cases collapsed to new single consonants. All were rare:
##''bh'' shifted to '''p'''.  (If ''B'' represents more than one phoneme, it will need to be separated from the true /b/ by this shift.)
#''mB'' > '''mb'''.
#Word-final clusters became simple, losing their second element.
#:Note that this implies there will be little or no final /ə/ in Tapilula.
#Probably all ''$G'' > '''$'''.
#Intervocalically, the geminate nasals ''  mm nn ňň ŋŋ  '' shifted to  the prenasalized voiced stops '''mb nd ňǯ ŋġ '''.
#An ''h'' or ''hʷ'' after a nasal also switched to a voiced stop, thus creating a prenasalized voiced (not voiceless) consonant. 
#The cluster ''mB'' now also shifted to '''mb'''. 
#:This is important much later on, as it allows epenthetic /b/ for vowel-initial words when padded by a 1st person marker (later extended to places where it would not be etymologically sound). 
#:This also means that /mB/ and /mh/ have identical reflexes and that analogy may have begun very early.  Also, note that /mbʲ/ later just becomes /mb/, so even the new /h/ that arises later on can sometimes be treated as B since this /h/ can come from /gi/ before a vowel.  Importantly, the 1P>2P marker in Tapilula is /nambə/, just as if it were from /nam/ + /gə/ instead of /nam/ + /hə/.  Essentially, the passive morpheme vanishes.
#The sequences ''pm tn čň kŋ'' shifted to '''mm nn ňň ŋŋ'''. 
#A prenasalized voiced stop following a closed syllable became denasalized; any such preceding coda became voiced.  This was allophonic, so that for example ''[bb]'' was still underlyingly /pmb/.  Or, maybe they were devoiced.
#Word-initial approximant  ''w  '' became  '''hʷ  '''. 
#:NOTE ON POLITICS: This is 3770 BC, a point of no political significance, but where the language suddenly began changing very rapidly instead of very slowly.
#Before a vowel, the sequences ''ɜy ɨy ɜw ɨw'' changed into '''i i u u'''.
#:NOTE, another way of saying this (more in line with how i write now) is that /ē ī/ merged into a new short /i/ before hiatus; the /u/ shift may have been separate.
#:the resulting sequences are treated as new coarticulates... also, the high vowel probably disappears before some consonants, creating clusters such as /kl/, and these can contrast labialization.
#The vowel ''ɨ'' became '''u''' before a labial in a closed syllable, or after a labialized consonant. That is, ''ʷɨ'' > '''ʷu'''.
#The vowels ''a ɜ'' before a labial in a closed syllable very likely became shifted to '''o o''' around this time, because it cannot be accounted for by the later shift that does almost the same thing. By contrast,  /ʷa ʷɜ/ did NOT shift to /o/.
#The vowel ''ɨ'' became '''i''' when adjacent of a palatal in either direction (though in compounds, this did not always apply progressively).
#Possibly at this time ''kʷ'' shifted to '''p'''.
#Remaining ''B'' shifted to '''Ø''' (not /g/).
#The labialized consonants '' w  mbʷ  kʷ hʷ ŋġʷ    ''  delabialized to ''' Ø    mb k h ŋġ  ''' before /i/.
#:try saying püa etc and remmeber syllable integrity ... earlier, there was a rule a bit below this one that would have these going to /w/, essentially deleting the /i/. 
#The clusters ''  tl  tr'' shifted to '''  kl  kr'''.  Other similar shifts of clusters almost certainly followed. The cluster ''kt'' shifted to '''k''' (not /ḳ/).   
#Before a vowel, the sequences ''ti ki'' shifted to '''č'''.
#The clusters ''kl kr'' merged as '''kʕ'''.  Thus, in handheld object prefixes, /ətr/ > /oḳ/.
#:The Play word /see/ requires that ''ml'' also shift to '''mʕ''' (and later to /mb/).  If this shift does not happen, the Play word would end up as /seu/ instead.
#Syllabic nasals were created: ''im ɨm um'' > '''ṁ''', ''ɨn'' > '''ṅ''', and ''ɨŋ'' > '''ŋ̇'''.
#:NOTE THAT THIS REQUIRES THERE TO BE A SECOND SOURCE OF /um/, perhaps implying that coda /ŋʷ/ still existed at this point.
#:Possibly the sequences ''mbṁ ndṅ ŋġŋ̇'' and others like them shift to simple syllabics '''ṁ ṅ ŋ̇'''.
#The palatal approximant ''y'' was changed to '''ʕ''' in all positions.  Between vowels, this became silent ('''Ø''') but used to show diphthongs' separation.  Thus the palatalized consonants ''pʲ mbʲ mʲ '' became the pharyngealized  clusters '''pʕ mbʕ mʕ '''.     
#The postalveolar nasal ''ň''  changed to '''ŋ'''.  However, ''ndʲ'' here became '''nd''', not something such as /nġ/.
#The voiced approximants ''λ ř  r'' changed to '''y'''.
#:It is possible that there was a shift of /iy/ > /ig/, which would account for the name of the religion beginning with ''yìga-'' in Tapilula.  Alternatively this could be due to some sort of syllable split, such that /yiya/ came to be seen as /yii/ + /a/, and all double vowels collapsed. The /g/ would be a regular insertion to break up hiatus in such a case.
#The vowel sequences ''aɨ ɨa'' changed to '''ɜu ʷɜ'''.  Note that the /ɨa/ covers all such sequences that did not previously have an intervening /y/ sound ... therefore, this was probably allophonically a back vowel even if it were not always rounded.
#The sequences ''ʷa ʷɜ'' changed to '''ʷo'''. 
#:It needs to be  that /kʷ/ > /pʷ/ > /p/ had taken place by this time, since otherwise there would be no contrast of /p/ vs /pʷ/.
#:This also implies ''pk'' (since it was really kʷk) shifting to '''pp'''.  This does not rule out a new /pk/ arising from new compounds.
#''yʷ čʷ'' > '''y č''', thus the word for hand is just "yò". This did not affect any ''$'' that was just /s/ by this time.
#''n$ʷ'' (when not /nčʷ/ as above) shifted to '''ntʷ''' (not to /ndʷ/). Likewise the sequence ''m$'' shifted to '''mp'''.
#The sequences ''au ɜu'' now both become '''o'''. ''ai ɜi'' changed to '''ɛ''' unconditionally. 
#The true mid vowel ''ɜ'', in a syllable adjacent to an /ɛ/ or /o/, changed to match that vowel.  Some double schwa words also changed, e.g. ''mɜčɜ'' > '''močo''' "fire". 
#The "clear" labialized consonants ''  kʷ ŋʷ '' became the rounded bilabials '''  pʷ mʷ'''.  (There was little or no /kʕʷ/.)
#The pharyngealized nasals ''mʕ mʕʷ'' shifted to '''mb mbʷ'''.
#:This may incorporate early shifts like /mh/ at morpheme boundaries.  Thus for example, hàga "fairy" could actually be /hàmba/.
#The voiceless fricatives  ''h hʷ'' became voiced to '''  g w''' unconditionally.
#Remaining ''ɜ ɨ'' in open syllables shift to '''ə i''', respectively.  This new schwa vowel is IPA /ɨ/but  is spelled "ə" or "y", since it is never ambiguous with IPA /j/.
#The voiceless affricate ''č'' shifted to '''h'''. (This probably had shifted to /s/ during depalatalization. if not,  /hy/ etc also switched.)
#:Gold /hìga ~ hìa/ showing up as /hà/, even if only in one word, relies on this assumption, and there are no words that rely on the sequence /hia/ existing in pre-Gold as it has no  specific reflexes.
#Syllables *preceding* heavy syllables became '''LOW ''' tone.


<br><br><br>
Thus the final phonology was:
Thus standard Ižda Mir, if labialized consonants are included as phonemes, had the consonants
<em>w p pʷ b bʷ m mʷ f fʷ v vʷ t tʷ n nʷ s sʷ l lʷ r rʷ š šʷ ž žʷ k kʷ g gʷ</em>
and the vowels <em>a e i o u y</em>, the last of which was a schwa. 
At the beginning of a syllable, all of the consonants as well as the clusters <em>pl pr bl br tš dž</em>
were allowed. At the end of a syllable, all consonants were allowed,
but were restricted based on the consonant that followed. At the end of
a word, <em>p m s l b pʷ bʷ mʷ fʷ vʷ tʷ nʷ sʷ lʷ rʷ šʷ žʷ kʷ gʷ</em> were allowed.


<br><br>
Rounded bilabials:    pʷ      mʷ  mbʷ mhʷ~hʷ  w
H </pd></pr></pbody></pable>
Spread bilabials:      p  pʕ  m  mb  mf ~f  (Ø)       
Alveolars:            t      n  nd          l
Rounded alveolars:    tʷ      nʷ  ndʷ   
  Velars:                k  ḳ  ŋ  ŋġ      h  g


<pable><pbody><pr><pd width="115"></pd><pd>
The (f) was pronounced [mʰ] in some dialects. Pharyngealization consonants can be spelled with voiced stop letters.
<!-- pinna top-->


===Bābākiam to Old Pabappa (~6500) ... too early?===
====Dreamlandic changes====
Alternate names: Pespimbesa
This change occurred in the [[Lenian languages]] but is not on that page because the dictionary is not synced properly with the MRCA.
<p class="body">
#The nasals ''mʷ m  n nʷ ŋ'' become prenasalized voiceless stops '''mpʷ mp  nt ntʷ ŋk''' when facing a labial.


</p><ol>
Proto-Dreamlandic retains the final primordial nasals even before voiceless consonants,  and also gains new voiceless prenasal clusters from plain nasals before labials.  As of 07:07, 1 May 2022 (PDT)~ this error is fixed  in the dictionary for  word-medial clusters (that is, some /p t k/ has been replaced with /mp nt ŋk/), but not for word-final codas.  This is because the codas are not indexed in the MRCA dictionary either; they are only derived when a word needs to appear in Dreamlandic.
<li>&nbsp; Debra shift I. (But rV and wV didnt compress)


("ae" and "al" remained distinct here).&nbsp; If the deleted  
====MRCA to Nuclear Tapilula (700 BC to 0 AD)====
vowel was y, o or u, the preceding consonant became a labial. If a consonant was already labialized or
#The  nasals ''m mʷ'' became '''f hʷ''' before a vowel facing another labial.
palatalizzed, it did not contract. l r &gt; u (later to disappear) </li>
#Possibly, ''g'' shifted to '''Ø''' at the head of a closed syllable,  at least when in initial position. This could explain the otherwise unpreditcable g~Ø variation in Gold and Andanese, but not why they sometimes disagree with each other, or why it also drops out in medial position in a few words (unless these are all compounds). Therefore, even if this rule is assumed, analogy is required.  In Dreamlandic, the reflexes of /g/ and /Ø/ are identical, so no explanation is needed.
<li>&nbsp;  <b></b> --&gt; <em>f</em>
#All syllable codas were deleted. The resulting ephemeral voiced stop ''ġ'' shifted to the fricative '''g'''.
#:The most likely order for the sound change above was:
#:#The final stops ''p t k'' shifted to '''ʔ''', which carries the stress and behaves as a '''HIGH''' tone.
#:#The final nasals ''m n ŋ'' merged as '''n''' and word-internal clusters became homorganic.
#:#The lateral ''l'' disappeared in the coda as well, bringing a high tone.
#:#The final nasal ''n'' disappeared, bringing a high tone.
#:Under this analysis, Dreamlandic would do all shifts but the last. It is possible that nasal POA was preserved in early Dreamlandic, however.
#The glottalized stops <b>pʕ pʕʷ  kʕ</b> changed to <em>b bʷ  ḳ</em> unconditionally. 
#The rounded labials ''pʷ bʷ mʷ  '' became plain labials '''p b m  '''. 
 
The consonant inventory was:


</li>
Rounded bilabials:                    hʷ  w
Spread bilabials:      p      m  b  f  (Ø)
Alveolars:            t      n  d      l
Rounded alveolars:    tʷ      nʷ  dʷ       
Velars:                k  ḳ  ŋ      h  g


<li>&nbsp; <b>ŋ</b> was denasalized to <em>g</em> in all positions, except
==Post-Tapilula changes==
certain clusters such as ŋr.</li>
<li>&nbsp;  <b>tš dž</b> (mostly in compounds because the normal k-&gt;tš shift didnt
happen here) --&gt; <em>kʲ gʲ</em>


</li>
===Tapilula to Gold (year 1900)===
 
Alternate names: Medium, Walking Girls, Wolf in Wool, Soft Hands, Slingshot, Broken Shields, Ukieipi, G̣ʷidiʕìləs
<li>&nbsp;  Diphthongs of all types were resolved in favor of the first vowel. (including
things like  <b>nʲa</b> and <b>tʲa</b> to <em>ni</em> and <em>ti</em>,
and <b>mwa</b> to <em>mu</em>.)  Be careful to remember that tʲ and fʲ did not merge here,
and in fact have different out comes even for the vowels that followed them.  Likewise,
kʷ and all other labialized consonants turned their next vowel into /u/.


Although Gold is the proper name of the language, the Gold party survives for thousands of years, and therefore these names can be used to give a more precise definition of the stage of the language being referred to.
   
   
'''Note on politics: It is not clear whether the Tapilula language split apart in 0 AD or in 500 AD.  If 500, the various Subumpamese languages are almost independent branches since they begin diverging not long after 600 AD.  However, Sub and Gold share much more in common than either of them does with Andanese, despite being supposedly only 100 years closer together. So perhaps the true date is 0 after all.'''


</li><li>&nbspStops occurring before a nasal assimilated fully.
====Tapilula to Gold (proper)====
#The aspirated velar stop ''k'' became '''č''' before the vowel /i/.  If another vowel followed, the /i/ disappeared.  This happened even if the /i/ was accented.
#When a "velaroid" consonant (/''k ḳ ŋ h g l''/) followed an accented high tone vowel, the vowel metathesized, leaving a closed syllable.  Thus, for example, /àli/ > /ail/.  These closed syllables were all high-toned, and are thus written without tone marks. Thus, for example, ''aa'' implies ''àa''.  Later, daughter languages introduced tone contrasts and independent sequences. 
#A schwa before another vowel in any syllable disappeared.  Thus ''əa əe əi əo əu əə'' shifted to '''a e i o u ə'''.  This happened in both open and closed syllables.
#The sequences ''iu'' and ''ui'' shifted to '''ə̄'''.
#The double-vowel sequences ''aa ee ii oo uu əə'' shifted to the single vowels '''a e i o u ə''' in closed syllables only. 
#The sequences ''ii uu əə'' (which now occurred only in open syllables) shifted to '''əi əu ə'''. This means that the /ə/ and "same vowel" infixes had switched places in some constructions, but this did not lead to confusion, at least in the four-vowel languages  such as Play, because the infixes typically did not occur in their free forms.
#The sequences ''ie uo'' shifted to '''i u''' in open syllables only.
#The sequences ''ai ei oi'' merged as '''ei'''; the sequences ''au eu ou'' merged as '''ou'''. 
#:'''NOTE ON POLITICS: SUBUMPAMESE CAN BE CONSIDERED TO BREAK OFF HERE, AS THIS IS THE LAST OF THE SHARED CHANGES. ''' (that is, there were only A FEW of them).
#The sequences ''ea eə'' shifted to '''ee'''; meanwhile, ''oa oə'' became '''oo'''. Then, ''aə'' shifted to '''aa'''. Thus, the sequences /ee aa oo/ once again appeared in both open and closed syllables. Note, however, that much inherited /ea oa/ had participated in grammatical alternations with /əa/, which had become a simple /a/ by this time, and this is the form that was usually generalized.
#The sequences ''  ia ie io iə'' shifted to '''  ī '''. Then ''ua ue uo uə'' shifted to '''ū'''.
#In absolute final position, syllable-final ''ŋ'' changed to '''n'''.  (But see below.) 
#Accented vowel-initial syllables gained a pharyngeal  '''ʕ''' as an onset. Then the clusters ''nʕ kʕ'' shifted to '''g ḳ'''.
#After long vowels, all consonants became voiced.  Also, consonants occurring after initial vowels also became voiced.  This created the new consonants '''  v      ǯ''' .  Thus, final ''-h'' in words like hʷīh became '''-g'''.  However, analogy made it so that the change was confined to open syllables in most words.  This sound change did not affect diphthongs.  There was no voiced velar stop, as all four velars simply shifted to fricatives. 
#After initial unstressed /u/, all consonants other than palatals became labialized.    This change extended even to clusters.  Because of the voicing rule, however, all of these consonants were voiced.  '' bʷ vʷ'' merged as '''w'''.
#In word-initial position, the six vowels ''a e i o u ə''  were deleted to '''Ø''' unconditionally. 
#:A few words began with double vowels, but these were grammatical alternants of V-/g/-V, V: (that is, inherited long vowels), and /g/-V-(g)-V pairlets, meaning that in these few words, the vowel deletion could affect both vowels, only the outermost vowel, or neither  of the two vowels.  Then, because of this same analogy, the words that now began with ''g'' came to be pronounced with '''Ø''' onset when following a word that ended with a consonant, and for some speakers also in isolation, but never when following a word ending in a vowel.  In both Leaper and Play, this shift was quickly undone, as it had never been phonemic.  In [[Thaoa]], it may have become firm, but with the expense of now having no words beginning in /g/.
#:Sometimes root-initial vowels were retained due to classifier prefixes; here, too, analogy played an important role. For example, the food item prefix ''mi-'' cleaved onto the root, and ceased to be seen as a classifier prefix, meaning that these new roots began with consonants after all. This was helped by the fact that only a few such roots began with vowels, even including those that had come to be pronounced with /g/.
#All schwas and diphthongs became low tone.
#Labialized consonants lost their labialization when occuring after another labial or labialized consonant.
#After a stressed syllable, intervocalic ''ʕ ʕʷ'' became '''g gʷ'''.  This is due to reanalysis, not a true sound change.
#The glottal fricatives ''h hʷ'' became velar; there was no spelling change.


</li><li>&nbsp;  The cluster <b>žž</b> simplified to <em>j</em>.
</li><li>&nbsp;  Clusters of two fricatives of differing points of articulation were assimilated in favor of the second consonant.
</li><li>&nbsp;  The cluster <b>žbž</b> simplified to <em>žž</em>.  Other clusters, such as <b>sps</b> (in ''pospsar'' "urine"), simplified similarly.
</li><li>&nbsp;  Clusters of fricatives and stops of dissimilar voicing were resolved in favor of the second consonant.  For example <b>žp</b> became <em>šp</em>.
</li><li>&nbsp; The clusters <b>gv kf</b> became <em>bv pf</em>. (Thus Paleo-Pabappa <i>kagyvi</i> "poison" became Proto-Pabappa <i>kabvi</i>.)
</li><li>&nbsp;  The fricative <b>š</b> changed to <em>h</em> in initial position and <em>s</em> elsewhere. '''NOTE: currently the wordlist assumes that initial pš- > /h/ as well, even though the sound change of /ps/ > /s/ is unrelated to this one and occurs much later. One or the other of these will need to be fixed.'''
</li><li>&nbsp;  The fricative <b>ž</b> came to be pronounced <em>z</em>. 
</li><li>&nbsp;  The cluster <b>rr</b> was reduced to <em>r</em>.&nbsp; This is
considered to be the stage of proto-Pabappa.</li></ol>
<br>
<p></p></pd></pr></pbody></pable>
<pable><pbody><pr><pd width="165"></pd><pd>
<link rel="stylesheet" type="text/css" href="./pabappa28_files/europe.htm">
====Old Pabappa (6500?) to Pabappa (8700)====
<p class="body">
<br>
Pabappa evolved from a dialect of Bloppabop that lacked the vowel m in place of the former and <em>k g</em> in
place of the latter in all words.  However, these sounds were pronounced as <em>/tš dž/</em> before all front vowels,
including <em>/a/</em>.  Thus the proto-Pabappa dialect of Bloppabop had the consonants
<em>p b m w f v t n s l r h z k g</em> and the vowels <em>a e i o u y</em>, the last of which was a schwa. 
At the beginning of a syllable, all of the consonants as well as the clusters <em>pl pr pw ps pš bl br bw kw gw</em>
were allowed. At the end of a syllable, all consonants were allowed,
but were restricted based on the consonant that followed. At the end of
a word, <em>p m s l r f v z b</em> were allowed. Then the following changes occurred:
</p><ol>
<li>&nbsp;  Double nasals were reduced to singles.
</li><li>&nbsp;  The vowel <b>ə</b> either disappeared or became <em>i</em> or <em>o</em> (governed by the
surrounding vowels' HEIGHT (not backness)).  If it disappeared, it also labialized the new final consonant.
</li><li>&nbsp;  The fricative <b>z</b> disappeared in all positions, except in a few words where it remained as <em>d</em>.  Meanwhile the fricative <b>v</b> changed to <em>d</em>
in all positions, except in a few words where it disappeared. These
irregularities are explained by an early irregular stage of the sound
change where <b>z</b> and <b>v</b> changed places in a few words.
</li><li>&nbsp;
Unstressed cliticized prefixes were dropped from the language or
phonologically incorporated into the following word by dropping the
vowel, even when this created a previously forbidden consonant cluster.
</li><li>&nbsp;  The velar stops <b>k g</b> changed to the fricatives <em>š ž</em> in all positions.  Before back vowels, they were usually pronounced as velar fricatives.
</li>
<li>&nbsp;  The labialized consonants <b>sʷ šʷ</b> both became <em>f</em> (only to change to s later).
</li>
<li>&nbsp;  The cluster <b>sš</b> (usually from /sk/) became <em>šš</em>.
</li>
<li>&nbsp;  The fricative <b>f</b> changed to <em>p</em> in initial position and <em>s</em> elsewhere.  This stage, reached around 7000 (?), is considered to be the classical stage of Old Pabappa, also called Pespimbesa.
</li><li>&nbsp;  The labiovelar stops <b>kw</b> and <b>gw</b> changed to the bilabial stops <em>p</em> and <em>b</em> in all positions.
  fw -&gt; f, sw -&gt; w, tw -&gt; pw, vw -&gt; w, žw -&gt; w.
</li><li>&nbsp;  The single bilabial stop <b>p</b> became the geminate <em>pp</em> when following a voiceless consonant plus a vowel.
</li><li>&nbsp;  The postalveolar fricatives <b>ž</b> and <b>š</b> became the labiodental fricatives <em>v</em> and <em>f</em> in all positions.
</li><li>&nbsp;  Unstressed initial <b>pu-</b> changed to <em>purp-</em> before a vowel, where <em>-rp-</em> is a hypercorrection of the epenthetic <b><font color="#000000">r</font></b> that had appeared near the earliest stages of the language.  <b>pu-p-</b> and <b>pu-w-</b> became <em>w-</em>.  <b>pu-b-</b> became <em>pu-br-</em>.  <b>pu-t-</b> became <em>pu-d-</em>.
</li><li>&nbsp;  In initial position, the clusters <b>sp st ps</b> coalesced as <em>s</em>.    <b>sl</b> survived, but only in one native word; the others were all either loans or proper names.
</li><li>&nbsp;  The voiced stops <b>b</b> and <b>d</b> became the voiceless stops <em>p</em> and <em>t</em> in all positions.  Fricatives did not devoice, but clusters such as <b>bv</b> became <em>pf</em>.
</li><li>&nbsp;  Non-labial final consonants were replaced with labials in most words
(except -l and -r). s-&gt;f-&gt;0.  Final <b>b</b> became <em>p</em>.
</li><li>&nbsp;  Final <b>e</b> disappeared, even after consonant
clusters, except in monosyllabic words and some suffixes.  This created
new consonant clusters in agglutinative forms of words and in
compounds. Sporadically, monosyllabic words that had ended in <b>e</b> got a new <em>-s</em> or <em>-ssi</em> suffix which
caused the original <b>e</b> to be retained; this happened to other monosyllabic vowel-final words too.
</li><li>&nbsp;  Before a nasal, <b>p s t n</b> assimilated completely.
</li><li>&nbsp;  Clusters of a nonlabial stop followed by a labial stop were resolved in favor of the nonlabial one.
</li><li>&nbsp;  Final <b>o</b> was were lowered to <em>a</em> except if the
accented vowel was mid-height (e or o).  Final e (in the case that it survived
the previous sound change) was raised to i in the
same environment.&nbsp; This is considered to be the classical stage of Middle Pabappa.
<br>&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;&nbsp;Thus the phonology of Middle Pabappa was /p m w f v t n s l r h/ for consonants and /a e i o u/ for vowels.
</li><li>&nbsp;  The sequence <b>aw</b> changed to <em>o</em> before any nonrounded vowel.  <b>ow</b> and <b>uw</b> became <em>o</em> and <em>u</em>.
</li><li>&nbsp;  The clusters <b>tl</b> and <b>ttl</b> both changed to <em>ll</em>. (was rpl) 
</li><li>&nbsp;  The sequence <b>sr</b> became <em>spr</em>. <b>lr</b> became <em>rr</em>.
Any other nonlabial consonant before <b>r</b> became labial.  Then <b>mr</b> changed to <em>mpr</em>.
</li><li>&nbsp; Clusters of a nasal followed by any other consonant of
differing point of articulation were assimilated in favor of the point
of articulation of the second consonant.
</li><li>&nbsp;  <b>ml</b> became <em>mpl</em>.
(7414 war occurs here?)</li><li>&nbsp;  The voiceless labiodental fricative <b>f</b> changed to <em>w</em> in initial position.
</li><li>&nbsp;  <b>h</b> changed to <em>0</em> in all positions.&nbsp; (maybe h
&gt; f &gt; w internally before u?)</li><li>&nbsp;    Initial <b>pw</b> became <em>w</em>.
</li><li>&nbsp;    Unstressed syllables of the form CVCC where the two latter C's were a geminate or one of a few other types of consonants changed the vowel to a very short schwa <em>ə</em>.
</li><li>&nbsp;  Intervocalic voiceless stops became voiced.  (Possibly omit this shift if a geminate voiceless is nearby.) The cluster <b>pl</b> became <em>bl</em> in all positions;
<b>pr</b> became <em>b</em> in word-initial position, and <em>br</em> elsewhere.  (maybe just -b- everywhere since it would be the only
b in the language after clusters and thus not
contrast with anything.)
</li><li>&nbsp;  The geminate stops <b>pp</b> and <b>tt</b> became <em>p</em> and <em>t</em> in all positions.  Meanwhile <b>ss</b> became <em>s</em>.  (Possibly omit this shift if another geminate is changing)
</li><li>&nbsp;  The cluster <b>sp</b> became <em>ss</em>.  Most suffixes that had formerly begun with <b>p</b> were now considered to begin with <em>b</em>, but this <em>b</em> was realized as <em>b</em> only after a vowel or a bilabial consonant; elsewhere the consonant disappeared or, in the case of <em>t</em> and <em>d</em>, doubled the preceding consonant.  After <em>p</em>, which would normally be omitted, the suffix came to begin with <em>p</em>.  (ANDANESE PABAPPA SPLITS OFF HERE)
</li><li>&nbsp;  Initial <b>v</b> became <em>f</em>.
</li><li>&nbsp;  The labiodental fricatives <b>f</b> and <b>v</b> became the bilabial stops <em>p</em> and <em>b</em> in all positions.
</li><li>&nbsp;  The marginal schwa phoneme disappeared, creating some new clusters and geminates. (Debra shift II.) Thus words like <b>popa</b> "oak tree/rabbit" (a homophone pair) became <em>-ppa</em> when they were the second element of a compound. If the new cluster was "impossible", it retained a vowel (either /i/ or /u/) instead. However, outside of place names and personal names, these new affixes did not actually survive very often, as they were a unique word form in the language, and in most cases the new words were restored to compounds (since they had been almost all compounds anyway).
</li><li>&nbsp;  The bilabial stop <b>b</b>, between two identical unaccented vowels (e.g. -aba, -obo), turned to <em>m</em> sporadically as the result of analogy from various noun declensions.
</li><li>&nbsp;  The clusters <b>tr dr</b>, which only occurred in loans and a few compounds, became <em>k g</em>.
</li></ol>
Thus the final phonology of Pabappa was /<em>p b m w t d n l s r</em>/ for consonants and /<em>a e i o u</em>/ for vowels.  The phonemes /k/ and /h/ were common in borrowings, but many speakers pronounced these as /ʔ/ or /0/.  A few speakers, however, did pronounce [k] and [h], because [k] appeared as an allophone of /t/ before /r/ (even though this sequence itself was also found only in loans), and [h] appeared as an allophone of /r/ in initial position.  In the so-called "Post-Pabappa" language, when the speakers of Pabappa were crushed by invading armies into living along the south coast, true phonemic /k/ and /x~h/ became more common.
<br><br>Western (Andanese) dialect: ar er ir or ur &gt; au eu iu oa ua; al el il ol ul &gt; al el il oi ui.  (i.e. clear L survives, dark L dies.)  v f &gt; b p (without the change of v &gt; f &gt; P initially; thus pipi "wall" is bipi).
<div align="bottom">
</div></pd></pr></pbody></pable>
<pable><pbody><pr><pd width="220"></pd><pd>
===Proto-Moonshine to Late Moonshine (6800)===
Alternat enames: '''Classical Moonshine'''
   
<ol>
<li>&nbsp; Clusters of any consonant plus a nasal simplified to single consonants: <b>n</b> disappeared and lengthened the previous vowel; <b>s</b> made the nasal voiceless, disappeared and lengthened the previous vowel; <b>ʔ</b> disappeared and raised the tone of the previous vowel.   
</li><li>&nbsp; Syllable-final <b>ʔ n s</b> were grammatically analogized to the consonants <b>k ŋ h</b> between two of the same vowel, which then became <em>ʔ n s</em> and deleted the final vowel.
</li><li>&nbsp; A schwa following another vowel disappeared and made that vowel a long vowel.
</li><li>&nbsp; Unaccented short schwas were lost. Because the language
had a very active compounding system, this shift led to a steep
increase in the number of types of allowable consonant clusters, as
well as new consonants allowed in final position. </li><li>&nbsp; Syllable-final <b>s</b> after a vowel disappeared and made the preceding consonant voiceless and aspirated.
</li><li>&nbsp; Syllable-final <b>s</b> after a consonant disappeared and made that consonant into an alveolar. 
</li><li>&nbsp; Unaccented <b>e</b> and <b>o</b> became <em>a</em>, often spelled as schwa or as ʕ, the vowel separator.  If high tone, this was replaced by ʔ, the glottal stop.  However, in neither case was this sound actually pronounced; it merely affected surrounding consonants for a short period of time after the shift.
</li><li>&nbsp; Unaccented <b>i</b> and <b>u</b> came to spelled as
palatalized/labialized consonants followed by a schwa, which had merged
in with these. Thus the old glyphs for coarticulated consonants were
revived, and stress was no longer fixed on the first syllable of the
word even when the first vowel in the word wasn't a schwa. There was
now only one orthographic unaccented vowel in the language: the schwa,
which was now often unwritten. Unaccented <em>i</em> and <em>u</em> were written as part of the preceding consonant (the syllable was always open).
Consonant clusters simplified according to the following rules:
</li><li>&nbsp; Labialized consonants (<b>kʷ ŋʷ hʷ</b>) in final position (or at the beginning of a cluster) became plain and added a <em>w</em> glide to the preceding vowel.
</li><li>&nbsp; Palatalized consonants (<b>pʲ mʲ tʲ nʲ sʲ č ñ š ž</b>) in final position or at the beginning of a cluster became plain and added a <em>j</em> glide to the preceding vowel.


'''NOTE ON POLITICS''': ''THIS IS WHERE THAOA & TARYTE SPLIT  OFF (YR 1085 AD)''


The consonant inventory was:


                        BASIC                        LABIALIZED


 
Bilabials:            p  b  m  f  v                    mʷ      w 
Alveolars:            t  d  n      l            tʷ  dʷ  nʷ           
Postalveolars:        č  ǯ          y                     
Velars:                k      ŋ  h  g  ḳ                ŋʷ  hʷ  gʷ


</li><li>&nbsp; Doubled consonants simplified to singles and caused the tone of the preceding vowel to become high.
The vowel inventory was


Short vowels:          a  e  i  o  u  ə
Long vowels:          ā  ē  ī  ō  ū
Falling diphthongs:      ae ei ao ou
                            əi    əu


</li><li>&nbsp; Any consonant before a nasal disappeared and lengthened
The long vowels /ā ē ō/ can be spelled '''aa ee oo''', but the high vowels /ī ū/ are usually not, because /i u/ before another vowel would indicate a glide.
the preceding vowel. If the sound had been voiceless, it caused the
tone of the preceding vowel to become high. If it had been voiced, it
caused the tone of the preceding vowel to become low. </li><li>&nbsp; Any remaining <b>ʷ</b> trapped between consonants became <em>u</em>.
</li><li>&nbsp; Any remaining <b>ʲ</b> trapped between consonants became <em>i</em>.


</li><li>&nbsp; <b>φ</b> and <b>β</b> changed to <em>w</em>.
There were also ''eo oe'' in historical compounds.  These would not have occurred in individual roots because of ancient vowel harmony rules.


</li><li>&nbsp;  Aspirated consonants became voiceless.
====Nuclear Gold====
Gold's unconditional shift of /f v b/ > /s d d/ sets it apart from all related languages. Note that it requires /mb/ > /nd/ as well but yet /ṁb/ (syllabic) remained.


</li><li>&nbsp; In unaccented syllables, all vowels became short.   
#All remaining ''e o'' shifted to '''a'''. This included elements of diphthongs.  Resulting /aa/ was spelled as /ā/; thus, ''ae ao eo oe ee oo'' all shifted to '''ā'''. Thus there were only four vowels.  In some cases, a short vowel was formed instead due to grammatical analogy, though unlike the /ea oa/ > /əa/ shift above, there was no etymological basis for this shift. 
#The velar fricatives ''h g'' were rounded to '''hʷ gʷ''' before /u/.  The labiodental /f/ did not shift, and by this time was likely already [fʲ] to maximize distinction.  If /v/ existed, it did not shift either.
#The plain labials ''f v b'' shifted to '''s d d'''This happened by intermediate palatalization, and therefore the sequences /fj vj bj/, which were very rare, also shifted to /s d d/. The sequence /fʷ/ remained,  often spelled '''ħʷ''', but this quickly merged with the existing /hʷ/.   
#:This apparently includes a shift of ''bh'' to '''dh''', meaning that the devoicing of aspirates did not happen until the Gold language split apart.
#In absolute final position, syllable-final ''h'' changed to '''s'''. 
#The postalveolar  affricates ''č ǯ'' shifted to '''š ž'''.


 
The consonant inventory was:
</li><li>&nbsp; Nonpalatalized alveolar consonants became velarized (not shown in the orthography). 
 
 
 
 
<br>
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp; An old method of deriving verbs from nouns by
truncating the word after the first vowel, and lengthening that vowel
if there was any missing info began to take over now. Although this was
not a true sound change, it affected the general language more than any
of the sound changes on the list. <br>
 
</li><li>&nbsp;  <b>o ò</b> changed to schwa in unaccented position and <em>a</em> in accented position.
 
</li><li>&nbsp;  <b>ō ó</b> changed to <em>o ò</em>.
 
</li><li>&nbsp;  The vowel sequences <b>aj ej ij oj uj</b> became <em>ē i ī i ī</em> respectively.
 
</li><li>&nbsp;  The vowel sequences <b>aw ew iw ow uw</b> became <em>ō u ū u ū</em> respectively.
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp;   
The consonant cluster <b>řp</b> became <em>lp</em> in all positions.
 
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp; Following an accented syllable in a word of three or more syllables, all vowels became schwa.
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp; Following an accented syllable in a word of two syllables or less, the consonant sequences <b>ts ns ss</b> changed to <em>`ts z s</em> respectively. 
 
</li><li>&nbsp;  Following an accented syllable in word-final position, the syllables <b>ka ke ki ko ku</b> became <em>ʔ ʔč ʔč ʔt ʔt</em>.  Before another consonant, they all became <em>ʔ</em>.
 
 
 
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp;  All consonant clusters except those beginning with <b>s</b> became homorganic; the <b>s-</b> clusters did not retain any distinction based on point of articulation but instead shifted the <b>s-</b> to <em>š</em> except before another <em>s</em>.
 
</li><li>&nbsp; The cluster <b>sw</b> (spelled <b>sbʷ</b>) became a bilabial <em>v</em> in all positions. 
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp; <b>sb</b> shifted to <em>žb</em>.
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp; Before front vowels, <b>k g ŋ</b> shifted to <em>č ǯ ñ</em>.
 
 
 
 
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp; Between two unstressed vowels, all labial consonants
except rounded bilabials disappeared unless a string of three vowels
would be created. </li><li>&nbsp;            Before a vowel, unaccented <b>ə</b> changed to <em>u</em>, which then shifted to the labial glide <em>w</em> in syllable-initial position and otherwise created a labialized consonant.  (year
5800; same as 26 in Izda Mir)
 
 
  </li><li>&nbsp;            Before a vowel, unaccented <b>a</b> changed to <em>i</em> ("the karaoke shift"), which then shifted to the palatal glide <em>j</em>
in syllable-initial position and otherwise created a palatalized
consonant. Like the new labialized consonants, palatalized consonants
could occur only before a vowel, but in orthography they could occur
before other consonants because they were used to denote whole
unstressed syllables. However, the only vowel allowed in these
unstressed syllables was the epenthetic schwa, and during the following
sound changes this schwa often disappeared. </li><li>&nbsp;    The labials <b>pʲ bʲ mʲ</b> became the labiodentals <em>ṗ ḅ ṃ</em> in all positions. 
</li><li>&nbsp;    The dentals <b>fʲ vʲ</b> changed to <em>f v</em> before vowels, but to <em>fĭ vĭ</em> elsewhere. 
</li><li>&nbsp;    The alveolars <b>tʲ dʲ sʲ zʲ nʲ</b> became the dentals <em>ṭ ḍ ṣ ẓ ṇ</em> in all positions.  <b>lʲ</b> became <em>j</em> and <b>řʲ</b> became <em>ř</em>.
</li><li>&nbsp;    The dorsals <b>kʲ hʲ rʲ</b> became the palatals <em>č š j</em> in all positions.
 
 
 
 
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp; The labialized postalveolar consonants <b>čʷ ǯʷ šʷ žʷ ñʷ</b> became delabialized.
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp; The palatal consonants <b>č ǯ š ž ñ</b> became <em>c ʒ s z n</em> in all positions. 
 
 
 
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp;    The rounded labials <b>pʷ bʷ mʷ fʷ vʷ</b> became the plain labials <em>pŭ bŭ mŭ fŭ vŭ</em> before a consonant. 
 
</li><li>&nbsp;    The labialized alveolars <b>tʷ dʷ sʷ zʷ nʷ řʷ</b> became plain alveolars <em>t d s z n ř</em> in all positions. 
 
</li><li>&nbsp;  Before a vowel, <b>lʷ</b> became <em>w</em>, but elsewhere it changed to <em>lŭ</em>.
<br> <em> &nbsp;  </em>   
</li><li>&nbsp;    The dorsals <b>kʷ hʷ rʷ</b> became <em>w</em> before a consonant, while also lengthening the preceding vowel.
<br> <em>  </em>   
    
    
   
  Bilabials:            p          m      (ʕ)  w  mʷ           
Alveolars:            t      d  n  s      l  nʷ      tʷ  dʷ
Postalveolars:                        š  ž  y                                 
Velars:                k  ḳ      ŋ  h  g  gʷ  ŋʷ  hʷ  kʷ      ḳʷ


The vowel inventory was


Short vowels:      a  i  u  ə
Long vowels:      ā  ī  ū
Diphthongs:          ai au
                      əi əu


</li><li>&nbsp; The labiodentals <b>ṗ ḅ ṃʰ ṃ</b> and the dentals <b>ṭ ḍ ṇʰ ṇ</b> became <em>c ʒ ns nz</em> in word-final position.
The consonants ''kʷ ḳʷ'' only occur as the realizations of clusters like /k/ + /gʷ/, and thus never occurred word-initially, whereas the labialized nasals occurred ''only'' word-initially (or morpheme-initially).


</li><li>&nbsp; The dentals <b>ṗ ḅ ṃʰ ṃ ṭ ḍ ṇʰ ṇ</b> became the affricates <em>pf bv mf mv tṣ dẓ nṣ nẓ</em>, but there was no change in spelling.
====Notes on Play and Leaper====
Note that both [[babakiam|Play]] and [[leaper language|Leaper]] often show reflexes implying a shift of /hi gi/ > /s d/ between vowels, but in different places; this change was grammatically conditioned and did not occur within the history of the Gold parent language.  This shift was later analogized with the preexisting true sound change of ''f v'' > '''s d''' to replace intervocalic /s si/ of any origin with  /d d/ when the preceding vowel was originally long.  When a vowel did not follow, there was no shift.  The shift also did not occur after a vowel that became long in Leaper but was a diphthong in Gold.


</li><li>&nbsp; The affricates <b>pf bv mf mv tṣ dẓ nṣ nẓ</b> became <em>f v f v ṣ ẓ ṣ ẓ</em> in initial position and after a consonant.
Note that this implies that there was no shift of /sʲ/  to /s/, and therefore likely no shift of /lʲ/ > /l/.


Play's reflex of /ll/ is /ww/ (the document is not on this wiki, unlike almost all others, so I have to put  the notes here).


===Gold (1900) to Khulls (4700)===
Alternate names: Kuroras, Leaper


</li><li>&nbsp; Epenthetic schwas after previously labialized and palatalized consonants disappeared. 
{{:Leaper language}}


</li><li>&nbsp; Nasals disappeared before a fricative.
===Macro-Pabap languages===
See [[Macro-Pabap languages]] for all non-Babakiam stem groups.


</li><li>&nbsp; The affricates <b>mbʷ mb mḅ nḍ nd nǯ ŋg</b> shifted to <em>bʷ b ḅ ẓ ʒ ǯ g</em> in all positions.  If the preceding vowel had been long, it became short.
{{:Babakiam/Sound_changes}}


===Play (4100) to Poswa (8700)===
Alternate names: Cherry, Blossom, Pavopa, Wupupa
:NOTE that this section is outdented for prominence. Poswa is a daughter language of Play, not of Gold.


</li><li>&nbsp; The affricates <b>mpʷ mp mṗ nṭ nt nč ŋk</b> shifted to <em>pʷ p ṗ ẓ ʒ ǯ g</em> in all positions.  If the preceding vowel had been long, it became short.
#Long vowels in initial syllables became double: ''ā ī ū'' became '''aa ii uu'''. This shift did not happen if the long vowel was supported by another following vowel. Meanwhile double vowels in final syllables  became long: ''aa ii uu'' shifted to '''ā ī ū'''.  Medial syllables followed morpheme boundaries.   
 
#The double vowels ''ii uu'' became '''ʲi ʷu''' in all positions.   
 
#Any ''bʷ žʷ'' created by the previous shift changed to '''b'''. Likewise any ''bʲ žʲ'' changed to '''ž'''.
 
#:NOTE THAT  THIS IS AN EXACT REPETITION of a shift that occurred late in the history of Play.  It is repeated here because new /ʷ ʲ/ had just been created.  It could be said that the shift was in continuous operation for several hundred years.  It may have applied twice in a few words, such as buivu > bivu > žuu.  It did not generally occur over morpheme boundaries because the rebracketing that created vowel-initial morphemes had not yet taken  place.
 
#Between two consonants in a single syllable, the diphthongs ''au ai əu əi'' changed to '''o ae u e'''. ''iu ui ii uu'' > '''y y i u'''.  The change was bypassed whenever a consonant cluster was frontloaded onto the next syllable, however.
 
#:Although /ae/ behaved like a regular vowel in this shift, it was still not possible to produce syllables like */paer/, as it would be later.  This is because the shift of /u i/ > /r l/ had not yet happened.
 
#''ā aa'' changed to '''aba''' in all positions.
</li><li>&nbsp; Unstressed <b>ər</b> shifted to <em>o</em>.
#At the beginning of a syllable and after /p m/, the semivowels ''w j'' shifted to '''r l'''.  Syllable-finally, the shift also occurred in some positions but was allophonic and remained so for thousands of years.  
 
#:This is the point at which the /ae/ diphthong starts behaving differently than the others; in particular, there was no *syllable-final [al], even allophonically before another /l/. All instances of the [al] sequences were open syllables followed by a singleton /l/ onset.
 
#The medial clusters ''pt mt  mn'' shifted to '''tt nt  nn'''.  Then ''pk mk    mŋ '' became '''pt mpt    mn '''.  ''ms mš'' became '''mps mpš'''.
</li><li>&nbsp;  <b>š</b> before a nasal changed to <em>ž</em> and the nasal changed into a voiced stop.
#:The shift of /pt mt mn/ > /tt nt nn/ might need to be back-dated because it affects the way vowels were compressed.  For example, Play /pk/ front-loaded onto a following syllable, but Play /pt/ did not. This would make more sense if the contrast was either /pk/ vs /tt/ or /pt/ vs /tt/.
 
#The cluster ''sf'' changed to '''ff'''. 
</li><li>&nbsp; The velar stops <b>k g</b> were fronted to <em>č ǯ</em> unless they occurred in a cluster after another consonant and before <b><font color="#000000">a o u</font></b>.
#:In most words where an /sf/ cluster might be expected, only a single /f/ was found due to a much earlier shift that occurred in the Gold language.  This had been maintained through analogy. Likewise, where one might expect /pf/, there was often just a /p/.  The words where these clusters did occur were newly coined compounds.
 
#The labialized alveolar stop ''tʷ'' shifted to '''pʷ''' in word-initial position or after one of /r l s/, and to '''pt''' between vowels.
</li><li>&nbsp; Labialization was lost on all consonants.
#The labialized consonants ''šʷ sʷ  nʷ'' shifted to '''pš ps bʷ'''. There was also a rare word-initial /bʷ/,  from earlier /bū/ > /bu/ > /bʷ/.  This shift also includes ''sšʷ ssʷ'' shifting to '''ppš pps'''.
 
#The consonant clusters '' mr ml  '' shifted to ''' br bl  ''' unconditionally. 
</li><li>&nbsp; The clusters <b>šb</b> and <b>bš</b> were devoiced to <em>šp</em> and <em>pš</em> respectively.
#In initial position before a vowel, the voiceless labial fricatives ''f fʷ'' changed to '''w'''It also happened often to a word-internal /f fʷ/ preceded by a /w/ of any origin, but note that the sequence /fVf/ only appeared in words that were originally compounds.     This shift did not affect <b></b>. NOTE: Shift affecting internal /fʷ/, not just /f/, added late to cover for situations in Pabappa. 
 
#After a labialized consonant (except /w/), the schwa vowel ''y'' changed to '''u'''.
</li><li>&nbsp; The clusters <b>žp</b> and <b>pž</b> became <em>žb</em> and <em>bž</em> respectively.
#Labialized consonants lost their labialization when they occurred before /u/
 
#The vowels ''i u'' changed to '''e o''' in closed syllables.
 
#The vowel ''y'' in closed syllables changed to either '''i''' or '''u''' depending on the other vowel in the root. The default choice was /u/, unless it followed a /w/.  /i/ appeared only when it followed a /w/ or was in a word in which an /i/ or /e/ was in an adjacent syllable and that was the only other vowel in the word.
 
#The sequences ''wu wo'' changed to '''wi we'''.
</li><li>&nbsp; Velar stops in accented syllables before another
#Then, ''p s'' disappeared before nasals and sporadically in stem-final position due to back-formation from plurals.
syllable beginning in a velar were fronted to postalveolar affricates
#:IMPORTANTLY, it also disappears before /b/ in many, if not all, words.  Apparent exceptions can be explained by later compounding; note that they mostly involve /p bl/ --> /bbl/ where /bl/ would be expected, but seldom /p b/ > /bb/ where /b/ would be expected.
before front vowels, and otherwise to alveolar stops.
#Unaccented ''i'' changed to '''e''' unless the accented syllable of the word contained an /i/.
</li><li>&nbsp; Alveolar stops in accented syllables before another syllable beginning in an alveolar became postalveolar affricates.
#Then unaccented ''y'' changed to '''i''' unless the syllable ended in a labial or the accented syllable of the word contained a /y/.  
 
#:''NOTE ON POLITICS:'' Politically, the proto-Poswobs became independent here (5547), but the language remained unified through physical contact for another 450 years or  so.
 
#The consonant cluster ''ŋʲ'' assimilated to '''nʲ'''.  
 
#Then unaccented ''u'' changed to '''y''' except when the syllable ended in a labial or the accented syllable also contained a /u/
</li><li>&nbsp; A bilabial sound in an accented syllable before a
#Before a vowel, unaccented ''y yb'' changed to '''u'''.
syllable beginning in a labiodental sound became labiodental. A
#Before a vowel, unaccented ''a ab'' changed to '''i'''.
labiodental sound in an accented syllable before a syllable beginning
#Next, ''i'' changed to '''y''' if the next syllable had /u/
in a bilabial became bilabial.
#Any ''l r'' became devoiced after a voiceless stop (probably only /p/ exists; so the rule would be ''pl pl'' shifting to '''prʰ plʰ''').
 
#The palatalized consonants ''pʲ mʲ sʲ lʲ rʲ'' changed to '''f v š ž b''' before a vowel.  ''šʲ žʲ bʲ'' also became '''š ž b'''. The rare sequence ''rrʲ'' most likely became '''bb''', not *rb.  However, ''mmʲ'' shifted to '''mv''', not *vv.
 
#The voiceless allophones of /lʲ rʲ/ shifted to the voiceless counterparts of the plain forms; that is, ''plʲ prʲ'' shifted to '''pš pp''', matching the /ž b/ of the plain forms.  This explains why the rare sequences /pž pb/ (from compounds; never inherited from Play) did not also become voiceless. 
</li><li>&nbsp;
#The sequence ''ppp'' (the reflex of earlier /pprʲ/) shifted to '''pp'''.  But /ppš/ remained.
</li><li>&nbsp;
#:''NOTE ON POLITICS:'' (year 6000; THIS IS WHERE PABAPPA BREAKS OFF)
</li><li>&nbsp; Sonority hierarchy shifts: #hp ---&gt; #kf̥ etc
#In stressed syllables, in only a few words, ''ol ul or ur'' changed to '''we wi wa wa'''.  The conditioning environment was that the syllables had to be unstressed and have only one consonant before them; in other words, they occurred in compounds only, in a syllable which would be stressed if it weren't a compound.
</li><li>&nbsp;
#In an accented syllable, ''ar'' shifted to '''o''', except after /w/.
</li><li>&nbsp;
#Simultaneously, syllable-final ''r'' in most words changed to '''bʷ'''.  Aquatically, it was f/v before coronals, b in absolute final position, stays r before šž + labials + velars (but really pronounced as /w/). ''rl'' > '''vl'''.  ''rr'' > '''rw'''. Sometimes au+labial > o even so, no particular rule. 
</li><li>&nbsp; After a vowel, the consonant clusters <b>ṿt ṿd</b> merged as <em>d</em>If after /u/ or /o/, that vowel became long.
#The surviving final  ''r'' changed to '''vʷ fʷ''' before labials. 
</li><li>&nbsp;
#''fʷ'' (and probably ''ffʷ'') became '''w''' before a vowel (that is, everywhere except before a labial).
</li><li>&nbsp; After a vowel, the consonant clusters <b>gč gǯ</b> (g is ɣ) changed to <em>ġ</em> (a voiced velar stop).
#:Note that /ffʷ/ defies the pattern set by /ppf/ because in the latter case there was no assimilation, but /ffʷ/ had both members as bilabial, meaning it was really /fʷ:/ all along.
</li><li>&nbsp;
#In words not affected by the previous shift (mostly due to grammatical analogy), syllable-final ''ar yr'' shifted to '''o''', and ''er ir'' shifted to '''u'''. 
</li><li>&nbsp;
#In unstressed syllables, the sequences ''el il'' merged as '''i''' and ''or ur'' merged as '''u'''. 
</li><li>&nbsp;
#Unstressed ''ol ul'' became '''e''' before a consonant or at the end of a word. 
</li><li>&nbsp;
#''i y'' shifted to '''u''' before a labial in a closed syllable, or before a syllable beginning with a labial cluster (in this case, even // etc counted as "clusters").  Thus for example /tipwu/ > /tupwu/.
 
#:However, other labialized consonants such // did not trigger the shift, so /titwu/ stays /titwu/.  If there was a new /fʷ vʷ/ series, it  most likely did not trigger the shift either.  But note that the "tʲw" cluster had not yet become //.
 
#:''NOTE ON POLITICS'' ... THIS IS WHERE TUPPY BREAKS OFF (year 6843)
 
#''ŋ ŋʷ'' were denasalized to '''g gʷ''' in all positions.
 
#The palatalized velar consonants ''kʲ gʲ'' became the postalveolar affricates '''tš dž'''.
</li>
#:Note: these may have still been palatals such as '''ć ǵ''', because // behaves differently than the rare cluster /dž/ that came from collapse of /tVž/ until the later shift of /g/ > // merges /g gʲ dž/ all together.
 
#The labialized velar consonants ''kʷ gʷ pʷ bʷ'' were decomposed to the clusters '''kw gw pw bw'''. 
<br>  The Moonshine language at this point had the consonants
#Final ''y'' in trisyllabic words disappeared.  Due to analogy, it disappeared in some shorter words as well.  However, consos that now occurred at the end of a word because of the dropped y became labialized, though this is not shown in Romanizaiton.  
 
#The remaining palatalized consonants became labiodental fricatives: ''fʲ tʲ'' merged as '''f''', and ''nʲ'' changed to '''v'''.  The sequence ''nnʲ'' shifted to '''nv''' and later /mv/ (still later /mb/ in most environments).
<p></p><p class="mid">
#The cluster ''pf'', when straddling a syllable boundary, became '''ff'''.  It is likely that /mpf/ and /ppf/ did not exist at this time, therefore making this shift unconditional.
<br>
#:NOTE, actually /pptʲ/ did exist, and this changes to /ppf/ in the shift above.
 
#Medial vowels in trisyllabic words disappeared if the resulting consonant cluster was acceppable ("the Debra shift"). ''wr'' > '''rw''' (distinct from earlier shift).  Here again, labialization hung around if the deleted vowel was o u or y.  Thus there were minimal pairs such as <em>puppa</em> "salamander" vs <em>pupʷpa</em> "kind, humanitarian".  This period (around the year 7300) is the beginning of what is often considered "Classical Poswa".
<pable>
#:Poswobs invaded Pabappa territory beginning around 7414, and the language was essentially unchanged at that time. The next few changes never occur in native words because they would have appeared in compounds only, and grammatical reanalysis eliminated the clusters that fed the shift; however, they occur in opaque loanwords and names.  
<pbody><pr><pd width="50"><em>    </em></pd><pd width="50">
#The clusters  ''tm km fm vm'' shifted to '''vb'''.  ''šm žm sm šb sb'' shifted to ''' žb'''.
<em>    </em></pd><pd width="50"><em>    </em></pd><pd width="50"><em>    </em></pd><pd width="50"><em>    </em></pd><pd width="50"><em>
#The clusters ''pn bn'' shifted to ''' vž'''. ''šn sn'' became '''žv'''.
    </em></pd><pd width="50"><em>    </em></pd><pd width="50"><em>    </em></pd></pr>
#The sequences ''pm tn'' shifted to '''mm nn'''. ''tp'' became '''pp'''.  Importantly, however, the nasal clusters /mn nm/ both remained intact.
 
#The clusters ''pk'' and ''bg'' became '''pw''' and '''bi''' respectively.  ''mk'' became '''mw''' (not mpw).  Similar changes applied to other clusters ending in velars. The clusters /bk pg/ did not exist at this time because regressive voicing assimilation had automatically shifted them to /pk bg/.  This voicing assimilation only happened when the Debra shift brought the consonants into direct contact.
<pr><pd><em> p  </em></pd><pd><em> b   </em></pd><pd><em> fʷ  </em></pd><pd><em> vʷ </em></pd><pd><em> m  </em></pd><pd><em> mʰ </em></pd><pd><em> w  </em></pd><pd><em>    </em></pd></pr>
#All posttonic ''k g'' shifted to '''w i''', taking any intervening vowels with them; this is the same as the shift above except that there was no voicing assimilation.  This also included the ''gʲ'' spoken of above which was listed as /dž/ but still behaves distinctly from preexisting /dž/ from //.  Note also that this shift did not affect /ž/.
<pr><pd><em> ṗ  </em></pd><pd><em> ḅ  </em></pd><pd><em> f   </em></pd><pd><em> v   </em></pd><pd><em> ṃ </em></pd><pd><em> ṃʰ </em></pd><pd><em>    </em></pd><pd><em>    </em></pd></pr>
#:''NOTE: This shift is important and may be handled incorrectly in some dictionary entries.  It seems to imply that '''*ANY*''' posttonic /w/ also consumes preceding vowels, which often happens when coming from earlier /fʷ/.  On the other hand, there needs to be a path that generates posttonic /uw/, as distinct from /w/, and this might need to come from sequences like /wiw/.''
<pr><pd><em> t  </em></pd><pd><em> d  </em></pd><pd><em> s  </em></pd><pd><em> z  </em></pd><pd><em> n  </em></pd><pd><em> nʰ  </em></pd><pd><em>    </em></pd><pd><em>    </em></pd></pr>
#Posttonic sequences like ''igigʷ'' and ''agik'' shifted to '''uw'''. That is, wherever a sequence like /iw/ arose, this became /uw/. But double /k/ sequences still ended up as a simple /w/.
<pr><pd><em> ṭ  </em></pd><pd><em> ḍ  </em></pd><pd><em> ṣ  </em></pd><pd><em> ẓ  </em></pd><pd><em> ṇ  </em></pd><pd><em> ṇʰ </em></pd><pd><em>    </em></pd><pd><em>    </em></pd></pr>
#The voiced velar stop ''g'' was fronted to '''dž''' unless it occurred in a cluster after another consonant and before /a o u/.   
<pr><pd><em> č  </em></pd><pd><em> ǯ  </em></pd><pd><em> š  </em></pd><pd><em> ž  </em></pd><pd><em> ñ  </em></pd><pd><em> ñʰ  </em></pd><pd><em> j  </em></pd><pd><em>    </em></pd></pr>
#:This shift is not being followed exactly; it is behaving mostly like /k/ except in the onset where it does indeed shift to //.
<pr><pd><em> k  </em></pd><pd><em> g  </em></pd><pd><em> h  </em></pd><pd><em> ɣ  </em></pd><pd><em> ŋ  </em></pd><pd><em> ŋʰ </em></pd><pd><em>    </em></pd><pd><em>    </em></pd></pr>
#The clusters ''šb bš'' were devoiced to '''šp pš''' respectively. Then  ''sb bs'' shifted to '''sp ps'''. That is, the fricative dominated the stop, unlike most such changes.
<pr><pd><em> ʔ  </em></pd><pd><em>    </em></pd><pd><em>    </em></pd><pd><em>    </em></pd><pd><em>     </em></pd><pd><em>     </em></pd><pd><em>    </em></pd><pd><em>    </em></pd></pr>
#:These clusters existed despite a previous shift of the same clusters because in both cases they appeared only in foreign loans and in some ad-hoc compounds.  This means that an original ''sb'' could variably appear in Poswa as any of '''s b žb sp''', depending on when the two consonants came together. 
 
#The clusters ''žp pž'' became '''žb bž''' respectively.
</pbody></pable>
#The clusters ''tv ttv'' shifted to '''v vv'''; this was actually through a [d] allophone, which explains why they did not both become geminates.
 
#Likewise, ''tf ttf'' shifted to '''t tt'''.  This may seem a contradiction but it is actually the same shift as the above, explained by the path of /t/ > /d/ > /ð/ > /v/ which is compressed into one step above.  (Note that in one word, /pts/ shifts to /pps/ as though /ts/ cannot start a syllable.)
</p><p class="body">
#The clusters ''nr nl'' shifted to '''ng''' (IPA [nd]). 
<br> and the vowels
#The clusters ''pl bl'' became '''p b''' respectively when overlaying two unaccented syllables.  The same also happened for other stop + nonstop clusters such as ''pr br pš ps bž pt pf bv'', althoiugh pš, pf, and ps survived as aspirates pʰ for long enough to survive a particular subsequent shift.  Clusters like ''mž mdž'' shifted to ''' mb'''. 
<em>/a ā e ē i ī o ō u ū ə/</em>.  
#The sequences ''yw ww wy'' changed to '''ʷu''' in all positions.
 
#When unaccented, the sequence ''wi'' shifted to '''i''' before another vowel.
 
#''iy ii'' > '''ia ie'''.  This also includes /ir/ > /ia/ (when not labialized).
<br><br>
#In wholly unstressed syllables, except when preceded by /a/, the clusters ''rl lr'' changed to '''al ar''' respectively, and raised the preceding vowel. 
The alphabet now consisted of the consonants <em>/p b ṗ ḅ f v m mʰ w t ṭ d ḍ s z ṣ ẓ n nʰ š ž k g ŋ ŋʰ h x l ř j r/</em> and the vowels <em>/a e i o u ə ā ē ī ō ū ə̄ à è ì ò ù ə̀ á é í ó ú ə́/</em>.
#The cluster ''rgw'' shifted to '''vbw''', with both consonants labialized.
 
#A velar-onset syllable preceded by another changed to alveolar if the vowel was a back vowel, but postalveolar if it was a front vowel. The affected consonants were ''k g kw gw r''. 
 
#The diphthongs ''al yl'' shifted to '''ae e'''.  These did not affect /lʷ/.
This is considered to be the state of classical Moonshine, also known as Rúló.
#Geminate stops overlaying two unaccented syllables reduced to single if there was another geminate earlier in the word. 
 
#The cluster ''mp'' shifted to '''mb''' when occuring after a voiceless stop.
 
#Initial ''ps pš'' shifted to '''p''', also sometimes if overlaying two unaccented slabs just as /pl bl/ had.
</p></ol>
 
 
 
 
 
 
<br><br><br>
 
<pable><pbody><pr><pd width="50"></pd><pd>
 
====Rúló to Xykhasl (year 12850 AD)====
</p>
 
 
<ol>
 
<li>&nbsp; Intervocalically, the dental consonants <b>ṗ ḅ ṭ ḍ</b> came to be written as <em>pf bv tṣ dẓ</em>, and to be treated as consonant clusters.
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp;  In word-initial position and after another consonant they became the plain fricatives <em>f v ṣ ẓ</em>.
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp; The vowel <b>/u/</b> came to be spelled <em><font color="#000000">/ū/</font></em>; this was a spelling change rather than a phonetic one.   
 
</li><li>&nbsp; In bisyllabic roots, if the vowel in the second syllable was rounded, the vowels in the first syllable changed from <b>/a e i o ū ə/</b> to <em>/â ū y ô y u/</em>, where
<b><font color="#000000">â</font></b> spells the SAMPA sound <em>Q</em>,
<b><font color="#000000">ū</font></b> spells <em>u:</em>,
<b><font color="#000000">ô</font></b> spells <em>u:</em>,
and <b><font color="#000000">y</font></b> spells <em>y</em>.
 


===Baba (~6000) to Old Pabappa (~6500) ... too early?===
Alternate names: Pespimbesa
   
   
#Debra shift I. (But rV and wV didnt compress).  ("ae" and "al" remained distinct here). If the deleted vowel was y, o or u, the preceding consonant became a labial. If a consonant was already labialized or palatalized, it did not contract.
#:This shift must be broken into steps because it does not turn /tut/ > /pt/.  It may be that the /tʷ/ > /p/ actually happened later in a second shift where only /ə/ was deleted.
#In the syllable coda, ''lʷ rʷ'' shifted to '''u'''.  Also, in clusters like /kʷr/, this became [kʷrʷ], and then the resulting ''lʷ rʷ'' shifted to '''w'''.  This can be identified as part of the same shift above.
#Homorganic stop-fricative sequences like ''pʷf'' resulting from the vowel elision came to stand at the head of a syllable, and therefore both consonants became labialized, resulting in sequences that were spelled '''pfʷ'''.
#After a stop, ''fʷ'' shifted to a fully rounded voiceless bilabial fricative, here spelled '''h''' because there was no other /h/. 
#:This also extended to shifts like ''tʷl'' shifting to '''tʷ''', since there was no /lʷ/  or /rʷ/.
#''fʲ'' shifted to '''f'''. This means that it cannot participate in the shift below where palatalized consonants stain the following vowel to /i/.
#''ŋ'' was denasalized to '''g''' in all positions, except bound clusters such as /ŋp ŋt ŋr ŋk/ which behaved as prenasals.
#:NEED to check dictionary to make sure this rule has been followed properly, including subsequent changes.  For example, if ''kg'' shifts to '''gg''' later, it ends up as '''bb''' ultimately, the only voiced geminate in the language.  But perhaps they should be devoiced when they were still stops.
#Clusters like ''np nb'' shifted to '''mp mb''' (homorganic). This does NOT shift /mpt/ etc intp /nt/. which happens later and is important to happe nalter.
#Following a consonant, diphthongs of all types were resolved in favor of the first vowel.  Thus, all palatalized consonants shifted to the corresponding plain form (unlike in Poswa, where most had distinct reflexes).  That is to say, ''ʲa ʲe ʲi ʲo ʲu ʲy'' all became a simple '''i''' and ''ʷa ʷe ʷi ʷo ʷu ʷy'' became a simple '''u'''.
#:Importantly, this did not come from a bare /w/, even in word-initial position; /wes/ remained /wes/ instead of shifting to /us/.
#:Note that this means syllable-final ''lʷ rʷ'', which earlier became /u/, now disappear to '''Ø''' except in the rare cases mentioned below.  And ''əu'' shifts to '''ə''' (usually later to /o/).
#Triphthongs with a high middle element were collapsed to two-vowel sequences along the opposite pattern; thus, for example, ''iwa''> '''ua'''.
#:It is possible that ''iai'' shifted to just an '''i'''.  If not, it would be /ia/, not /ai/.
#Stops occurring before a nasal assimilated fully.
#Clusters of two fricatives of differing points of articulation were assimilated in favor of the second consonant.
#The clusters ''žbž špš sps'' shifted to '''žž šš ss''', and likewise for mismatched pairs.
#Clusters of fricatives and stops of dissimilar voicing were resolved in favor of the second consonant. This includes ''fž sž'' shifting to '''vž žž''' (later to /d Ø/).
#The clusters ''gv kf'' became '''bv pf'''.
#Word-initial ''pš tš'' shifted to '''š ''' while word-initial ''ps ts'' shifted to '''s'''. 
#:Note that initial /tš/ occurs only in the copula; it is not /kʲ/, which had remained as such and then dropped its palatalization in a shift just shortly before this one. 
#The fricatives ''šʷ žʷ'' (possibly only found in the coda) shifted to '''f v'''.
#:NOTE that this is an important and newly introduced shift, as before they behaved as normal /š ž/ and therefore mostly shifted to /s Ø/.
#The fricative ''š  '' changed to '''h''' in initial position and to '''s''' elsewhere.
#The voiced sequences ''ž dž'' came to be pronounced '''z'''. 
#The cluster ''rr'' was reduced to '''r'''.


====Old Pabappa (6500?) to Pabappa (8700)====


</li><li>&nbsp; If the vowel in the second syllable was <em>/i/</em>, then <b>/o ū/</b> in the first syllable changed to <em>/oj ūj/</em>.   
#Double nasals were reduced to singles.
 
#The vowel ''ə'' either disappeared or became '''i''' or '''o''' (governed by the surrounding vowels' HEIGHT (not backness))If it disappeared, it also labialized the new final consonant.
 
#:NOTE THAT THIS INCLUDES MEDIAL SYLLABLES, since /rə/ did not delete earlier. However it is not clear what /rʷ/ will turn into, and /b/ is unlikely.
</li><li>&nbsp; High tone vowels came to be distinguished primarily by
#:This also includes WORD-FINAL SYLLABLES that did not delete during the Debra shift because there was no following syllable.
being lax rather than by being of high pitch. Allophonic pitch
#:Consider also forcing /kʷ/ > /p/ at least in final position, since it will otherwise disappear. Additionally, earlier an implied shift of ''tʷ'' > '''p''' took place word-finally, which would imply // > /p/ too if kept.
distinctions began to arise, and soon tone had been completely replaced
#The fricatives ''v z'' shifted to '''d Ø''' in all positions. This included doubled forms.
by laxness.
#:It is possible that this causes the relatively common cluster ''bv'' to become '''dd''' (not /bd/), as per the shift involving /ps/ below. This would be an affricate at first.
</li><li>&nbsp; Consonant clusters and final consonants, aside from <em>c ʒ č ǯ</em> were simplified: any <b>/p/</b> or <b>/š/</b> at the end of a syllable disappeared and made the preceding vowel into a lax vowel.
#:Another possible exception is that ''vz'' (common in a suffix, from earlier -fž-) might also become '''dd''' because the two consonants were shifting at about the same time, and  the v > d shift passed through /ð/.  Still, if /z/ disappeared even slightly before /v/ became /ð/, the reflex would be a simple '''d''' after all. One reason to expect that /z/ > /Ø/ was slightly earlier is that the shift of /f/ > /s/ is displaced below.
 
#:Note also that the shift of /zz/ > /Ø/ means that the reflexes of the palatalized forms of ''sl šl žl'' are all '''Ø'''; this is important because many words for handheld objects end in /-sla -šla -žla/, which therefore all become /-a/ when possessed.
 
#The velar stops ''k g'' changed to the fricatives '''š ž''' in all positions.
</li><li>&nbsp; (although in the case of <b>žb</b> and <b>šp</b>, the <b>ž</b> and <b>š</b>
#:These were weakly labialized, but it is important not to confuse them with the true labialized fricatives belowThis is complicated, because the reflexes essentially switch places: /š ž/ mostly become /p b/, but /šʷ/ becomes /s/ except in initial position (and it may never have occurred initially), while /žʷ/ might not exist at all.
survived and the labials didn't). Double consonants and affricates
#The labialized consonants ''sʷ šʷ  žʷ'' shifted to '''f f  b'''(It is not clear that a distinct /žʷ/ still existed, but if it did, it too would have been carried away.) Then ''nʷ'' shifted to '''m''' at least in final position.
simplified to singles and also laxed the preceding vowel. Final <b>/m/</b>
#The clusters ''šs sš'' became '''ss šš'''. (Unless /š/ had already shifted to /f/.)
disappeared with no effect, although it began to spread as an allophone
#The fricative ''f'' changed to '''p''' in initial position and ''s'' elsewhere.  This also triggers a shift of all ''pf'' to '''ts''', even though /ps/ remained as such.
to places in which it had never been before. Voiceless nasals also
#This was around the year 7000.  Note that the Play 1st and 2nd person past passive participle endings /-su -si/  had become /-s -se/ by this stage; it is unlikely that Pabappa would preserve them since the 1st person would have become silent, but other daughter languages may preserve both.
laxed the preceding vowel.
#The voiceless stops ''p t'' became the geminates '''pp tt''' when following a voiceless stop plus a vowel.
</li><li>&nbsp; The dental fricatives <b>f v</b> changed to <em>ṣ ẓ</em> at the beginning of a word and between vowels.   
#The postalveolars  ''š ž dž'' became  '''f v bv''' in all positions.
 
#:The shift of /dž/ > /bv/ is listed here because if /d/ remained unassimilated it would devoice and then fall into the /tp/ > /tt/ rule below. But a shift of /ž/ > /v/ without affecting an adjoining /d/ is highly unlikely.
 
#The cluster ''ph'' (phonetically something like /pɸ/) shifted to   '''pp'''.
</li><li>&nbsp; The postalveolar affricates <b>č ǯ</b> became the fricatives <em>š ž</em> in all positions.  
#:Note that this is actually a dummy shift to prevent it from being voiced to /b/.  In fact, it remained as roughly /pɸ/, still distinct from the rarer true /pf/, which shifts to /pp/  later on, and also distinct from a simple /p/, which in some cases shifts to /b/ later on.
 
#The glottal fricative ''h'' shifted to '''f''' before /u/.
 
#:It is possible that this shift can be moved further down so as not to interfere with the shift above.
</li><li>&nbsp; The alveolar stops <b>t d</b> and the affricates <b>c ʒ</b> merged as <em>č ǯ</em> before front vowels.  In other positions they remained the same.
#The voiced stops ''b d'' became the voiceless stops '''p t''' in all positions.  Adjacent fricatives also devoiced.
 
#:It is possible that the cluster ''vv'' should become '''ff''' here, even though there was no stop.  It is the only surviving voiced fricative, and therefore the only surviving voiced fricative cluster.
</li><li>&nbsp; In clusters the dental fricatives <b>ṣ ẓ</b> became the alveolar stops <em>t d</em>.
#Word-final ''s  '' disappeared to '''Ø'''.
 
#:The word-final /-s/ was soon restored in just one instance, the Play word '''pais''' and anything rhyming with it; this is because all of the inflected forms of /pais/ had become identical to those of Play /pasi/, which had not lost its /s/ because it was still padded by /e/. The time interval between these two shifts was very short, and effectively by merging the two words the shift was undone in just one environment.
 
#:Note that there is probably no word-final /f/, but if it does somehow appear, it would also shift to Ø.
</li><li>&nbsp; All unstressed short vowels were reduced to the set <em>/ă ĭ u ə/</em>.   If they had been lax, they also laxed the preceding vowel.   
#Word-final ''e'' disappeared, except after a consonant cluster; in this case it changed to '''i'''.
 
#:Many longstanding irregulars were regularized here. For example, ''nisol ~ nipse-'' became '''nipsi ~ nipse-''' by making what had been the genitive (ending in -s) the new nominative form. 
</li><li>&nbsp; After a vowel, <b>wĭ</b> changed to <em>j</em>,  
#Before a nasal, ''p s t n'' assimilated completely.
 
#Clusters of a nonlabial stop followed by a labial stop were resolved in favor of the nonlabial one. It seems most likely that this is just ''tp'' shifting to '''tt'''.
</li><li>&nbsp; <b>sĭ</b> changed to <em>š</em>,  
#Final ''o'' was were lowered to '''a''' except if the accented vowel was mid-height (e or o).  
 
#:NOTE ON POLITICS: This is considered to be the classical stage of Middle Pabappa.
 
#The clusters ''tl ttl'' both changed to '''ll'''. 
</li><li>&nbsp; and <b>kĭ</b> and <b>tĭ</b> coalesced as <em>tš</em>.   
#The sequences ''mr sr lr'' became '''mpr spr rr'''.  Any other nonlabial consonant before /r/ became labial. 
 
#Clusters of a nasal followed by any other consonant of differing point of articulation were assimilated in favor of the point of articulation of the second consonant. Thus surviving ''mn'' shifted to '''nn'''.
 
#''ml'' became '''mpl'''.
</li><li>&nbsp; Unaccented long vowels and diphthongs were reduced to the monophthongs <em>a e i o u y</em>.
#:NOTE ON POLITICS: This occurred around 7414 AD.
 
#The voiceless labiodental fricative ''f'' changed to '''w''' in initial position.
</li><li>&nbsp; The remaining long vowels <b>ā ē ī ō ū ȳ ə̄</b> changed to <em>a aj i aw ū y ə</em>. The letter <em>ū</em> was not a true long vowel any longer, but only a higher and clearer version of <em>u</em>.
#The voiceless glottal fricative ''h'' shifted to '''Ø'''.
 
#Unstressed syllables of the form CVCC where the two latter C's were a geminate or one of a few other types of consonants changed the vowel to a very short schwa /ə/.
 
#Intervocalic voiceless stops became voiced.
 
#The cluster ''pl'' became '''bl''' in all positions.  
</li><li>&nbsp; All final vowels in bisyllabic roots were deleted. If the vowel deleted was <b>ĭ</b>, the vowels in the first syllable changed from <b>/a à è ì ə/</b> to <em>/aj àj e i ĭ/</em>.
#The cluster ''pr'' became '''b''' in word-initial position or after /m/, '''p''' after /s/, and '''br''' elsewhere.
In compound words and certain inflected forms, the second vowel in the
#:Consider ''lbr rbr'' > '''lb rb''' as well, and perhaps extension to all clusters (though other clusters would be rare, since pVrr was rare or perhaps nonexistent).
word was deleted if the resulting consonant cluster was acceppable
#The geminates  ''pp tt ss'' became '''p t s''' in all positions.
("the Debra shift"). If the second vowel occurred between two labial
#The cluster ''sp'' became '''ss'''. 
consonants, the first labial consonant was deleted and the second was
#The rare vowel sequences ''ei ou'' (found almost entirely in verb conjugations) shifted to '''e o'''.
metathesized so that it took the place of the first. Then the place of
#:NOTE ON POLITICS: (ANDANESE PABAPPA SPLITS OFF HERE)
articulation of that consonant changed to match the vowel it occurred
#Initial ''v'' became '''f'''.
next to, as the vowel was deleted. </li><li>&nbsp; <b>u ù</b> became fronted to mid vowels but there was
#The labiodental fricatives ''f v'' became the bilabial stops '''p b''' in all positions.
no change in spelling. All roots that came from Rúló had been either
#The marginal schwa phoneme disappeared, creating some new clusters and geminates. (Debra shift II.
one or two syllables. With this sound shift they nearly all came to be
#The bilabial stop ''b'', between two identical unaccented vowels (e.g. -aba, -obo), turned to '''m''' sporadically as the result of analogy from various noun declensions.
one syllable, although due to changes in grammar they were almost
always used with a suffix containing a vowel and thus adding a
syllable. That is to say, the suffixes from the old monosyllables were
applied to these new monosyllables, making the old suffixes and infixes
for bisyllables obsolete.
</li><li>&nbsp; The dental fricatives <b>ṣ ẓ</b> changed back to <em>f v</em> in all positions.
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp; The labiodental fricatives <b>f v</b> became <em>h x</em> in word-initial position before a back vowel and between a back vowel and another vowel of any type;
 
</li><li>&nbsp; In clusters the labiodental fricatives <b>f v</b> became <em>p b</em>. 
 
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp; Before the front vowels <em>e è i ì û ú</em>, the velar stops <b>k</b> and <b>g</b> were fronted to the postalveolar affricates <em>č</em> and <em>ǯ</em>, which were considered single phonemes rather than clusters.  
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp; At the end of a closed syllable the bilabial stop <b>b</b> came to be pronounced as <em>/ə/</em>, with a common allophone of <em>[w]</em>; however there was no change in the native spelling.
 
</li><li>&nbsp; At the end of a closed syllable the bilabial stop <b>p</b> came to be pronounced as <em>[ʔ]</em>, however there was no change in the native spelling.  That is, the ligatures of <em>vowel</em> + <em>p</em>, which are transliterated with grave accents, continued to be used.
 
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp; The labiodental fricatives <b>f v</b> became the bilabial stops <em>p b</em>
in all positions, although at the end of a few words they disappeared
completely. They were spelled with the letters for the "hard" <em>p b</em> because in some writings the letters for the ordinary <em>p b</em> were used for <em>/? ə/</em>.
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp; Voiced stops became prenasalized after a tense vowel;
lax vowels before voiced stops became allophonically tense but did not
gain prenasalization. </li><li>&nbsp; The lax/tense distinction in vowels disappeared,
leaving vowel quality alone to distinguish them and meaning that
glottal stops after certain vowels were no longer pronounced. However,
the changes that the earlier system had inflicted on the consonants
still remained.
</li><li>&nbsp;  A chain shift occurred: the old vowel <b>ì</b> came to be pronounced as <em>e</em>, meaning that the old vowel <b>e</b> came to be pronounced as <em>ɛ</em>, which caused the old vowel <b>è</b> to become pronounced as <em>a</em>, which caused the old vowel <b>a</b> to become pronounced as a back <em>ɑ</em>.  Meanwhile a similar shift occurred in the back vowels: <b>o</b> became <em>ɔ</em>, which caused <b>ò</b> to become a low back <em>ɒ</em>.  Now, only roundedness and frontness distinguished the two forms of <em>o</em> and <em>a</em>; the heights were the same.
 
<br><br>
<p class="topic">
<small>Classical-Era Changes:</small>
</p>
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp; In some idiolects, a religious taboo forbade the pronunciation of the phonemes <b>k g</b> except when used for the names of the forces of good and evil; in other contexts they shifted to <em>/q G/</em>.
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp; Most speakers began to merge the new <b>q G</b> phonemes with <em>h x</em>.
 
</li><li>&nbsp; Roundedness disappeared on <em>o ò</em>, thus leaving only frontness to distinguish them from <em>a à</em>.
 
</li><li>&nbsp; Unstressed <b>u</b> became a true schwa.  The script was now written with <em>u</em> as the inherent vowel (previously it was schwa).
 
</li><li>&nbsp; <em>o ò</em> merged with <em>a à</em>.
 
 
 
</li><li>&nbsp; The low vowel <b>a</b> rounded and moved to the back position and <b>à</b> became low to replace it.   
 
 
<br><br>
 
The alphabet now consisted of the consonants
<em>/p b š ž m w t d s z n j c ʒ č ǯ k g h x ŋ r l ř/</em>
and the vowels
<em>/@ i e E a A O o u y/</em>.
 
<br> The spelling of the vowels was as such:
 
<br><br>
 
<pable class="body">
<pbody><pr><pd>ə</pd><pd>i</pd><pd>e</pd><pd>ɛ</pd><pd>a</pd><pd>ɑ</pd><pd>ɔ</pd><pd>o</pd><pd>u</pd><pd>y</pd><pd>  </pd></pr>
<pr><pd>u</pd><pd>i</pd><pd>ì</pd><pd>e</pd><pd>è</pd><pd>o</pd><pd>a</pd><pd>â</pd><pd>ô</pd><pd>û</pd><pd>  </pd></pr>
</pbody></pable>
 
o could also be spelled à, and a could also be spelled ò.
This is considered to be the state of classical Laveti Moonshine.
<br><br>
<p class="topic">
<small>Post-Classical Changes:</small>
</p>
 
</li><li>&nbsp; Letters with inherent vowels sometimes appeared as the
onset of a stressed syllable (mostly in Bloppabop loans, but in a few
native words also). Previously the <b>u</b> ones were pronounced with /w/, but that disappeared, and as it did so the ones with <b>a</b> became velarised and in some cases (especially velars) also labialized. 
 
</li></ol>
 
 
</pd></pr></pbody></pable>
 
 
</pd></pr></pbody></pable>
 
 
 
 
<pable><pbody><pr><pd width="65"></pd><pd>
 
 
 
 
 
<link rel="stylesheet" type="text/css" href="./pabappa28_files/europe.htm">
 
===Tapilula to Late Andanese (4178)===
====Tapilula (0) to Old Andanese (1900)====
#The accent pattern involved in certain infixes with accented schwa switched to favor the following vowel.  e.g. ăpo "field", apə̀ho "field (possessive) became <em>ăpo apəhò</em>.
#The "labial" vowel <b>ə</b> (like /u/ but with flat lips) disappeared, syllabified nearby consonants or turned to <em>i</em> if the nearby consonants were not possible to become syllabic. 
#:Often, this sound change created new clusters whose second element was /h/.  Since all voiceless stops were inherently aspirated (that is, /ph/ was equivalent to /p/, and so on), this change effectively removed /h/ after a voiceless stop.  However, voiced stops were devoiced by the shift.  Other consonants simply retained the cluster for the time being; so that, for example, /mh/ was allowed. No daughter languages retained these clusters as such, but they resolved them in different ways in each branch.
#The labial stops <b>p b</b> both changed to <em>w</em> except if they were: 1) After an accented or high-tone vowel (but not before); 2) In a consonant cluster of any kind; or 3) In a monosyllabic word.
#:See below ... it's possible that this change could have been [ph] > [px] > [xʷ] > [gʷ], which would make it identical with the pretonic allophone of /w/, or it could be simply [p] > [b] > [w] unrelated to the change involving /t/. 
#The velar nasal <b>ŋ</b> changed to <em>n</em> in all positions.  (this was originally 3 steps lower but is irrelevant for the end result)
#The alveolar voiceless stop <b>t</b> became <em>k</em> except if it was: 1) After an accented or high-tone vowel (but not before); 2) In a consonant cluster of any kind; or 3) In a monosyllabic word. 
#:Since all voiceless stops were inherently aspirated all along, a likely path for this change would be [th] > [tx] > [kx] > [kh], which would make it identical with /k/.
#''tʷ dʷ nʷ'' > ''kʷ ġʷ ŋʷ''.
#The labialized sounds <b>kʷ ġʷ hʷ w</b> changed to <em>k ġ h g</em> when they preceded a vowel followed by a labial consonant (including /w/), and changed the pitch of the following vowel to high (3).    (this was originaly near FAHAH but was moved up to match Ukieipi's version). POSSIBLY SCRUB THE TONE CHANGE
#''ŋʷ''> '''ŋ''''.
#The glottalized stop <b>k̩</b> changed to a uvular <em>q</em> in all positions.
#The voiced stops <b>b d ġ ġʷ </b> became voiceless '''p t k kʷ''', but /p t/ remained voiced allophonically between vowels. 
 
'''NOTE ON POLITICS: This was the stage of Old Andanese (year 1900), the same date as Gold. '''
 
====Old Andanese (1900) to Late Andanese (4178)====
Note that all of the tone changes below are entirely rirrelevant, because Late Andanese ends up losing its tones, and so does Babakiam, which was the only language that took any significant number of loans from Late Andanese.
 
<li>&nbsp; Labialized stops <b>pʷ tʷ kʷ</b> all changed to <em>p</em>.
</li>
 
<li>Like-vowel sequences pulled up again, so e.g. /tuhu/ > /tu/, but /tohu/ remains and later becomes /tuhu/.</li>
 
<li>/hi ki ti/ > /s/ before a vowel</li> .
 
<li>&nbsp;  The labial fricative <b>f</b> changed to <em>h</em> in all positions, and changed the pitch of the following vowel to high (3).  This is sometimes spelled h́.
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; All remaining occurrences of the labial approximant <b>w</b> changed to <em>l</em>. (CHANGE ID FAHAH)
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; The uvular stop <b>q</b> changed to the glottal stop <em>ʔ</em> unconditionally.
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; The mid vowels <b>e o</b> changed to <em>i u</em> in all positions.
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; Nasalized vowels changed fricatives on either side of them into nasals. 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; Nasalized vowels changed stops after them (but not before) into nasals.  Thus tãpa "vineyard" &gt; tama.
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; Nasalization disappeared everywhere.
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; Tones were eliminated except in syllables with no consonants.
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; Tones were eliminated.  Andanese now had only 9 consonants, 3 vowels, and no tones, and was entirely CV, thus making it the most phonologically simple language in the world.
</li>
 
</ol>
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
<pable><pbody><pr><pd width="15"></pd><pd>
 
 
 
 
 
<link rel="stylesheet" type="text/css" href="./pabappa28_files/europe.htm">
 
===Pre-Proto-Tapilula to POP3 (4000 BC)===
</p>
 
<p class="body">
Original phonology:<br> 
<br>  Pre-Proto-Tapilula, also known as Paleo-Southeast Laban, had 18 consonants ( <em>p b m w ṭ ḍ ṇ ḷ t d n l r j k ġ ŋ h </em>  ) and 4 vowels,<em> a i u ə</em> (schwa is spelled {y}).
 
 
<br><br>V was really a spread-labial w, not actually /v/.  Nor was it wʲ or jʷ.  ɰ is silent, but separates vowels so that they arent
dip[hthongs, and does have a sound between 2 identical vowels. 
</p>
 
 
<ol>
 
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; The voiced velar sounds <b>g ġ</b> changed to <em>ž</em> before /i/, <em>v</em> before /u/, and disappeared otherwise.
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;  The voiceless sound <b>h</b> changed to <em>š</em> before /i/, <em>f</em> before /u/, and disappeared otherwise.
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp;  Are the above f/v distinct from rounded labials??  Or labiodentals?
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;  f &gt; v?
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;  The labiodental fricatives <b>f v</b> changed to <em>p b</em>.
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;  <b>hʷ</b> became <em>h</em>. (???)
</li>
 
 
 
 
 
<li>&nbsp;   
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
 
 
<li>&nbsp;  The dental sounds <b>ṭ ṇ ḷ ḍ</b> all coalesced as <em>v</em> (true v this time).
</li>
 
 
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;  All stops became voiceless.
</li>
 
 
 
<li>&nbsp; The dental fricative <b>v</b> turned into a voiced bilabial stop <em>b</em>.  This was thus the only voiced stop in the language.
 
</li>
 
 
 
 
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
 
 
<li>&nbsp;   
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
 
 
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
 
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp;   
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
 
</ol>
 
 
 
 
 
</pd></pr></pbody></pable>
 
 
 
 
 
</pd>
 
 
</pr></pbody></pable>
 
<pable><pbody><pr><pd width="15"></pd><pd>
 
 
 
 
 
<link rel="stylesheet" type="text/css" href="./pabappa28_files/europe.htm">
 
 
 
 
 
<p class="topic">
===Silatibarra to Proto-Outer Poswob (1583 (wrong date???))===
</p>
 
<p class="body">
Original phonology:<br> 
<br>pʷ mʷ w bʷ    ......    hʷ  ..... m: ṇ: n: ŋ: ..... voiceless nasals????
<br>pʲ mʲ v bʲ
<br>ṭ ṇ ḷ ḍ
<br>t n l d s r
<br>tl nl  dl sl
<br>k ŋ ɰ ġ h
 
 
<br><br>V was really a spread-labial w, not actually /v/.  Nor was it wʲ or jʷ.  ɰ is silent, but separates vowels so that they arent
dip[hthongs, and does have a sound between 2 identical vowels. 
</p>
 
 
<ol>
 
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; Tones became raised before voiceless consonants: ă --&gt; à; à --&gt; á. Note that the symbols are used slightly differently here from their usage in other lanmguages.  In the future, â is low tone (but usually unmarked); ă or à is medium tone; á is high tone.
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; All prefixes came to have low tone, regardless of what consonant was before them. 
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp;  The vowels <b>a e i</b> became <em>e i i</em> whenever not held down by other vowels, or by labialization.
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp;<s> The spread-labial sounds <b>pʲ mʲ v bʲ</b> all coalesced as <em>v</em> (true v this time).</s> <!-- BAD!!!!! -->
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;  The glottal fricative <b>h</b> became <em>š</em> when touching a front vowel (/e i/).
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;  Rounded labials became plain labials in all words.  <b>hʷ</b> became <em>h</em>.
</li>
 
 
 
 
 
<li>&nbsp;  The schwa vowel disappeared in all possible syllables, unless accented.
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
 
 
<li>&nbsp;  The dental sounds <b>ṭ ṇ ḷ ḍ</b> all coalesced as <em>v</em> (true v this time).
</li>
 
 
 
<li>&nbsp;  Any remaining schwa became <em>a</em>.
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;  All stops became voiceless.
</li>
 
 
 
<li>&nbsp; The dental fricative <b>v</b> turned into a voiced bilabial stop <em>b</em>.  This was thus the only voiced stop in the language.
 
</li>
 
 
 
 
 
<li>&nbsp;  The laterals <b>tl nl</b> became plain <em>t n</em>.  (This was near the end of the time spent near Tapilula).
</li>
 


====Sister languages of Pabappa====
:''For sound changes, see [[Macro-Pabap_languages#East_of_Paba]].''


<li>&nbsp;   
Sister languages split off from Pabappa in several waves, and could arguably include tiny, relic branches that are older than the Poswa-Pabappa split as well as those that are youngerThe arbitrary breaks at around 6500, 7000, 7414, and the unlabeled Andanese Pabappa split do not correspond to wars, as the Pabaps had long since ceased fighting wars.
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp; "Suppressed" tones were released.
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;  
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
 
 
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
 
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp;   
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp; Stops became allophonically voiced after a low tone.  Thus the distinction between /b/ and voiced /p/ was muddled.
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp;  Stops became allophonically geminated after a high tone. (????)
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp; 
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
<li>&nbsp;
</li>
 
 
</ol>
 
 
 
 
 
</pd></pr></pbody></pable>
 
 
 
 
 
</pd>
 
 
</pr></pbody></pable>
 
 
 
 
 
</pd><pd valign="top" bgcolor="#ffffff">
 
 
  </pd></pr></pbody></pable>


==See also==
==See also==
Line 1,620: Line 583:
==Notes==
==Notes==
[[Category:A priori conlangs]]
[[Category:A priori conlangs]]
[[Category:Languages of Teppala]]

Latest revision as of 05:25, 19 October 2024


Humans on the planet Teppala are confined to a single continent, Rilola, and its offshore islands. Thus all human languages can be traced back to a single starting point, and have many traits in common.

Historical distribution of Teppalan languages

Human civilization on planet Teppala peaked twice: first around 1700-2600AD, and then again around 3500-4200AD. During the first peak, the human population was concentrated on the south coast of the continent of Rilola, from 10°N to about 28°N. During the second peak, humans were more spread out, but the areas with the most political and military power were in the interior of the continent, though always along major rivers and lakes, ranging from about 30°N to 37°N in the eastern (older) areas of settlement and upwards to about 45°N in the far west.

After the second peak, human civilization entered a long decline, the population decreased, and languages with small populations went extinct.

During the peaks of human civilization, it was common for every religion to have its own language. Since religions were tied to political parties, almost every political party also had its own language. This is why many Teppalan languages have unusual names, such as the Gold language and the Moonshine language; these were named after political parties rather than ethnic groups.

Since political parties (and religions) coexisted with rival parties within the same ethnic group's nation, the boundaries of a given language often hinged on tiny differences such as speech registers or a set of unique vocabulary words used only members of a particular political party. When languages such as this coexisted, they tended to evolve in parallel directions, so that mutual intelligibility might persist among a pair of languages for hundreds of years.

However, democratic governments were very rare throughout Teppalan history, and many political parties, once in power, banned and persecuted members of rival parties. In these nations, the population really did consist of just a single ethnicity since anyone not claiming membership would be arrested or killed.

Traits common to all Teppalan languages

Phonology

  1. Bilabial consonants are very common, especially the stops p b and the nasal m.
  2. The commonest syllable shape is always CV, even in languages that allow dense consonant clusters and/or diphthongs and triphthongs.
  3. If a language has only one series of coarticulated consonants, they are labialized consonants.
  4. No language has more than six vowels. All vowels are one of these inventories:
    /a i u/
    /a ə i u/
    /a e i o u/
    /a e i o u ɨ/
    /a ə ɨ/
    Some differences in Romanization may appear such as writing a schwa as e or the high central vowel as schwa.
  5. Any consonant that can occur in syllable-final position can also occur in syllable-initial position.
  6. Languages are consonant-strong: consonants have greater effect on surrounding vowels than vowels have on consonants. For example, if all vowels are inherently unrounded, rounded allophones appear adjacent to labialized consonants.

Grammar

Grammatical traits common to all of the languages on the entire planet throughout all of recorded history are confined to negatives.

  • For example, no Teppalan language has or has ever had a definite or indefinite article.

Traits common to most Teppalan languages

Phonology

  1. The commonest stop in a language is usually /p/. However, in some languages, this is because /p/ stands alone whereas stops further back in the mouth are divided into several articulation types. For example, a language may have /p/ as its only bilabial stop but also have a distinction between a plain velar stop /k/ and an ejective /ḳ/; or there may be co-articulations such as palatalization or labialization associated with dorsal stops but not with bilabials.
  2. Languages with five or more phonemic vowels often do not permit diphthongs; those that do have a very small set. Thaoa is an outlier in that it has six phonemic vowels and several diphthongs. Note that rising diphthongs are generally parsed as a sequence of consonant + vowel since, in almost all Teppalan languages, there is no restriction on which vowels can follow an onset of [j] or [w].
  3. Dense consonant clusters do not appear, except in some languages where a certain vowel, usually /a/ or /ə/, is not distinguished at the phonemic level from silence. That is, some languages may always pronounce /tk/ as [tək], without the schwa actually being present as a phoneme.
  4. Syllables are commonly front-loaded, such that a sequence like /papsa/ is more likely to be pronounced [pa.psa] rather than *[pap.sa]. This happens most often when the first element of a cluster is lower on the sonority hierarchy than any following consonants, but in many languages, nasal-stop clusters such as /mp nt ŋk/ will also be front-loaded.
  5. On the continent of Rilola, the voiced velar stop /ġ/ (IPA /g/) is rarely used even in languages with a robust series of voiced stops. This is a long-standing trait and crosses language family boundaries. On the islands of Laba, however, /ġ/ is common.
  6. There are no minimal pairs between a diphthong and a sequence of the same two vowels. Thus diphthongs can be analyzed as allophones of vowel sequences.
  7. Voiceless obstruents occur more frequently than voiced ones. In some languages, /b/ or /d/ is the only voiced stop. In others, there are no voiced stops at all but the voiced velar fricative /g/ (IPA /ɣ/) takes on a stop allophone after a nasal or a high tone.
  8. There are often marginal consonant phonemes. These mostly arise from previously existing consonant clusters that were worn down. However, some marginal phonemes arise from sound changes affecting consonants that previously were more common, which survived in only a few phonemic environments. For example, in Khulls voiced stops survived a lenition shift only after a nasal. Later, the nasal sometimes disappeared, meaning that the voiced stops could no longer be analyzed as allophones of voiced fricatives. But they remained rare.
  9. It is common to have restrictions forbidding certain consonants to appear in certain parts of a word; for example, in Khulls /r/ cannot begin a word. Most languages allow only a small subset of their consonants to appear at the end of a word

Grammar

  • Polysynthesis is common, and it is nearly always fusional. It is common to find words with more morphemes than phonemes due to the prevalence of single-phoneme grammatical suffixes that represent two individual morphemes that at an earlier stage of the language were pronounced individually but combined into a new single sound after a sound change.
  • Pronouns are of limited usage, as verbs usually carry the relevant information about the person, number, and gender of the participants. In some languages, pronouns are entirely absent.

Gender

  • If there is any grammatical gender, feminine words in most semantic fields outnumber masculine ones.
  • Grammatical gender, if present, classifies people based on age and sex rather than just sex. The age categories are not firmly defined and can be used metaphorically. There are often several age categories for children, but all adults share just a single age category.
  • Many languages have a consonant-based gender system; if not grammaticalized, associated more weakly due to persistence of previously grammaticalized morphemes and contact with nearby languages.
  • Mixed gender categories are often present; a man and a woman, referred to as a unit, will take an epicene gender rather than having one gender overrule the other. If there is no epicene, a group containing both males and females will be described with words in one of the feminine genders.
  • Gender and animacy, if present, can be inherited by nouns describing syntactically inanimate objects, by borrowing from a parent object. That is, a man's arm will be animate (and masculine), and so will his books or any other possessions.

Structure of words

  • Most languages have a fixed word order, with SOV word order by far the most common. Anomalous constructions within SOV languages may require additional marking on certain words, usually those found near the beginning of the sentence.
  • Suffixes are nearly universal; infixes are common; prefixes are rare and usually confined to a single part of speech (for example, in Andanese, the only prefixes are the noun classifiers).
  • Verbs are generally the longest words in a sentence. Verbs are heavily inflected even in languages where noun inflection is absent or relatively skimpy.

Parts of speech

  • There are no adjectives or adverbs. Verbs are used instead of these. In some languages, even the nouns can be analyzed as a subset of the verbs.
  • Pronouns play only a minor role in the language, and some languages lack pronouns altogether, instead using nouns and verbs with person markers.
  • Person markers on nouns, denoting their possessor, are common. Languages that lose this system often redevelop it from grammatically unrelated words later on.

List of very early sound changes

Many minor languages have been wiped from this list; some are recorded on their own articles, but others are only in the edit history.

Pre-Mapi changes

Proto-Greater-Laban (18343 BC) to Mapi (year 14000 BC)

The Pre-Proto-Macro-Hyper-Greater-Laban (PGL) language had a consonant inventory of /pʷ p mʷ m hʷ w t c s č š j k kʷ ŋ ŋʷ h l r ř/ and a vowel inventory of /a i u ə/. Schwa is usually spelled "e". /ŋ/ was allophonically [g] and the other nasals could become stops occasionally in word-initial position. At first, "c" was homophonous with the cluster /ts/, but later /ts/ was dragged down to dental and /c/ remained alveolar.

See Primordial scratchpad for details on the language.

  1. The primordial final nasal -/n/ disappeared, but lengthened any preceding vowel (including schwa).
  2. The schwa vowel ə disappeared in all positions, creating new consonant clusters.
  3. Consonant clusters like hp ht hk flipped to put the stop first, as in ph th kh.
  4. Possibly also /ki/ > /ć/ or even just /k/.
  5. Word-final h disappeared, and also lengthened any preceding vowel. Thus, the final syllable /-hə/ had vanished completely. However, note that word-internal /h/ that was at the end of a syllable had survived this change.
  6. Syllable-final -p became the glottal stop ʔ, except before a "weak" sound such as /h/ or before a vowel-initial syllable.
  7. The consonant clusters ʔs ʔš ʔts ʔč became c č c č.
  8. The consonant clusters ks kš kts kč kh sk šk čk tsk hk all become kh.
  9. The consonant clusters ts th st ht all became th.
  10. The consonant clusters ph sp šp čp tsp hp' all become ph.
  11. Clusters of two voiceless stops were resolved in favor of the second stop, except for /pk/ and its kin.
  12. The aspirated nasal mh became h.
  13. The labialized stops kʷ kʷh changed to pʷ pʷh.
  14. The aspirated stops pʷh ph th kh changed to hʷ h h x respectively. (possibly use f f f x)
  15. The affricates c č became s š.
  16. The glottal fricative h disappeared unconditionally, leaving vowel hiatus. This included the deaspiration of to w along with any other remaining sequences such as /nh/. VOWEL HIATUS STAGE 1!!!!
  17. The vowel sequences ai au contracted into new vowels ē ō, but retained the glide if another vowel followed.
  18. The identical vowel sequences aa ii uu became ā ī ū. The vowel system was thus /a i u ā ē ī ō ū/ and a tiny bit of remaining schwa.
  19. The vowel ō was shortened to o, but ē remained long.
  20. Before another vowel, the sequences aw iw ow uw āw ēw īw ūw became ō ū ō ū ō ē ī ū.
  21. Before another vowel, the vowel u changed to w.
  22. Voiced stops disappeared unconditionally, leaving vowel hiatus. VOWEL HIATUS STAGE 2!!!!! Note that sequewnces like /sua/ now contrasted with /swa/ from the earlier change.
  23. The labialized fricative sequences sw šw xw hw changed to h. Note that there was still an /x/, from earlier /kh/. THus there was a contrast between /h/ vs /x/, but neither of them had labialized forms.
  24. The short vowels e o merged as ə (usually spelled "e"). The long vowels ē ō merged as ə̄ (usually spelled "ē").

Pre-Tapilula changes

NOTE
Some languages in this section are reconstructed internally, meaning that they have the smallest possible phonology capable of developing into the phonologies of its immediate daughter languages. Marginal phonemes can thus be inserted arbitrarily into any language provided that they merge into one of the phonemes of the daughter language.
NOTE
Many ideas deleted. See history for info.

There were 3 central vowels: /a ɜ ɨ/. Further back in time, each of these three could be preceded by the palatal glide /y/, even after a consonant, but later the palatal glide disappeared after labials & labialized consonants, and merged with the other consonants to form palataloids. This happened without creating any new vowels.

The daughter languages are Silatibarra (8000 BC; sometimes just called "Southeast Laban"), proto-paleo-Andanese (PPAnd), and proto-macro-Haswarabic (PMH).

All of the labialized consonants were "stops" (nasal or oral). The labialized nasals /mʷ ŋʷ/ may have been pronounced as voiced stops (as in Khulls). The lack of the expected */xʷ hʷ/ was due to a late change in the parent language, and in many ways the /h/ patterned as if it were the labialized counterpart of /x/.

Syllables were CVC at maximum, and there were limits on the coda ... a preference for nasal codas if there is one. PoA was not contrastive before another consonant. The final stops allowed were /t, kw/ and possibly /ć/. There were thus no syllables like /kʷya/ or even /pya/.

Note that Mapi is the parent language of the Paleo-Andanese languages, so called because they are the ancestral languages of the people who, 14000 years later, began to speak Tapilula and later Andanese.

Stops & frics early on came to be voiced when occurring after a nasal.

Mapi (14000 BC) to Primordial MRCA (11000 BC)

The grammar of this language may have had infixes, because they are present in both Owl and Tapilula. The MRCA of those is in the middle of this chart, ~11000 BC. Nonetheless, Tapilula's infixes can be "recovered" (biological sense) from suffixes and therefore Mapi can have essentially any grammar.

init:

Rounded labials:      pʷ mʷ    w
Plain bilabials:      p  m
Alveolars:            t  n  s  l  r
Postalveolars:        č  ň  š  ł  ř
Palatals:             ć  ń  ś  
Velars:               k  ŋ  x
Labiovelars:          kʷ ŋʷ 
Glottals:                   h


  1. The fricatives š ś h shifted to y y Ø unconditionally . This created vowel sequences of /aa aɜ aɨ ɜa ɜɜ ɜɨ ɨa ɨɜ ɨɨ/ most of which occurred as the only vocoid in a word since most roots had been bisyllabic.
  2. The sequences ɨa ɨɜ ɨɨ shifted to ʕa ʕɜ ʕɨ, where /ʕ/ is a voiced stop after a nasal and otherwise a fricative(?). Thus prenasalized stops were created. rule3 might not be needed if we keep h
  3. The sequences ɨwa ɨwɜ ɨwɨ shifted to ʷa ʷɜ ʷɨ.
  4. The sequences ɨya ɨyɜ ɨyɨ shifted to ʲa ʲɜ ʲɨ . Thus new "mismatched" palatalized (pʲ mʲ ) and labialized (tʷ nʷ sʷ lʷ rʷ xʷ ) consonants were created, and they were a full set.
  5. The sequences aa aɜ ɜa ɜɜ shifted to a: a: ɜ: ɜ:. aɨ ɜɨ shifted to ɜ: ɜ: as well.
  6. mʷ nʷ ŋʷ > mbʷ nd ŋġʷ. These may have also been the reflexes of /mw nw ŋw/, but the syllable boundaries would have kept them apart even so. That is, a syllable-straddling /m-w/ would turn into /m-mbʷ/.
  7. Fricatives became weakened between vowels, and in syllable-final position: s x sʷ xʷ changed to h h hʷ hʷ.
  8. The cluster ts shifted to s.
  9. The labial consonants p pʷ pʲ became B w y.


And the vowel inventory was probably

SHORT VOWELS
a   ɜ   ɨ
LONG VOWELS AND FALLING DIPHTHONGS
ā  ai  au
ɜ̄  ɜi  ɜu
ɨ̄   ī   ū

See Repilian languages.

Primordial (11000 BC) to Southeast Laban (8000 BC)

  1. The fricatives h hʷ (including the newly generated ones) in syllable-final position voiced to Ø w. ś had been shifted to /y/ earlier. Thuis frics no longer occured at end of tillable.
  2. rh > h.
  3. > mb.
  4. Possibly, also hʷ h ś > vless stops when after a nasal, but they later become voiced in Tapilula.
  5. The voiceless stops č ć shifted to s₂ s₃ (cover symbol: $) between vowels.
  6. Most postalveolar consonants trapped in final positions become plain alveolars: č ñ ł become t n l. Note that there were no longer any palatals in the language.
  7. ć ń ś > s₄ ň s₅.
  8. Probably > w; maybe ł > y.
  9. s₅ s > s₁.
  10. Probably > .
  11. The sequences ɜ̄ ɜi ɨ̄ ī shifted to ē ē ī ī unconditionally.
  12. The high vowel ɨ shifted to ū before a labialized consonant in a closed syllable. (This is a dummy shift to make it easier to understand changes that happen in Tapilula, but it may be of use in other daughter languages as well.)
  13. Final after a labialized consonant shifted to u. Final after a palatalized consonant shifted to i. These are both dummy shifts as well.


And the vowel inventory may have been /a ɜ ɨ/ for short vowels and /ā ē ī ō ū/ for long vowels.

The prenasals contrast with syllable-straddling sequences of nasal + *voiceless* stop, and with nasal + prenasal (that is, /mmb/ etc).

Southeast Laban (8000 BC) to Tapilula (500 BC)

Rounded labials:                  w      mbʷ
Plain bilabials:          m              mb  B                        
Alveolars:            t   n       l   r  nd  $
Rounded alveolars:                   (rʷ)
Postalveolars:        č   ň       λ   ř  
Palatals:                         y  
Velars:               k   ŋ   h              G 
Labiovelars:          kʷ      hʷ         ŋġʷ

Tentative consonants are in parens. Note that /nʷ/ is not inherited from MRCA, so it's possible that /tʷ/ > /kʷ/ > /p/ and that all /tʷ/ in Tapilula is secondary just as all nʷ must be.

There may be a few other consonants not listed here resulting from the creation of new labialized and palatalized consonants in the parent language.

The cover symbol B can be used for "a consonant that needs to disappear" to Ø since there is so much vowel hiatus required to get things the way they are in Tapilula. The cover symbol $ can likewise be used for any consonant that ultimately ends up as /h/, since there is so much /h/ in Tapilula as well. Lastly, G indicates any consonant that ends upo as /g/, though this might be adqeuately covered by grammatical reanalysis from hiatus and not need a new consonant.

The B consonant was most likely just /b/, but the capital letter spelling leaves open the possibility that it traces back to more than one phoneme and that these were still distinct at a fairly late stage of development, just as $ corresponds to a set of phonemes that merged only very late.

The vowel inventory was

a ɜ ɨ
ā ɜ̄   ē ī ō ū

These are spelled inconsistently however, and there may have also been a fourth pair of long vowels for primordial /ai au/.

  1. Final short ɜ ɨ shifted to ɨ Ø unconditionally. Here, "final" refers to polysyllabic words only.
    It is possible that at this time, a plural -ɨw (later /u/) became generalized to all consonant stems, from original ɨ-stems. It is not clear what the original plural would have been from the pre-existing consonant stems.
  2. In at least some combinations, word-internal ɨ also disappeared to Ø, thus allowing the later creation of word-initial /ḳ/ and, through contraction of -ɨB-, a source of new labialized consonants. Alternatively, this shift could be united with the shift below in which /ɜy ɨy ɜw ɨw/ become /i i u u/.
    This created new clusters, which in some cases collapsed to new single consonants. All were rare:
    1. bh shifted to p. (If B represents more than one phoneme, it will need to be separated from the true /b/ by this shift.)
  3. mB > mb.
  4. Word-final clusters became simple, losing their second element.
    Note that this implies there will be little or no final /ə/ in Tapilula.
  5. Probably all $G > $.
  6. Intervocalically, the geminate nasals mm nn ňň ŋŋ shifted to the prenasalized voiced stops mb nd ňǯ ŋġ .
  7. An h or after a nasal also switched to a voiced stop, thus creating a prenasalized voiced (not voiceless) consonant.
  8. The cluster mB now also shifted to mb.
    This is important much later on, as it allows epenthetic /b/ for vowel-initial words when padded by a 1st person marker (later extended to places where it would not be etymologically sound).
    This also means that /mB/ and /mh/ have identical reflexes and that analogy may have begun very early. Also, note that /mbʲ/ later just becomes /mb/, so even the new /h/ that arises later on can sometimes be treated as B since this /h/ can come from /gi/ before a vowel. Importantly, the 1P>2P marker in Tapilula is /nambə/, just as if it were from /nam/ + /gə/ instead of /nam/ + /hə/. Essentially, the passive morpheme vanishes.
  9. The sequences pm tn čň kŋ shifted to mm nn ňň ŋŋ.
  10. A prenasalized voiced stop following a closed syllable became denasalized; any such preceding coda became voiced. This was allophonic, so that for example [bb] was still underlyingly /pmb/. Or, maybe they were devoiced.
  11. Word-initial approximant w became .
    NOTE ON POLITICS: This is 3770 BC, a point of no political significance, but where the language suddenly began changing very rapidly instead of very slowly.
  12. Before a vowel, the sequences ɜy ɨy ɜw ɨw changed into i i u u.
    NOTE, another way of saying this (more in line with how i write now) is that /ē ī/ merged into a new short /i/ before hiatus; the /u/ shift may have been separate.
    the resulting sequences are treated as new coarticulates... also, the high vowel probably disappears before some consonants, creating clusters such as /kl/, and these can contrast labialization.
  13. The vowel ɨ became u before a labial in a closed syllable, or after a labialized consonant. That is, ʷɨ > ʷu.
  14. The vowels a ɜ before a labial in a closed syllable very likely became shifted to o o around this time, because it cannot be accounted for by the later shift that does almost the same thing. By contrast, /ʷa ʷɜ/ did NOT shift to /o/.
  15. The vowel ɨ became i when adjacent of a palatal in either direction (though in compounds, this did not always apply progressively).
  16. Possibly at this time shifted to p.
  17. Remaining B shifted to Ø (not /g/).
  18. The labialized consonants w mbʷ kʷ hʷ ŋġʷ delabialized to Ø mb k h ŋġ before /i/.
    try saying püa etc and remmeber syllable integrity ... earlier, there was a rule a bit below this one that would have these going to /w/, essentially deleting the /i/.
  19. The clusters tl tr shifted to kl kr. Other similar shifts of clusters almost certainly followed. The cluster kt shifted to k (not /ḳ/).
  20. Before a vowel, the sequences ti ki shifted to č.
  21. The clusters kl kr merged as . Thus, in handheld object prefixes, /ətr/ > /oḳ/.
    The Play word /see/ requires that ml also shift to (and later to /mb/). If this shift does not happen, the Play word would end up as /seu/ instead.
  22. Syllabic nasals were created: im ɨm um > , ɨn > , and ɨŋ > ŋ̇.
    NOTE THAT THIS REQUIRES THERE TO BE A SECOND SOURCE OF /um/, perhaps implying that coda /ŋʷ/ still existed at this point.
    Possibly the sequences mbṁ ndṅ ŋġŋ̇ and others like them shift to simple syllabics ṁ ṅ ŋ̇.
  23. The palatal approximant y was changed to ʕ in all positions. Between vowels, this became silent (Ø) but used to show diphthongs' separation. Thus the palatalized consonants pʲ mbʲ mʲ became the pharyngealized clusters pʕ mbʕ mʕ .
  24. The postalveolar nasal ň changed to ŋ. However, ndʲ here became nd, not something such as /nġ/.
  25. The voiced approximants λ ř r changed to y.
    It is possible that there was a shift of /iy/ > /ig/, which would account for the name of the religion beginning with yìga- in Tapilula. Alternatively this could be due to some sort of syllable split, such that /yiya/ came to be seen as /yii/ + /a/, and all double vowels collapsed. The /g/ would be a regular insertion to break up hiatus in such a case.
  26. The vowel sequences aɨ ɨa changed to ɜu ʷɜ. Note that the /ɨa/ covers all such sequences that did not previously have an intervening /y/ sound ... therefore, this was probably allophonically a back vowel even if it were not always rounded.
  27. The sequences ʷa ʷɜ changed to ʷo.
    It needs to be that /kʷ/ > /pʷ/ > /p/ had taken place by this time, since otherwise there would be no contrast of /p/ vs /pʷ/.
    This also implies pk (since it was really kʷk) shifting to pp. This does not rule out a new /pk/ arising from new compounds.
  28. yʷ čʷ > y č, thus the word for hand is just "yò". This did not affect any $ that was just /s/ by this time.
  29. n$ʷ (when not /nčʷ/ as above) shifted to ntʷ (not to /ndʷ/). Likewise the sequence m$ shifted to mp.
  30. The sequences au ɜu now both become o. ai ɜi changed to ɛ unconditionally.
  31. The true mid vowel ɜ, in a syllable adjacent to an /ɛ/ or /o/, changed to match that vowel. Some double schwa words also changed, e.g. mɜčɜ > močo "fire".
  32. The "clear" labialized consonants kʷ ŋʷ became the rounded bilabials pʷ mʷ. (There was little or no /kʕʷ/.)
  33. The pharyngealized nasals mʕ mʕʷ shifted to mb mbʷ.
    This may incorporate early shifts like /mh/ at morpheme boundaries. Thus for example, hàga "fairy" could actually be /hàmba/.
  34. The voiceless fricatives h hʷ became voiced to g w unconditionally.
  35. Remaining ɜ ɨ in open syllables shift to ə i, respectively. This new schwa vowel is IPA /ɨ/but is spelled "ə" or "y", since it is never ambiguous with IPA /j/.
  36. The voiceless affricate č shifted to h. (This probably had shifted to /s/ during depalatalization. if not, /hy/ etc also switched.)
    Gold /hìga ~ hìa/ showing up as /hà/, even if only in one word, relies on this assumption, and there are no words that rely on the sequence /hia/ existing in pre-Gold as it has no specific reflexes.
  37. Syllables *preceding* heavy syllables became LOW tone.

Thus the final phonology was:

Rounded bilabials:     pʷ      mʷ  mbʷ mhʷ~hʷ  w
Spread bilabials:      p   pʕ  m   mb  mf ~f  (Ø)         
Alveolars:             t       n   nd          l
Rounded alveolars:     tʷ      nʷ  ndʷ     
Velars:                k   ḳ   ŋ   ŋġ      h   g

The (f) was pronounced [mʰ] in some dialects. Pharyngealization consonants can be spelled with voiced stop letters.

Dreamlandic changes

This change occurred in the Lenian languages but is not on that page because the dictionary is not synced properly with the MRCA.

  1. The nasals mʷ m n nʷ ŋ become prenasalized voiceless stops mpʷ mp nt ntʷ ŋk when facing a labial.

Proto-Dreamlandic retains the final primordial nasals even before voiceless consonants, and also gains new voiceless prenasal clusters from plain nasals before labials. As of 07:07, 1 May 2022 (PDT)~ this error is fixed in the dictionary for word-medial clusters (that is, some /p t k/ has been replaced with /mp nt ŋk/), but not for word-final codas. This is because the codas are not indexed in the MRCA dictionary either; they are only derived when a word needs to appear in Dreamlandic.

MRCA to Nuclear Tapilula (700 BC to 0 AD)

  1. The nasals m mʷ became f hʷ before a vowel facing another labial.
  2. Possibly, g shifted to Ø at the head of a closed syllable, at least when in initial position. This could explain the otherwise unpreditcable g~Ø variation in Gold and Andanese, but not why they sometimes disagree with each other, or why it also drops out in medial position in a few words (unless these are all compounds). Therefore, even if this rule is assumed, analogy is required. In Dreamlandic, the reflexes of /g/ and /Ø/ are identical, so no explanation is needed.
  3. All syllable codas were deleted. The resulting ephemeral voiced stop ġ shifted to the fricative g.
    The most likely order for the sound change above was:
    1. The final stops p t k shifted to ʔ, which carries the stress and behaves as a HIGH tone.
    2. The final nasals m n ŋ merged as n and word-internal clusters became homorganic.
    3. The lateral l disappeared in the coda as well, bringing a high tone.
    4. The final nasal n disappeared, bringing a high tone.
    Under this analysis, Dreamlandic would do all shifts but the last. It is possible that nasal POA was preserved in early Dreamlandic, however.
  4. The glottalized stops pʕ pʕʷ kʕ changed to b bʷ ḳ unconditionally.
  5. The rounded labials pʷ bʷ mʷ became plain labials p b m .

The consonant inventory was:

Rounded bilabials:                     hʷ  w
Spread bilabials:      p       m   b   f  (Ø)
Alveolars:             t       n   d       l
Rounded alveolars:     tʷ      nʷ  dʷ         
Velars:                k   ḳ   ŋ       h   g

Post-Tapilula changes

Tapilula to Gold (year 1900)

Alternate names: Medium, Walking Girls, Wolf in Wool, Soft Hands, Slingshot, Broken Shields, Ukieipi, G̣ʷidiʕìləs

Although Gold is the proper name of the language, the Gold party survives for thousands of years, and therefore these names can be used to give a more precise definition of the stage of the language being referred to.

Note on politics: It is not clear whether the Tapilula language split apart in 0 AD or in 500 AD. If 500, the various Subumpamese languages are almost independent branches since they begin diverging not long after 600 AD. However, Sub and Gold share much more in common than either of them does with Andanese, despite being supposedly only 100 years closer together. So perhaps the true date is 0 after all.

Tapilula to Gold (proper)

  1. The aspirated velar stop k became č before the vowel /i/. If another vowel followed, the /i/ disappeared. This happened even if the /i/ was accented.
  2. When a "velaroid" consonant (/k ḳ ŋ h g l/) followed an accented high tone vowel, the vowel metathesized, leaving a closed syllable. Thus, for example, /àli/ > /ail/. These closed syllables were all high-toned, and are thus written without tone marks. Thus, for example, aa implies àa. Later, daughter languages introduced tone contrasts and independent sequences.
  3. A schwa before another vowel in any syllable disappeared. Thus əa əe əi əo əu əə shifted to a e i o u ə. This happened in both open and closed syllables.
  4. The sequences iu and ui shifted to ə̄.
  5. The double-vowel sequences aa ee ii oo uu əə shifted to the single vowels a e i o u ə in closed syllables only.
  6. The sequences ii uu əə (which now occurred only in open syllables) shifted to əi əu ə. This means that the /ə/ and "same vowel" infixes had switched places in some constructions, but this did not lead to confusion, at least in the four-vowel languages such as Play, because the infixes typically did not occur in their free forms.
  7. The sequences ie uo shifted to i u in open syllables only.
  8. The sequences ai ei oi merged as ei; the sequences au eu ou merged as ou.
    NOTE ON POLITICS: SUBUMPAMESE CAN BE CONSIDERED TO BREAK OFF HERE, AS THIS IS THE LAST OF THE SHARED CHANGES. (that is, there were only A FEW of them).
  9. The sequences ea eə shifted to ee; meanwhile, oa oə became oo. Then, shifted to aa. Thus, the sequences /ee aa oo/ once again appeared in both open and closed syllables. Note, however, that much inherited /ea oa/ had participated in grammatical alternations with /əa/, which had become a simple /a/ by this time, and this is the form that was usually generalized.
  10. The sequences ia ie io iə shifted to ī . Then ua ue uo uə shifted to ū.
  11. In absolute final position, syllable-final ŋ changed to n. (But see below.)
  12. Accented vowel-initial syllables gained a pharyngeal ʕ as an onset. Then the clusters nʕ kʕ shifted to g ḳ.
  13. After long vowels, all consonants became voiced. Also, consonants occurring after initial vowels also became voiced. This created the new consonants v ǯ . Thus, final -h in words like hʷīh became -g. However, analogy made it so that the change was confined to open syllables in most words. This sound change did not affect diphthongs. There was no voiced velar stop, as all four velars simply shifted to fricatives.
  14. After initial unstressed /u/, all consonants other than palatals became labialized. This change extended even to clusters. Because of the voicing rule, however, all of these consonants were voiced. bʷ vʷ merged as w.
  15. In word-initial position, the six vowels a e i o u ə were deleted to Ø unconditionally.
    A few words began with double vowels, but these were grammatical alternants of V-/g/-V, V: (that is, inherited long vowels), and /g/-V-(g)-V pairlets, meaning that in these few words, the vowel deletion could affect both vowels, only the outermost vowel, or neither of the two vowels. Then, because of this same analogy, the words that now began with g came to be pronounced with Ø onset when following a word that ended with a consonant, and for some speakers also in isolation, but never when following a word ending in a vowel. In both Leaper and Play, this shift was quickly undone, as it had never been phonemic. In Thaoa, it may have become firm, but with the expense of now having no words beginning in /g/.
    Sometimes root-initial vowels were retained due to classifier prefixes; here, too, analogy played an important role. For example, the food item prefix mi- cleaved onto the root, and ceased to be seen as a classifier prefix, meaning that these new roots began with consonants after all. This was helped by the fact that only a few such roots began with vowels, even including those that had come to be pronounced with /g/.
  16. All schwas and diphthongs became low tone.
  17. Labialized consonants lost their labialization when occuring after another labial or labialized consonant.
  18. After a stressed syllable, intervocalic ʕ ʕʷ became g gʷ. This is due to reanalysis, not a true sound change.
  19. The glottal fricatives h hʷ became velar; there was no spelling change.


NOTE ON POLITICS: THIS IS WHERE THAOA & TARYTE SPLIT OFF (YR 1085 AD)

The consonant inventory was:

                       BASIC                         LABIALIZED


Bilabials:             p   b   m   f   v                     mʷ      w  
Alveolars:             t   d   n       l             tʷ  dʷ  nʷ            
Postalveolars:         č   ǯ           y                       
Velars:                k       ŋ   h   g   ḳ                 ŋʷ  hʷ  gʷ

The vowel inventory was

Short vowels:          a  e  i  o  u  ə
Long vowels:           ā  ē  ī  ō  ū 
Falling diphthongs:      ae ei ao ou
                            əi    əu

The long vowels /ā ē ō/ can be spelled aa ee oo, but the high vowels /ī ū/ are usually not, because /i u/ before another vowel would indicate a glide.

There were also eo oe in historical compounds. These would not have occurred in individual roots because of ancient vowel harmony rules.

Nuclear Gold

Gold's unconditional shift of /f v b/ > /s d d/ sets it apart from all related languages. Note that it requires /mb/ > /nd/ as well but yet /ṁb/ (syllabic) remained.

  1. All remaining e o shifted to a. This included elements of diphthongs. Resulting /aa/ was spelled as /ā/; thus, ae ao eo oe ee oo all shifted to ā. Thus there were only four vowels. In some cases, a short vowel was formed instead due to grammatical analogy, though unlike the /ea oa/ > /əa/ shift above, there was no etymological basis for this shift.
  2. The velar fricatives h g were rounded to hʷ gʷ before /u/. The labiodental /f/ did not shift, and by this time was likely already [fʲ] to maximize distinction. If /v/ existed, it did not shift either.
  3. The plain labials f v b shifted to s d d. This happened by intermediate palatalization, and therefore the sequences /fj vj bj/, which were very rare, also shifted to /s d d/. The sequence /fʷ/ remained, often spelled ħʷ, but this quickly merged with the existing /hʷ/.
    This apparently includes a shift of bh to dh, meaning that the devoicing of aspirates did not happen until the Gold language split apart.
  4. In absolute final position, syllable-final h changed to s.
  5. The postalveolar affricates č ǯ shifted to š ž.

The consonant inventory was:

Bilabials:             p           m      (ʕ)  w   mʷ            
Alveolars:             t       d   n   s       l   nʷ      tʷ  dʷ
Postalveolars:                         š   ž   y                                  
Velars:                k   ḳ       ŋ   h   g   gʷ  ŋʷ  hʷ  kʷ      ḳʷ

The vowel inventory was

Short vowels:      a  i  u  ə
Long vowels:       ā  ī  ū
Diphthongs:          ai au
                     əi əu

The consonants kʷ ḳʷ only occur as the realizations of clusters like /k/ + /gʷ/, and thus never occurred word-initially, whereas the labialized nasals occurred only word-initially (or morpheme-initially).

Notes on Play and Leaper

Note that both Play and Leaper often show reflexes implying a shift of /hi gi/ > /s d/ between vowels, but in different places; this change was grammatically conditioned and did not occur within the history of the Gold parent language. This shift was later analogized with the preexisting true sound change of f v > s d to replace intervocalic /s si/ of any origin with /d d/ when the preceding vowel was originally long. When a vowel did not follow, there was no shift. The shift also did not occur after a vowel that became long in Leaper but was a diphthong in Gold.

Note that this implies that there was no shift of /sʲ/ to /s/, and therefore likely no shift of /lʲ/ > /l/.

Play's reflex of /ll/ is /ww/ (the document is not on this wiki, unlike almost all others, so I have to put the notes here).

Gold (1900) to Khulls (4700)

Alternate names: Kuroras, Leaper

Alternate names: Khulls

(Dummy edit link)

This language was originally spoken in AlphaLeap.

  1. The velar fricatives h hʷ came to be spelled x xʷ.
    As /tanči/ "wine" demonstrates, a preceding coda /n/ did *not* assimilate to the /x/. This also implies nx shifted to nt in Leaper but not in Play; it is possible that the shift was pre-Gold, but was then undone in Play due to its close attention to morphology, but it would make more sense to have the shift appear after the separation.
  2. tʷ dʷ nʷ shifted to tl dl nl.
  3. When not occurring after a labialized consonant, the vowels ŭ ù ū shifted to ɜ̆ ɜ̀ ɜ̄. (This is spelled differently from schwa to ease confusion.) This was a low back vowel comparable to IPA [ɤ].
  4. The sequences ə əi əu (all syllables with inherited /ə/ were toneless) shifted to ʉ ɜi ɜu.
  5. In a closed syllable, the new ʉ vowel disappeared and created a syllabic consonant. In an open syllable, ʉ changed to ʷ, thus labializing the preceding consonant and then disappearing. Where /ʉ/ collapsed, stress shifted syllables to the nearest adjacent one. This tone was mid-tone (ă), which was sometimes called the low tone since it behaved as such when joined to any other morpheme that carried stress. Thus all morphemes that had once contained a schwa came to be pronounced entirely with low tones.
    Note that any syllabic formed here always assimilated to a following consonant because they arose from a non-syllabic nasal, which had already been assimilated to a following consonant. By contrast, the primordial syllabic nasals /ṁ ṅ ŋ̇/ still did not assimilate, and thus words like /mṅpà/ "to ask" still existed.
  6. Sequences like aʕa became pharyngealized vowels; these could still have tones, but later all pharyngealized tones merged with each other except for sandhi effects. Pharyngealized vowels are spelled â ; though there is only one pharyngealized surface tone, pharyngealized vowels exhibited different sandhi effects depending on their origin, and this is not reflected in the Romanization. Note that /iʕV/ did not create pharyngealization, but /uʕV əʕV/ did, and both caused labialization.
  7. The cluster sg shifted to x. This is actually a tone shift, since it had been [x] all along but was previously abound to a high tone.
  8. The clusters pʷn kʷn shifted to pʷt kʷt.
    Note that this shift, needed to process the placename Gatupəna, is unsatisfactory, and would not help explain what would happen to the ejective cluster /ḳʷn/. Syllabic nasals are a possible answer, since the only clusters considered unwieldy are stop+nasal (so /pʷl/ etc were fine). This would produce the forbidden clusters /pṁ tṅ kŋ̇/ and these would probably lose the stop and become high tones.
  9. After a high tone, the voiced stop d shifted to . This includes a shift of dl to ṭl after a high tone as well.
  10. The clusters mh nh ŋh dh became mp nt ŋk t, except that replaces /nt/ before any /i/. These clusters were often morphologically /s/ + a voiced consonant, but the [h] pronunciation is actually the more archaic one.
  11. After the vowel [u] (any tone, any length), k ḳ in a syllable coda became coarticulated labial-velar stops kp ḳṗ . This change also took place after the /ʷ/ that had replaced earlier schwa, since this was behaving as an allophone of /u/. Likewise, it took place after /au/, but not /ɜ̄/, even though both ended up as /ō/ later on.
  12. After a syllabic nasal, the final stops k ḳ (which was the only ones that did occur) changed to match the position of the nasal. However, these were written with the letters for "kp ḳṗ".
  13. The voiced coronal stops d dʲ dʷ became r ž gʷ. The sequence rl (always from Gold /dʷ/) became ll, which in word-initial position then changed to a simple l.
  14. In word-initial position, r shifted to l.
  15. Labialized coronals became velar.
  16. Labialization disappeared before any syllabic consonant.
    This shift makes it possible to interpret the syllabic consonants as sequences of short low-tone /u/ + C. Previously, this would have failed because there was a contrast between /ʷC/ and /ʷuC/.
  17. The sequences mmṡ ŋŋṡ shifted to mpṡ ŋkṡ.
  18. The sequences gp gṗ shifted to kp ḳṗ.
  19. In unstressed position after a vowel, the syllabic consonants ṁ ṅ ŋ̇ ḷ ṡ shifted to plain consonants m n ŋ l s. Thus unstressed closed syllables were created.
  20. Final raised the preceding vowel to a high tone à (á if it was long) and then disappeared, though it left an allophonic glottal stop in some positions. Then, final k disappeared and changed the preceding vowel to the long high tone á. The surface tone change did not apply to pharyngealized vowels, but the sandhi effects did. Thus there were two pharyngealized tones .... both pronounced the same, but with different effects on surrounding unstressed syllables.
  21. The labialized fricative šʷ became ħʷ . The ħ is a spelling convention to distinguish it from /x/. The cluster gʷš (which had always been [gʷšʷ] because of syllable metrics) most likely became ħʷ as well, not /xʷ/. The /sg/ > /x/ shift above is different from this because /sg/ had never been phonetically realized as such.
    NOTE ON POLITICS: The Proto-Moonshine language breaks off here. (Year 3958) The tropical survivor language must have also broken off shortly afterward; what remains is for the language of the ruling class of Baeba Swamp.

Post-Moonshine changes

All of these changes take place in just 800 years, despite the list being nearly as long as that for the preceding 2,000 years.

  1. The diphthongs ai au shifted to ē ō unconditionally. Then aiʕ auʕ became ê ô. Note that the sequence /uʕ/ was distinct from a coda /ʕʷ/.
  2. The diphthongs ɜi ɜu shifted to ĕ ū unconditionally. If pharyngealized forms existed, they followed the rule above.
  3. The sequences ya yɜ (on all tones) shifted to ye of the same tone. Later, all /ye/ became /e/, but this was not phonemic because of intervening consonant changes. Note that this does NOT include /yau yɜu/, which had escaped the change by shifting to /yō yū/. This /ō/ is distinct from the one that forms below.
  4. The mid vowels ɜ̆ ɜ̀ ɜ̄ shifted to ŏ ò ō unconditionally. Likewise, any remaining unstressed ɜ became o.
  5. The velar-palatal sequences ky ḳy ŋy hy xy gy shifted to č č n̆ š š ž.
  6. Nasal consonants followed by /y/ hardened to prenasals: my n̆y became mby n̆ǯy . (This includes the reflexes of /ny/ and /ŋy/.) These later became stops.
  7. After a high tone, the fricatives x xʷ ħ ħʷ shifted to k kʷ q qʷ.
  8. sl>q, which is an allophone of /h/. This shift is essentially a restating of a consequence of the above, since there never was an /sl/, but only a coarticulated /lh/ cluster. Likewise, posttonic ħ ħʷ (spelling here used for emphasis) shifted to q qʷ.
  9. The voiceless bilabial stops p pʷ shifted to h hʷ except after a high tone. The plain /p/ had a brief intermediate of /ɸ/ but this stage lasted mere years before shifting to /h/. The labialized stop shifted directly. This shift excludes /kp/ and any other context in which the stops were part of a cluster; note that since the high tone always ended in a glottal stop, this environment can be considered to be a cluster as well.
    NOTE THAT THE SPELLING OF /h/ as ħ is for clarity only, because in many names, /x/ is spelled with the plain "h".
  10. The sequences ly hy (the latter only from /py/) shifted to λ š.
  11. The clusters ml nl changed to mbl ndl, thus restoring voiced stops to a marginal phonemic position.
  12. The labialized consonants mʷ ŋʷ changed to mbʷ ŋġʷ .
  13. The remaining clusters tl ṭl shifted to `l . This is a plain /l/ but makes the preceding vowel high-toned.
  14. The sound /l/ disappeared after any stop, even over a morpheme boundary.
  15. The voiced prenasals mbʷ mb nd nǯ ŋġʷ shifted to plain voiced stops bʷ b d ǯ ġʷ. There may have been an extremely rare plain voiced velar stop, arising only from the sequence /ŋ̇l/, which would have changed to /ŋġl/, then to /ŋġ/, and finally to /ġ/. Note that the original velar nasal must be syllabic for it to occur before another consonant.
  16. The coarticulated stops kp ḳṗ shifted to p ṗ. (If there ever was a voiced /ġb/, it too would shift.)
  17. The voiced velar fricative g came to be pronounced as a voiced stop ġ asfter a high tone. This was allophonic, and occurred at least a thousand years after the shift of /d/ > /ṭ/. Thus the two are not connected and this newer shift is not represented in the script.
  18. Probably the sequences ăʕʷ ĕʕʷ ĭʕʷ ŏʕʷ ŭʕʷ shifted to ô ô û ô û, thus finally eliminating diphthongs from the language, even those that had arisen from VCV sequences. But it is possible (remember the ʕʕ>ʔ rule) that pharyngealization was not pronounced in this position even in a closed syllable (it had been eliminated for sure in an open syllable). It is also possible that the diphthongs simply remained and that /ʕʷ/ was seen as a consonant.

Note that the only /y/ is before /i/ and unlabialized /u/, the latter of which was rare. The only other clusters in the language had initial elements unmarked for place of articulation, possibly aside from a few marginal holdovers across morpheme boundaries involving inherited syllabic nasals.

Thus the final consonant inventory was

Rounded bilabials:       pʷ  ṗʷ  bʷ      hʷ          w
Spread bilabials:        p   ṗ   b   m   
Alveolars:               t   ṭ   d   n   s   r   l
Postalveolars:           č       ǯ       š   ž  (λ)  y
Velars:                  k   ḳ       ŋ   x   g
Labiovelars:             kʷ  ḳʷ  ġʷ      xʷ  gʷ
Postvelars:              q               h       ʕ
Labialized postvelars:   qʷ

And the vowels /a e i o u/ on six tones: à ă ā á â a͆, where the last two differ in sandhi effects only.

All five vowels are unrounded except when following a labialized consonant. Because /u/ almost always follows a labialized consonant, its unrounded form is very rare unless analyzed as /Ø/. This can be spelled /ʉ/.

A rare palatal lateral λ (IPA /ʎ/) can be added, which occurs only in environments where /y/ can also occur. Unlike the other five palatal consonants /č ǯ š ž y/, however, it is entirely of secondary origin, arising entirely from the sequence /ly/, and it cannot contrast with the sequence /ly/, even over a morpheme boundary.

The w is always phrayngealized as the onset of a stressed syllable.

The sounds q̇ q̇ʷ z are not part of Leaper's phonology, but have distinct letters in the script because they occur in loans from Qaš and different speakers replace them with different Leaper phonemes. Specifically, q̇ q̇ʷ can be replaced either with /ḳ ḳʷ/ or with /q qʷ/, and some speakers even pronounce the original phonemes after all; likewise z can be replaced either with /ž/ or with /s/, and some speakers pronounce the /z/ as in Qaš as well.

Late dialectal changes

One late change might be the deletions of all word-initial g, thus creating many vowel-initial words. Then, old word-initial e- deletes, but the new e- is unaffected. This change is difficult to explain, even assuming the two /e/'s had different sounds (because the old one was always preceded by /j/ and the new one never was).

Macro-Pabap languages

See Macro-Pabap languages for all non-Babakiam stem groups.

Gold (1900) to Play (4100)

The Play language evolved from the Soft Hands dialect of Gold, also known as Wolf in Wool, Broken Shields, and perhaps at least one other name. It drove out the Lazy Palms language and took relatively few loanwords. There were also several other languages spoken in this territory, including one language spoken by Star immigrants, probably a branch of Amade.

Wolf in Wool had not yet evolved its characteristic sound, so the relative scarcity of loanwords was not due to the acoustics of the language, but rather a cultural identification with the new language being imported from overseas. Any loans that were taken in had /e o/ shifting to /ə/ for the entire time period of this language, though /ē ō/ may have been borrowed as /əi əu/ or /ai au/ or either.

  1. At the end of a syllable, the pharyngeal fricative ʕ disappeared and changed the previous vowel to a high tone. It also voiced the following consonant.
  2. Syllable-final k ḳ ŋ changed to kʷ ḳʷ ŋʷ.
  3. Feeding on the above change, in compounds, if the final consonant was one of /kʷ ḳʷ/ and the first consonant of the next morpheme was one of the velars k ḳ h ŋ, it also became labiovelar. Thus for example /kk/ > /kʷkʷ/ or /kʷ:/. It did not happen for other consonants. Prenasals did not shift; later, the cluster /ŋʷk/ becomes /mk/, which is pronounced as spelled but later becomes [ŋk], [mpt], etc depending on dialect.
  4. In initial position, the labialized coronals tʷ dʷ nʷ shifted to t d n. Elsewhere, even in clusters, they decoupled to the sequences tu du nu.
  5. The bilabial approximant w changed to v (in internal reconstructions, also spelled "β") before a vowel.
  6. Then l lʷ both became w (not */v/) in all positions although it retained a rhotic allophone. The distinction between this new /w/ sound and the one that had just changed to /v/ is important later on, as it keeps sequences like /ʕl/ from being corrupted to /ʕʷ~gʷ/ and then on to /v/, /b/, and /p/. Rather, /l/ stays as /w/.
    Notably, the sequence sl (which was pronounced as IPA [hl] or for some speakers [ɬ]) shifted here to sw, and did not become */hʷ/ or */f/. That is, it behaved as the sequence that it was morphologically, instead of sliding with the phonetics into a new single consonant.
    NOTE ON POLITICS: Proto-Highland Poswa breaks off here.
  7. The labiovelar consonants kʷ ḳʷ hʷ gʷ became p ṗ f v unconditionally. This includes sequences like /kʷl/, despite the precedent set by /sl/ above, because in this case, /kʷl/ was already [kʷ] at the surface level in the proto-language.
  8. Sequences of two vowels in which the first vowel was i or u became rising diphthongs. Then all clusters of a consonant followed by a semivowel came to be pronounced as coarticulated single consonants. Thus pua became pʷa, pia became pʲa, and so on.
  9. Stressed syllabic nasals were opened to sequences containing a schwa.
  10. The voiced fricative g assimilated to a neighboring glide /j/ or /w/, thus creating sequences of /jj/ and /ww/. The shift thus was gj jg gw wg > jj jj ww ww. This includes g after /ī/ and /ū/.
  11. The voiced fricatives d dh g became silent between vowels and occasionally in initial position (due to compounding).
    When I wrote this, there was no /ž/ in the language at this stage, and so it is possible that ž also shifts to Ø.
    NOTE ON POLITICS: This time period is around 3100 AD, near the beginning of the "Time of Happiness" (Yeisu Kasu: 3138 - 3302 AD). The branches of the language that fork off from mainline Bābākiam in 3138 all die out, and therefore all of their names in the history are written in Babakiam, but they could be revived as minor local languages, and there would be quite a lot of them.
  12. A voiced consonant in a cluster after /p/ or /s/ changed briefly to ʕ and then disappeared.
    This shift is responsible for important consequences in verb morphology in Poswa more than 5000 years later. Note that the inherited clusters gh hg had been merged as h already in Gold; /hg/ was morphologically equivalent to /sg/, which explains why /sg/ shows up in Play as š instead of s like the others. Lastly, this shift explains why the Play toponym Fanašasa corresponds to Leaper Xʷanaxanta.
  13. The voiced fricatives v z ž g changed to b d ǯ ġ before a high tone. Unlike other languages, Play considered the long vowels to be high tones here.
    This is how Play does /g/ > /k/ even though /g/ was a fricative. Note however that in hypothetical words like /vuau/, where a /d/ dropped out, the initial /v/ was part of a separate syllable, not stressed, and so did not shift to /b/.
  14. The post-velar fricative consonants ħ ʕ, which had been developing labial compression, changed unconditionally to f v.
  15. The velar fricatives h g were fronted to š ž unconditionally. šʲ žʲ became š ž. This includes the /čʲ/ sequence, which had long ago become [šʲ] but was maintained in spelling because of its importantly distinct grammatical behavior.
    Importantly, this shift included conditions in hiatus ("holes" in Play terminology), so that čiva became čua.
  16. The labialized voiced stops bʷ dʷ ǯʷ ġʷ changed to b.
  17. The palatalized voiced stops bʲ dʲ ǯʲ ġʲ changed to ǯ.
  18. Any remaining voiced stops b d ǯ ġ changed unconditionally to p t č k (except when in clusters).
  19. The voiced fricative žʷ changed to v.
  20. Tones were eliminated. However the stress accent (nouns on the penultimate syllable, verbs on the ultimate) remained and became regularized.
  21. The voiced stops d ǯ ġ (now found only in clusters) changed to n nʲ ŋ unconditionally.
  22. Remaining v changed to b.
  23. Remaining z changed to s.
  24. Newly created vowel sequences beginning with i or u collapsed into rising diphthongs, thus creating a new series of palatalized and labialized consonants.
    This same shift happened twice but many words missed by the first change were captured by this change. Note, however, that the reflex of /buya/ is still /buya/; it did not become /bʷia/ and then /bia/.
  25. The labialized consonants bʷ žʷ changed to b unconditionally. (Despite the fact that a nearly identical sound change had occurred only shortly before this one, this rule was very common in verb forms that were created by the shift of /bua/ > /bʷa/ > /ba/, and likewise for other vowels.)
  26. The palatalized consonants bʲ žʲ changed to ž unconditionally. (The above shift also applies here; many verbs underwent a shift of /bia/ > /bʲa/ > /ža/.) This shift did not apply to words such as bivu, from earlier /buivu/, because the /i/ in this word was not [ʲ] but still a true /i/.
  27. A schwa ə in a word in which the following syllable had /a/ changed also to a. Note that this is the only vowel change in the entire history of the language going back 3500 years, even before the Gold language, except for a few diphthongizations such as /ua/ > /wa/. However, the vowel system became very unstable in the succeeding period as the language developed into Poswa and Pabappa.
  28. The stress was shifted to the first syllable in all words.

Play (4100) to Poswa (8700)

Alternate names: Cherry, Blossom, Pavopa, Wupupa

NOTE that this section is outdented for prominence. Poswa is a daughter language of Play, not of Gold.
  1. Long vowels in initial syllables became double: ā ī ū became aa ii uu. This shift did not happen if the long vowel was supported by another following vowel. Meanwhile double vowels in final syllables became long: aa ii uu shifted to ā ī ū. Medial syllables followed morpheme boundaries.
  2. The double vowels ii uu became ʲi ʷu in all positions.
  3. Any bʷ žʷ created by the previous shift changed to b. Likewise any bʲ žʲ changed to ž.
    NOTE THAT THIS IS AN EXACT REPETITION of a shift that occurred late in the history of Play. It is repeated here because new /ʷ ʲ/ had just been created. It could be said that the shift was in continuous operation for several hundred years. It may have applied twice in a few words, such as buivu > bivu > žuu. It did not generally occur over morpheme boundaries because the rebracketing that created vowel-initial morphemes had not yet taken place.
  4. Between two consonants in a single syllable, the diphthongs au ai əu əi changed to o ae u e. iu ui ii uu > y y i u. The change was bypassed whenever a consonant cluster was frontloaded onto the next syllable, however.
    Although /ae/ behaved like a regular vowel in this shift, it was still not possible to produce syllables like */paer/, as it would be later. This is because the shift of /u i/ > /r l/ had not yet happened.
  5. ā aa changed to aba in all positions.
  6. At the beginning of a syllable and after /p m/, the semivowels w j shifted to r l. Syllable-finally, the shift also occurred in some positions but was allophonic and remained so for thousands of years.
    This is the point at which the /ae/ diphthong starts behaving differently than the others; in particular, there was no *syllable-final [al], even allophonically before another /l/. All instances of the [al] sequences were open syllables followed by a singleton /l/ onset.
  7. The medial clusters pt mt mn shifted to tt nt nn. Then pk mk mŋ became pt mpt mn . ms mš became mps mpš.
    The shift of /pt mt mn/ > /tt nt nn/ might need to be back-dated because it affects the way vowels were compressed. For example, Play /pk/ front-loaded onto a following syllable, but Play /pt/ did not. This would make more sense if the contrast was either /pk/ vs /tt/ or /pt/ vs /tt/.
  8. The cluster sf changed to ff.
    In most words where an /sf/ cluster might be expected, only a single /f/ was found due to a much earlier shift that occurred in the Gold language. This had been maintained through analogy. Likewise, where one might expect /pf/, there was often just a /p/. The words where these clusters did occur were newly coined compounds.
  9. The labialized alveolar stop shifted to in word-initial position or after one of /r l s/, and to pt between vowels.
  10. The labialized consonants šʷ sʷ nʷ shifted to pš ps bʷ. There was also a rare word-initial /bʷ/, from earlier /bū/ > /bu/ > /bʷ/. This shift also includes sšʷ ssʷ shifting to ppš pps.
  11. The consonant clusters mr ml shifted to br bl unconditionally.
  12. In initial position before a vowel, the voiceless labial fricatives f fʷ changed to w. It also happened often to a word-internal /f fʷ/ preceded by a /w/ of any origin, but note that the sequence /fVf/ only appeared in words that were originally compounds. This shift did not affect . NOTE: Shift affecting internal /fʷ/, not just /f/, added late to cover for situations in Pabappa.
  13. After a labialized consonant (except /w/), the schwa vowel y changed to u.
  14. Labialized consonants lost their labialization when they occurred before /u/.
  15. The vowels i u changed to e o in closed syllables.
  16. The vowel y in closed syllables changed to either i or u depending on the other vowel in the root. The default choice was /u/, unless it followed a /w/. /i/ appeared only when it followed a /w/ or was in a word in which an /i/ or /e/ was in an adjacent syllable and that was the only other vowel in the word.
  17. The sequences wu wo changed to wi we.
  18. Then, p s disappeared before nasals and sporadically in stem-final position due to back-formation from plurals.
    IMPORTANTLY, it also disappears before /b/ in many, if not all, words. Apparent exceptions can be explained by later compounding; note that they mostly involve /p bl/ --> /bbl/ where /bl/ would be expected, but seldom /p b/ > /bb/ where /b/ would be expected.
  19. Unaccented i changed to e unless the accented syllable of the word contained an /i/.
  20. Then unaccented y changed to i unless the syllable ended in a labial or the accented syllable of the word contained a /y/.
    NOTE ON POLITICS: Politically, the proto-Poswobs became independent here (5547), but the language remained unified through physical contact for another 450 years or so.
  21. The consonant cluster ŋʲ assimilated to .
  22. Then unaccented u changed to y except when the syllable ended in a labial or the accented syllable also contained a /u/.
  23. Before a vowel, unaccented y yb changed to u.
  24. Before a vowel, unaccented a ab changed to i.
  25. Next, i changed to y if the next syllable had /u/.
  26. Any l r became devoiced after a voiceless stop (probably only /p/ exists; so the rule would be pl pl shifting to prʰ plʰ).
  27. The palatalized consonants pʲ mʲ sʲ lʲ rʲ changed to f v š ž b before a vowel. šʲ žʲ bʲ also became š ž b. The rare sequence rrʲ most likely became bb, not *rb. However, mmʲ shifted to mv, not *vv.
  28. The voiceless allophones of /lʲ rʲ/ shifted to the voiceless counterparts of the plain forms; that is, plʲ prʲ shifted to pš pp, matching the /ž b/ of the plain forms. This explains why the rare sequences /pž pb/ (from compounds; never inherited from Play) did not also become voiceless.
  29. The sequence ppp (the reflex of earlier /pprʲ/) shifted to pp. But /ppš/ remained.
    NOTE ON POLITICS: (year 6000; THIS IS WHERE PABAPPA BREAKS OFF)
  30. In stressed syllables, in only a few words, ol ul or ur changed to we wi wa wa. The conditioning environment was that the syllables had to be unstressed and have only one consonant before them; in other words, they occurred in compounds only, in a syllable which would be stressed if it weren't a compound.
  31. In an accented syllable, ar shifted to o, except after /w/.
  32. Simultaneously, syllable-final r in most words changed to . Aquatically, it was f/v before coronals, b in absolute final position, stays r before šž + labials + velars (but really pronounced as /w/). rl > vl. rr > rw. Sometimes au+labial > o even so, no particular rule.
  33. The surviving final r changed to vʷ fʷ before labials.
  34. (and probably ffʷ) became w before a vowel (that is, everywhere except before a labial).
    Note that /ffʷ/ defies the pattern set by /ppf/ because in the latter case there was no assimilation, but /ffʷ/ had both members as bilabial, meaning it was really /fʷ:/ all along.
  35. In words not affected by the previous shift (mostly due to grammatical analogy), syllable-final ar yr shifted to o, and er ir shifted to u.
  36. In unstressed syllables, the sequences el il merged as i and or ur merged as u.
  37. Unstressed ol ul became e before a consonant or at the end of a word.
  38. i y shifted to u before a labial in a closed syllable, or before a syllable beginning with a labial cluster (in this case, even /pʷ/ etc counted as "clusters"). Thus for example /tipwu/ > /tupwu/.
    However, other labialized consonants such /tʷ/ did not trigger the shift, so /titwu/ stays /titwu/. If there was a new /fʷ vʷ/ series, it most likely did not trigger the shift either. But note that the "tʲw" cluster had not yet become /fʷ/.
    NOTE ON POLITICS ... THIS IS WHERE TUPPY BREAKS OFF (year 6843)
  39. ŋ ŋʷ were denasalized to g gʷ in all positions.
  40. The palatalized velar consonants kʲ gʲ became the postalveolar affricates tš dž.
    Note: these may have still been palatals such as ć ǵ, because /gʲ/ behaves differently than the rare cluster /dž/ that came from collapse of /tVž/ until the later shift of /g/ > /dž/ merges /g gʲ dž/ all together.
  41. The labialized velar consonants kʷ gʷ pʷ bʷ were decomposed to the clusters kw gw pw bw.
  42. Final y in trisyllabic words disappeared. Due to analogy, it disappeared in some shorter words as well. However, consos that now occurred at the end of a word because of the dropped y became labialized, though this is not shown in Romanizaiton.
  43. The remaining palatalized consonants became labiodental fricatives: fʲ tʲ merged as f, and changed to v. The sequence nnʲ shifted to nv and later /mv/ (still later /mb/ in most environments).
  44. The cluster pf, when straddling a syllable boundary, became ff. It is likely that /mpf/ and /ppf/ did not exist at this time, therefore making this shift unconditional.
    NOTE, actually /pptʲ/ did exist, and this changes to /ppf/ in the shift above.
  45. Medial vowels in trisyllabic words disappeared if the resulting consonant cluster was acceppable ("the Debra shift"). wr > rw (distinct from earlier shift). Here again, labialization hung around if the deleted vowel was o u or y. Thus there were minimal pairs such as puppa "salamander" vs pupʷpa "kind, humanitarian". This period (around the year 7300) is the beginning of what is often considered "Classical Poswa".
    Poswobs invaded Pabappa territory beginning around 7414, and the language was essentially unchanged at that time. The next few changes never occur in native words because they would have appeared in compounds only, and grammatical reanalysis eliminated the clusters that fed the shift; however, they occur in opaque loanwords and names.
  46. The clusters tm km fm vm shifted to vb. šm žm sm šb sb shifted to žb.
  47. The clusters pn bn shifted to . šn sn became žv.
  48. The sequences pm tn shifted to mm nn. tp became pp. Importantly, however, the nasal clusters /mn nm/ both remained intact.
  49. The clusters pk and bg became pw and bi respectively. mk became mw (not mpw). Similar changes applied to other clusters ending in velars. The clusters /bk pg/ did not exist at this time because regressive voicing assimilation had automatically shifted them to /pk bg/. This voicing assimilation only happened when the Debra shift brought the consonants into direct contact.
  50. All posttonic k g shifted to w i, taking any intervening vowels with them; this is the same as the shift above except that there was no voicing assimilation. This also included the spoken of above which was listed as /dž/ but still behaves distinctly from preexisting /dž/ from /tž/. Note also that this shift did not affect /ž/.
    NOTE: This shift is important and may be handled incorrectly in some dictionary entries. It seems to imply that *ANY* posttonic /w/ also consumes preceding vowels, which often happens when coming from earlier /fʷ/. On the other hand, there needs to be a path that generates posttonic /uw/, as distinct from /w/, and this might need to come from sequences like /wiw/.
  51. Posttonic sequences like igigʷ and agik shifted to uw. That is, wherever a sequence like /iw/ arose, this became /uw/. But double /k/ sequences still ended up as a simple /w/.
  52. The voiced velar stop g was fronted to unless it occurred in a cluster after another consonant and before /a o u/.
    This shift is not being followed exactly; it is behaving mostly like /k/ except in the onset where it does indeed shift to /dž/.
  53. The clusters šb bš were devoiced to šp pš respectively. Then sb bs shifted to sp ps. That is, the fricative dominated the stop, unlike most such changes.
    These clusters existed despite a previous shift of the same clusters because in both cases they appeared only in foreign loans and in some ad-hoc compounds. This means that an original sb could variably appear in Poswa as any of s b žb sp, depending on when the two consonants came together.
  54. The clusters žp pž became žb bž respectively.
  55. The clusters tv ttv shifted to v vv; this was actually through a [d] allophone, which explains why they did not both become geminates.
  56. Likewise, tf ttf shifted to t tt. This may seem a contradiction but it is actually the same shift as the above, explained by the path of /t/ > /d/ > /ð/ > /v/ which is compressed into one step above. (Note that in one word, /pts/ shifts to /pps/ as though /ts/ cannot start a syllable.)
  57. The clusters nr nl shifted to ng (IPA [nd]).
  58. The clusters pl bl became p b respectively when overlaying two unaccented syllables. The same also happened for other stop + nonstop clusters such as pr br pš ps bž pt pf bv, althoiugh pš, pf, and ps survived as aspirates pʰ for long enough to survive a particular subsequent shift. Clusters like mž mdž shifted to mb.
  59. The sequences yw ww wy changed to ʷu in all positions.
  60. When unaccented, the sequence wi shifted to i before another vowel.
  61. iy ii > ia ie. This also includes /ir/ > /ia/ (when not labialized).
  62. In wholly unstressed syllables, except when preceded by /a/, the clusters rl lr changed to al ar respectively, and raised the preceding vowel.
  63. The cluster rgw shifted to vbw, with both consonants labialized.
  64. A velar-onset syllable preceded by another changed to alveolar if the vowel was a back vowel, but postalveolar if it was a front vowel. The affected consonants were k g kw gw r.
  65. The diphthongs al yl shifted to ae e. These did not affect /lʷ/.
  66. Geminate stops overlaying two unaccented syllables reduced to single if there was another geminate earlier in the word.
  67. The cluster mp shifted to mb when occuring after a voiceless stop.
  68. Initial ps pš shifted to p, also sometimes if overlaying two unaccented slabs just as /pl bl/ had.

Baba (~6000) to Old Pabappa (~6500) ... too early?

Alternate names: Pespimbesa

  1. Debra shift I. (But rV and wV didnt compress). ("ae" and "al" remained distinct here). If the deleted vowel was y, o or u, the preceding consonant became a labial. If a consonant was already labialized or palatalized, it did not contract.
    This shift must be broken into steps because it does not turn /tut/ > /pt/. It may be that the /tʷ/ > /p/ actually happened later in a second shift where only /ə/ was deleted.
  2. In the syllable coda, lʷ rʷ shifted to u. Also, in clusters like /kʷr/, this became [kʷrʷ], and then the resulting lʷ rʷ shifted to w. This can be identified as part of the same shift above.
  3. Homorganic stop-fricative sequences like pʷf resulting from the vowel elision came to stand at the head of a syllable, and therefore both consonants became labialized, resulting in sequences that were spelled pfʷ.
  4. After a stop, shifted to a fully rounded voiceless bilabial fricative, here spelled h because there was no other /h/.
    This also extended to shifts like tʷl shifting to , since there was no /lʷ/ or /rʷ/.
  5. shifted to f. This means that it cannot participate in the shift below where palatalized consonants stain the following vowel to /i/.
  6. ŋ was denasalized to g in all positions, except bound clusters such as /ŋp ŋt ŋr ŋk/ which behaved as prenasals.
    NEED to check dictionary to make sure this rule has been followed properly, including subsequent changes. For example, if kg shifts to gg later, it ends up as bb ultimately, the only voiced geminate in the language. But perhaps they should be devoiced when they were still stops.
  7. Clusters like np nb shifted to mp mb (homorganic). This does NOT shift /mpt/ etc intp /nt/. which happens later and is important to happe nalter.
  8. Following a consonant, diphthongs of all types were resolved in favor of the first vowel. Thus, all palatalized consonants shifted to the corresponding plain form (unlike in Poswa, where most had distinct reflexes). That is to say, ʲa ʲe ʲi ʲo ʲu ʲy all became a simple i and ʷa ʷe ʷi ʷo ʷu ʷy became a simple u.
    Importantly, this did not come from a bare /w/, even in word-initial position; /wes/ remained /wes/ instead of shifting to /us/.
    Note that this means syllable-final lʷ rʷ, which earlier became /u/, now disappear to Ø except in the rare cases mentioned below. And əu shifts to ə (usually later to /o/).
  9. Triphthongs with a high middle element were collapsed to two-vowel sequences along the opposite pattern; thus, for example, iwa> ua.
    It is possible that iai shifted to just an i. If not, it would be /ia/, not /ai/.
  10. Stops occurring before a nasal assimilated fully.
  11. Clusters of two fricatives of differing points of articulation were assimilated in favor of the second consonant.
  12. The clusters žbž špš sps shifted to žž šš ss, and likewise for mismatched pairs.
  13. Clusters of fricatives and stops of dissimilar voicing were resolved in favor of the second consonant. This includes fž sž shifting to vž žž (later to /d Ø/).
  14. The clusters gv kf became bv pf.
  15. Word-initial pš tš shifted to š while word-initial ps ts shifted to s.
    Note that initial /tš/ occurs only in the copula; it is not /kʲ/, which had remained as such and then dropped its palatalization in a shift just shortly before this one.
  16. The fricatives šʷ žʷ (possibly only found in the coda) shifted to f v.
    NOTE that this is an important and newly introduced shift, as before they behaved as normal /š ž/ and therefore mostly shifted to /s Ø/.
  17. The fricative š changed to h in initial position and to s elsewhere.
  18. The voiced sequences ž dž came to be pronounced z.
  19. The cluster rr was reduced to r.

Old Pabappa (6500?) to Pabappa (8700)

  1. Double nasals were reduced to singles.
  2. The vowel ə either disappeared or became i or o (governed by the surrounding vowels' HEIGHT (not backness)). If it disappeared, it also labialized the new final consonant.
    NOTE THAT THIS INCLUDES MEDIAL SYLLABLES, since /rə/ did not delete earlier. However it is not clear what /rʷ/ will turn into, and /b/ is unlikely.
    This also includes WORD-FINAL SYLLABLES that did not delete during the Debra shift because there was no following syllable.
    Consider also forcing /kʷ/ > /p/ at least in final position, since it will otherwise disappear. Additionally, earlier an implied shift of > p took place word-finally, which would imply /kʷ/ > /p/ too if kept.
  3. The fricatives v z shifted to d Ø in all positions. This included doubled forms.
    It is possible that this causes the relatively common cluster bv to become dd (not /bd/), as per the shift involving /ps/ below. This would be an affricate at first.
    Another possible exception is that vz (common in a suffix, from earlier -fž-) might also become dd because the two consonants were shifting at about the same time, and the v > d shift passed through /ð/. Still, if /z/ disappeared even slightly before /v/ became /ð/, the reflex would be a simple d after all. One reason to expect that /z/ > /Ø/ was slightly earlier is that the shift of /f/ > /s/ is displaced below.
    Note also that the shift of /zz/ > /Ø/ means that the reflexes of the palatalized forms of sl šl žl are all Ø; this is important because many words for handheld objects end in /-sla -šla -žla/, which therefore all become /-a/ when possessed.
  4. The velar stops k g changed to the fricatives š ž in all positions.
    These were weakly labialized, but it is important not to confuse them with the true labialized fricatives below. This is complicated, because the reflexes essentially switch places: /š ž/ mostly become /p b/, but /šʷ/ becomes /s/ except in initial position (and it may never have occurred initially), while /žʷ/ might not exist at all.
  5. The labialized consonants sʷ šʷ žʷ shifted to f f b. (It is not clear that a distinct /žʷ/ still existed, but if it did, it too would have been carried away.) Then shifted to m at least in final position.
  6. The clusters šs sš became ss šš. (Unless /š/ had already shifted to /f/.)
  7. The fricative f changed to p in initial position and s elsewhere. This also triggers a shift of all pf to ts, even though /ps/ remained as such.
  8. This was around the year 7000. Note that the Play 1st and 2nd person past passive participle endings /-su -si/ had become /-s -se/ by this stage; it is unlikely that Pabappa would preserve them since the 1st person would have become silent, but other daughter languages may preserve both.
  9. The voiceless stops p t became the geminates pp tt when following a voiceless stop plus a vowel.
  10. The postalveolars š ž dž became f v bv in all positions.
    The shift of /dž/ > /bv/ is listed here because if /d/ remained unassimilated it would devoice and then fall into the /tp/ > /tt/ rule below. But a shift of /ž/ > /v/ without affecting an adjoining /d/ is highly unlikely.
  11. The cluster ph (phonetically something like /pɸ/) shifted to pp.
    Note that this is actually a dummy shift to prevent it from being voiced to /b/. In fact, it remained as roughly /pɸ/, still distinct from the rarer true /pf/, which shifts to /pp/ later on, and also distinct from a simple /p/, which in some cases shifts to /b/ later on.
  12. The glottal fricative h shifted to f before /u/.
    It is possible that this shift can be moved further down so as not to interfere with the shift above.
  13. The voiced stops b d became the voiceless stops p t in all positions. Adjacent fricatives also devoiced.
    It is possible that the cluster vv should become ff here, even though there was no stop. It is the only surviving voiced fricative, and therefore the only surviving voiced fricative cluster.
  14. Word-final s disappeared to Ø.
    The word-final /-s/ was soon restored in just one instance, the Play word pais and anything rhyming with it; this is because all of the inflected forms of /pais/ had become identical to those of Play /pasi/, which had not lost its /s/ because it was still padded by /e/. The time interval between these two shifts was very short, and effectively by merging the two words the shift was undone in just one environment.
    Note that there is probably no word-final /f/, but if it does somehow appear, it would also shift to Ø.
  15. Word-final e disappeared, except after a consonant cluster; in this case it changed to i.
    Many longstanding irregulars were regularized here. For example, nisol ~ nipse- became nipsi ~ nipse- by making what had been the genitive (ending in -s) the new nominative form.
  16. Before a nasal, p s t n assimilated completely.
  17. Clusters of a nonlabial stop followed by a labial stop were resolved in favor of the nonlabial one. It seems most likely that this is just tp shifting to tt.
  18. Final o was were lowered to a except if the accented vowel was mid-height (e or o).
    NOTE ON POLITICS: This is considered to be the classical stage of Middle Pabappa.
  19. The clusters tl ttl both changed to ll.
  20. The sequences mr sr lr became mpr spr rr. Any other nonlabial consonant before /r/ became labial.
  21. Clusters of a nasal followed by any other consonant of differing point of articulation were assimilated in favor of the point of articulation of the second consonant. Thus surviving mn shifted to nn.
  22. ml became mpl.
    NOTE ON POLITICS: This occurred around 7414 AD.
  23. The voiceless labiodental fricative f changed to w in initial position.
  24. The voiceless glottal fricative h shifted to Ø.
  25. Unstressed syllables of the form CVCC where the two latter C's were a geminate or one of a few other types of consonants changed the vowel to a very short schwa /ə/.
  26. Intervocalic voiceless stops became voiced.
  27. The cluster pl became bl in all positions.
  28. The cluster pr became b in word-initial position or after /m/, p after /s/, and br elsewhere.
    Consider lbr rbr > lb rb as well, and perhaps extension to all clusters (though other clusters would be rare, since pVrr was rare or perhaps nonexistent).
  29. The geminates pp tt ss became p t s in all positions.
  30. The cluster sp became ss.
  31. The rare vowel sequences ei ou (found almost entirely in verb conjugations) shifted to e o.
    NOTE ON POLITICS: (ANDANESE PABAPPA SPLITS OFF HERE)
  32. Initial v became f.
  33. The labiodental fricatives f v became the bilabial stops p b in all positions.
  34. The marginal schwa phoneme disappeared, creating some new clusters and geminates. (Debra shift II.)
  35. The bilabial stop b, between two identical unaccented vowels (e.g. -aba, -obo), turned to m sporadically as the result of analogy from various noun declensions.

Sister languages of Pabappa

For sound changes, see Macro-Pabap_languages#East_of_Paba.

Sister languages split off from Pabappa in several waves, and could arguably include tiny, relic branches that are older than the Poswa-Pabappa split as well as those that are younger. The arbitrary breaks at around 6500, 7000, 7414, and the unlabeled Andanese Pabappa split do not correspond to wars, as the Pabaps had long since ceased fighting wars.

See also

Notes