Late Andanese: Difference between revisions

From FrathWiki
Jump to navigationJump to search
 
(93 intermediate revisions by 2 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
Andanese is the name of a people who currently have no language of their own.  Around the year 4200, their language went extinct although it was preserved in loanwords and in written records.  This written language is called '''Late Andanese''', a language with a very simple phonology.
Around the year 4178, the last surviving [[Andanic languages|Andanic]] language went extinct as an everyday spoken language, but its ornate writing system survived, and thus knowledge of the language persisted for thousands of years.  This preserved language is called '''Late Andanese'''.


==scratch pad==
'''itanu inuhahuka inuimina inuihupuhu inuihuhatahupuunatata''' "road to a camp fire wood store"


The above sentence could be loosely glossed as "ROAD STORE WOOD FIRE CAMP", but this does not explain why the words get longer towards the end, and why the increase is much greater on the last word than on the steps in between.  Andanese does not need twelve syllables to say "camp"; the last word in the clause must carry the agreement morphemes of all the others, and these are piled on both sides of the root.


==Phonology==
==Phonology==
Late Andanese, spoken around the year 4200 and thereafter as a ceremonial language, has only 12 phonemes: the consonants /l h k m n ŋ s p t/ and the vowels /a i u/.  And of these, the consonants /s/ and /ŋ/ are rare because they originated primarily from sequences rather than single phonemes.  Vowel sequences are allowed, but final consonants are not.  Thus there are only 30 syllables in the language.   
Late Andanese has only 12 phonemes: the consonants /l h k m n ŋ s p t/ and the vowels /a i u/.  And of these, the consonants /s/ and /ŋ/ are rare because they originated primarily from sequences rather than single phonemes.  Vowel sequences are allowed, but final consonants are not.  Thus there are only 30 syllables in the language.   
 
   
[[Old Andanese]] had had a much higher syllable count because it had more consonants, five vowels, two tones, and allowed clusters and syllable-final consonants.  However, in reality the vast majority of syllables in Old Andanese were open syllables as well, and only one syllable per word could carry tone, which means that for the most part Old Andanese could be spelled with only 75 syllables, not greatly different from its descendant. Also, the Old Andanese consonants /kʷ qʷ/ could be analysed as non-phonemic.


Late Andanese as spoken today is based on historical records, since there is no surviving population that has been continuously speaking the language during the entire 4500 years that have passed since its extinction around the year 4200.    Thus the pronunciation varies from place to place without the language itself being different.  In general though, these differences are small and mostly related to the pronunciation of whole syllables rather than individual phonemes.  It could be argued that syllables like /ni/, /ti/, /si/ are actually single phonemes because many populations read them as single consonants such as /ñ/, /č/, /š/ when they occur before a vowel and in some cases even before a consonant.  And thus it could be said that modern Andanese has more than 12 phonemes after all.  However, no Andanese tradition has reintroduced tones or phonemes not directly descended from one of the 30 syllables in the language.
    
    
===Consonants===
===Consonants===
Line 18: Line 19:


Another way that Andanese differs from the Gold languages is that, although /a/ is the commonest vowel, it is noticeably less common by comparison to the other two vowels than it is in most Gold languages.  The three vowels are all well represented in Andanese speech.
Another way that Andanese differs from the Gold languages is that, although /a/ is the commonest vowel, it is noticeably less common by comparison to the other two vowels than it is in most Gold languages.  The three vowels are all well represented in Andanese speech.
====Traits of the vowel system shared with Babakiam====
The trait of contracting /i u/ to the semivowels [j w] is shared with [[Babakiam]], a language that is only distantly related genetically (having separated about 4500 years before the maturation of Late Andanese), but closely linked culturally, as it was spoken in the same nation.
Another unusual trait shared with Babakiam is the occurrence of long sequences of the vowel /a/, which does not contract into a semivowel in any position.  For example, '''kaaaaka''' "tree" is a common word,<ref>But note Babakiam '''abāp''', which would normally pair with an Andanese root such as ''aaki'' or even just ''aa''.</ref> despite being five syllables long.  Both Andanese and Babakiam developed this trait by dropping intervocalic consonants.  However, unlike Andanese, Babakiam maintains a distinction between vowel sequences like this and inherited long vowels; by contrast, the inherited long vowels of Old Andanese changed to simple ones before the deletion of intervocalic consonants, and are thus reflected as single (short) vowels in Late Andanese.
===Phonemic contractions and other processes of morphophonology===
====Historical use of the glottal stop====
Old Andanese had held onto a phonemic voiceless uvular stop /q/ inherited from [[Tapilula]].  This eventually changed into a glottal stop /ʔ/.  While this glottal stop was still phonemic, Andanese underwent a sound change that turned the sound sequences /ki ti hi/ before another vowel into a new phoneme, /s/.  Then, later on, the glottal stop disappeared, leaving behind new vowel sequences.  Thus, sequences of /ki ti hi/ plus a vowel were once again possible, and the words in which it had changed to /s/ as part of a grammatical operation came to be seen as irregular.  The native Andanese script kept the now-silent glottal stop distinct by using different symbols for its syllables, so this unpredictability was not generally a problem for the speakers of the language, but soon the use of the separate symbols fell out of use and they came to be recycled to spell /s/ itself.  Thus, one of Andanese's few irregularities is that compounds of a morpheme ending in /ki ti hi/ and one beginning in a vowel sometimes collapse into /s/ sequences and sometimes they do not.  This has still not been regularized by analogy.
At the same time as the above shift, the syllables /ku tu pu/ plus a vowel all shifted to /p/.  This was not a new phoneme, but it previously had been rare in initial position because of an earlier shift which had changed it to /gʷ/ (whose pronunciation varied between a true [gʷ] and [w]) when it began an accented syllable, and most words at that time were accented on their initial syllable.  Thus, this sound change also takes place unpredictably when a word ending in one of those syllables is compounded with certain vowel-initial words.
;NOTE, WOULD IT MAKE MORE SENSE TO RESTRICT THIS SECOND SHIFT TO JUST THE INHERITED LABIALIZED CONSONANTS?
At older stages of the language, similar shifts had taken place which led to different results.  For example, the classifier prefix '''ki-'''' indicates men or boys, and contracts to a '''k-''' before a word historically beginning in a vowel.  It doesn't become ''s-'' because the sound shift that caused this change happened much earlier than the /ki/ > /s/ shift, and therefore /ki/ had been eliminated in all of the environments in which it would have been able to shift to /s/.  This /ki/ > /k/ shift is still blocked by words historically beginning with a glottal stop, though, because the glottal stop was still there (as /q/) at the time of the /ki/ > /k/ shift.  Thus, to refer to the ''ulihipinimu'' "hip bone" of a male, one would say '''kiulihipinimu''' "man's hip bone", not ''*kulihipinimu'' or ''*sulihipinimu''.


==Phonology and vocabulary==
==Phonology and vocabulary==
With only 30 syllables in the language, many sentences, even with basic vocabulary items, are highly repetitive.  For example '''hahaha''' is the word for "hat, cap, headgear" (''ha-'' "shaped like" + ''haha'' "hair of the head").
With only 30 syllables in the language, many sentences, even with basic vocabulary items, are highly repetitive.  For example '''hahaha''' is the word for "hat, cap, headgear" (''ha-'' "shaped like" + ''haha'' "hair of the head").


Consonants besides /l h k/ are less frequent.  The overrepresentation of the consonants /l h k/ (in roughly that order) is part of the reason why Andanese words are often so long even compared to other languages such as [[Babakiam]] that have small phonologies.   
Consonants besides /l h k/ are less frequent.  The overrepresentation of the consonants /l h k/ (in roughly that order) is part of the reason why Andanese words are often so long even compared to other languages such as [[Babakiam|Play]] that have small phonologies.   


===Expressive wordplay===
===Expressive wordplay===
Another reason why words in Late Andanese are so long is the use of extensive wordplay.
====Boys' names====
====Boys' names====
Boys' names were often extremely long-winded  due to the deliberate repetition of similar-sounding words and syllables. For example, '''Kukukukukukuku''' was a common boy's name; '''Kaaaaaaia''' was another.  '''Haaaaaaaaaaa''' was also a very common boy's name.  '''Taaaamaaaaaamaaaaa''' was less common but still not unheard of.  '''Aaaaaaaaaaatataaaa''' is another common boy's nameEach /a/ is a seaprate syllable.  The longest boy's name in common use was '''Kakakaaakakatakakakakakakakakaka'''A close second was '''Matamataamatatataamaataaaatata'''.  '''Lilalaaiilalalalalalaa''' is another very long boy's name, but can be abbreviated to '''Lalaaalai'''.
Boys' names were often extremely long-winded  due to the deliberate repetition of similar-sounding words and syllables. Simple reduplication was avoided, but since the language had many homophones, long sequences of identical syllables piled up even so. By omitting classifier prefixes, the inventory of homophones increased vastly, with dozens of possible interpretations for each morphemeThus, even two boys with the same name could claim unique derivations.   


A classroom of children would often sit quietly when a teacher called out their names, as if hearing lottery numbers read off, as a boy with a lengthy name would need to listen for quite a while to know whether the child being called up to the front of the class was him or his friend whose name differed only on the 17th syllable.
The repetitive names were inspired by the Late Andanese process of syllable harmony, in which a whole syllable in a word changes to that of its classifier prefix.  But, as with reduplication, Andanese names avoided syllable harmony, since parents considered it unimpressive to coin a name whose internal assonance was simply due to their language's grammar.  Instead, every morpheme in each name was a proper word on its own and could be interpreted in a fixed number of ways.


====Girls' names====
One common name, '''Kukukukukukuku''', was an extreme example, since it consisted of the thematic syllable /ku/ seven times in a row.  All speakers recognized ''kuku'' as their word for "leg, run", but from there, thousands of possible interpretations of the name opened up, since, with classifier prefixes removed, the same ''kuku'' could also mean "canopy; place high up", "night", or "road"; meanwhile, the remaining ''ku'' could also mean "forest", "hand", "arrow (weapon)", or "to expose, show publicly"Thus one boy with the name Kukukukukukuku might tell friends his name meant "running down trails  at night" (''kuku kukuku kuku''; trails being roads in a forest) while another would say that it meant "sitting on a treetop with arrows in hand" (''kuku kukuku ku ku'', treetops being high places in a forest).  Stress was placed only on the final syllable, irrespective of morpheme boundaries, just as in the language as a whole.
Girls' names typically did not use reduplication, and were thus typically shorter, but parents often chose long names that did not use reduplicationThe number of girls' names in use was always much greater than the number of boys' names, and many parents coined novel names never used before.


====Relationship to other languages====
A more common name template would intersperse a dominant thematic syllable with several minor ones. '''Kaaaaaaya''' was a common boy's name, where the thematic syllable is /a/ and the two minor syllables are /ka/ and /i/'''Haaaaaaaaaaa''' was also a very common boy's name.  '''Taaaamaaaaaamaaaaa''' was less common.  '''Aaaaaaaaaaatataaaa''' is another common boy's name.  Each /a/ is a separate syllableThe longest boy's name in common use was '''Kakakaaakakatakakakakakakakakaka'''.
Andanese is highly divergent even from closely related languagesSpeakers of [[Pabappa]] fail to recognize even those few words that are cognates, because both languages have changed them radically over timeOne such word pair that does look similar is Pabappa ''Uranu'', Andanese '''Ulanu''', the name of a godHowever, this similarity is partly coincidental, as both languages removed the initial consonant of the name independently and at different times.


===Allophony and sandhi===
Some names had two thematic syllables; an example of this was '''Matamataamatatataamaataaaatata'''.  The bare /a/ was common enough in the language as a whole that its repetition here was not considered to create a third thematic syllable.
Allophony in Andanese is best understood as a process that affects whole syllables rather than individually affecting consonants or vowels.  With only 30 syllables in its phonology, the syllable has come to be the minimal phonological unit, and each of the 30 syllables behaves uniquely.  For example, ''si'' behaves sometimes like ''ti'', sometimes like ''su'', and sometimes unlike either of them.


====Allophones of vowel sequences====
Some names had abbreviated forms. For example, '''Lilalaayilalalalalalaa''' was another very long boy's name, but could be abbreviated to '''Lalaaalai'''.
The vowel sequences ''ia ii iu'' are pronounced ['''ya yi yu'''], and this is reflected in Romanization. Likewise, ''ua ui uu'' are pronounced ['''wa wi wu'''.]  In the case of ''ii'' and ''uu'', the on-glide is weak but still distinctly present, and the second vowel is lowered slightly.  Note that the Romanization here uses '''v''' for IPA /w/ and '''y''' for IPA /j/.


Triple vowel sequences are resolved by starting from the right.  For example, ''tiui'' can only be ['''tiwi'''], never [*tyui].
A classroom of children would often sit quietly when a teacher called out their names, as though hearing lottery numbers read off, since a boy with a lengthy name would need to listen for quite a while to know whether the student being called up to the front of the class was him or his friend whose name differed only on the thirteenth syllable.


The vowel [a] changes very little in any context.  The sequence ''aa'' is simply ['''a:'''], and the sequences ''ai au'' raise the /a/ only slightlyIt is common to find long sequences of vowels, including /a/, especially in derived words. Here, the same rules are followed, and thus ''aaa'', ''aaaa'', and so on, are simply more lengthened versions of /a/.  However, with sequences of three or more /a/'s, the intonation resets on every odd-numbered vowel, counting from the right. Thus ''vapaaa'' is pronounced ['''wapa:a'''].
====Girls' names====
Girls' names typically did not use repeating syllables, and were thus typically shorter, but parents often chose long names that did not use repetitionThe number of girls' names in use was always much greater than the number of boys' names, and many parents coined novel names never used before.


====Allophones of syllables beginning with /h/====
====Other proper names====
As /a/ is the commonest vowel in the language, the allophone of /h/ that occurs before /a/ is considered the primary one.  This varies between [x] and [χ] depending on stress and emphasis.  Before the vowel /i/, /h/ is fronted to a voiceless palatal fricative [ç], and before /u/, it becomes a sound that varies between a rounded voiceless bilabial approximant [hʷ] and a spread voiceless bilabial fricative [ɸ].
The same pattern of repeating syllables showed up in placenames such as '''Mipatatatatai''' (Land of the Ruling Children of Tata) and party names such as '''Laaatilalatitiaa''' (Rusted Pearls).


Since the allophones of /h/ are entirely determined by which of Andanese's three vowels follows it, in rapid speech the vowel may be spoken so quickly that the listener does not even hear it, and perceives instead a consonant cluster beginning with one of the above sounds.  This shortening never occurs in word-final position, even when another word follows.  Also, /a/ is less likely to be dropped than either of the two high vowels.
==Allophony and sandhi==
:''NOTE: This section was much longer until Nov 10 2022.  See edit history for details.''
Late Andanese has a much quicker speech tempo than the [[Play language]] that occupies the same territory.  This is because the most common consonants in Play are the bilabials /p b m/ and the most common vowel is the open low vowel /a/; the time required to move the lips slows down the pronunciation of syllables containing these sounds, and sets the rhythm for the language, so other syllables are also pronounced slowly.


====Other allophony involving /h/====
By contrast, the most common consonant sounds in Andanese are /l h k/, and therefore can be paired with any vowel without slowing down the speech tempoAlthough Andanese has labial consonants /p m/, they are not nearly so common as in Play, and so the tongue and lips move independently during the pronunciation of each syllable far more often than in Play.  
In rapid speech, syllables beginning with /h/ resemble those beginning with vowels, as the [h] spreads backwards over the vowel of a preceding syllable. This also entails  contraction of the VC sequences ''-ih- -uh-'' into voiceless glides [ç ɸ].  For example, '''puha''' may sound like [pʰwa] or [pʷxʷa], and '''hihahuhi''' "womb" will be pronounced ['''çḁɸi'''], with the whisper tapering off only towards the end of the final vowel.  In careful speech, this process is avoided if the preceding consonant is another fricative (/s/ or /h/), or if the preceding syllable is vowel-initial, and therefore in careful speech ''hihahuhi'' would sound like [çixaɸuçi], with four clear vowels, albeit very short ones.
 
====Allophones of syllables beginning with /k/====
In isolation, the syllables ''ka ki ku'' are pronounced like ['''ka ći kʷu'''], with the [ka] also slightly further back than velar.  Before a vowel (including /h/+vowel in rapid speech) ''ki ku'' contract to ['''ć kʷ''']The sequences ''ki ku'' are still audibly distinct from '''kii kuu''', however, because in the latter the starting point of the double vowel sequence is higher, and thus the glides [j w] are audible.
 
====Allophones of syllables beginning with /t/====
In isolation, the syllables ''ta ti tu'' are pronounced like ['''ta či ṭu'''].  Before a vowel (including /h/+vowel in rapid speech) ''ti tu'' contract to ['''č ṭʷ'''].  The sequences ''ti tu'' are still audibly distinct from '''tii tuu''', however, because in the latter the starting point of the double vowel sequence is higher, and thus the glides [y w] are audible.
 
====Allophones of syllables beginning with /p/====
Before a vowel (including /h/+vowel in rapid speech) ''pi pu'' contract to ['''pj pʷ'''].  The sequence ''pu'' is still audibly distinct from '''puu''', however, because in the latter the starting point of the double vowel sequence is higher, and thus the glide [w] is audible.
 
====Allophones of syllables beginning with /s/====
In rapid speech, the sequences ''si su'' are pronounced ['''š ṣʷ''']. This contraction occurs even before consonants, except before another /s/. 
 
====Allophones of syllables beginning with /l/====
The lateral approximant /l/ is advanced slightly before any /u/, and retracted slightly before any /i/, but these differences are very minor by comparison to the changes that affect /t/.
 
====Other allophony involving /l/====
The vowels in the VC sequences ''-al- -il- -ul-''  are often very short, or even inaudible, when the preceding consonant is a plosive (''p t k'').
 
 
 
====Allophones of syllables beginning with /m/====


===Allophones of vowel sequences===
The vowel sequences ''ia ii iu'' are pronounced ['''ya yi yu'''], and this is reflected in Romanization.  Likewise, ''ua ui uu'' are pronounced ['''wa wi wu'''.]  In the case of ''ii'' and ''uu'', the on-glide is weak but still distinctly present, and the second vowel is lowered slightly.  Note that the Romanization here uses '''v''' for IPA /w/ and '''y''' for IPA /j/.


Triple vowel sequences are resolved by starting from the right.  For example, ''tiui'' can only be ['''tiwi'''], never [*tyui].  And ''kiiii'' is ['''kyiyi'''].


====Allophones of syllables beginning with /n/====
The vowel [a] changes very little in any context.  The sequence ''aa'' is simply ['''a:'''], and the sequences ''ai au'' raise the /a/ only slightly.  It is common to find long sequences of vowels, including /a/, especially in derived words. Here, the same rules are followed, and thus ''aaa'', ''aaaa'', and so on, are simply more lengthened versions of /a/, with the speaker's volume and pitch the same throughout the entire vowel sequence. Thus Late Andanese ''aa'' resembles Play /ā/ more than it resembles Play /aa/.


====Allophones of syllables beginning with /ŋ/====
===Allophones of VC sequences===
When a  syllable beginning with /h/ follows a syllable beginning with a stop,    the [h] spreads backwards over the vowel of that preceding syllable, making it voiceless. Thus, '''puha'''  sounds like [pu̥a] or [pʰwa].


The VC sequence ''-um-'' is pronounced like a syllabic ['''m̩'''] unless the preceding consonant is another labial.


====Rate of speech====
The VC sequence ''-un-'' is pronounced like a syllabic ['''n̩'''] unless the preceding consonant is one of ''t n l''.
Andanese is spoken so quickly that speakers of neighboring languages perceive it as a harsh, guttural language consisting mostly of consonants, especially /l h k/, the three most common consonants in the language.  However, even though /s/ is rare, it is the loudest consonant in the language, and speakers of neighboring languages often perceive Andanese to be rich in /s/ as well.


====Voiceless sounds====
The VC sequence ''-uŋ-'' is pronounced as a syllabic ['''ŋ̍'''] unless the preceding consonant is one of ''k ŋ''.
The voiceless consonants /k s p t/ are always voiceless unaspirated, even between vowels in rapid speech.


==Grammar==
==Grammar==
Andanese uses prefixes for inflection and suffixes<ref>Are these really suffixes?</ref> for derivation. There are no exceptions to this pattern, despite the strong influence of the many Gold-family languages surrounding Andanese, which use suffixes and infixes, but never prefixes.  There are still some infixes inherited from Old Andanese, but because they can only infect the prefixes, Late Andanese is best described as a language that has tables of related prefixes, with one row of prefixes for each infix, rather than a system combining prefixes and infixes.
Andanese uses prefixes for inflection, and bound  '''postbases''' for derivation. There are no exceptions to this pattern, despite the strong influence of the many Gold-family languages surrounding Andanese, which use suffixes and infixes, but never prefixes.  There are still some infixes inherited from Old Andanese, but because they can only infect the prefixes, Late Andanese is best described as a language that has tables of related prefixes, with one row of prefixes for each infix, rather than a system combining prefixes and infixes.


There is no morphological distinction between nouns and verbs in Andanese, but there is a fixed word order of Subject-Object-Verb, showing that verbs and nouns cannot be considered the same part of speech.  In this respect Andanese resembles isolating languages such as Chinese and to a lesser extent English.
There is no morphological distinction between nouns and verbs in Andanese, but there is a fixed word order of Subject-Object-Verb, showing that verbs and nouns cannot be considered the same part of speech.  In this respect Andanese resembles isolating languages such as Chinese and to a lesser extent English.
Line 142: Line 107:


===Private verbs===
===Private verbs===
:''Note, it is most likely that the private verbs will depend on only the agent, and will exclude humans and inanimate objects. Therefore the scope is much less.''
Andanese preserves the '''private verbs''' of Tapilula, which also persisted in the [[Gold language]] but were dropped in all of Gold's descendants, along with the classifier system itself.  Private verbs are morphemes, usually only one or two syllables long, whose meaning depends on the preceding classifier prefixes.  For example, there exists a verb '''vutami''' "to gallop, run quickly on all fours".  This verb begins with ''vu-'', which is also the first syllable of the word for horse.  Thus, if a horse is the subject, there is no need to repeat its classifier prefix before the verb; the classifier prefix is already there.  If a man is galloping, one would say
Andanese preserves the '''private verbs''' of Tapilula, which also persisted in the [[Gold language]] but were dropped in all of Gold's descendants, along with the classifier system itself.  Private verbs are morphemes, usually only one or two syllables long, whose meaning depends on the preceding classifier prefixes.  For example, there exists a verb '''vutami''' "to gallop, run quickly on all fours".  This verb begins with ''vu-'', which is also the first syllable of the word for horse.  Thus, if a horse is the subject, there is no need to repeat its classifier prefix before the verb; the classifier prefix is already there.  If a man is galloping, one would say


Line 157: Line 123:
::The horse ''is running like a human'' (on its hind legs only).
::The horse ''is running like a human'' (on its hind legs only).


Note that the neuter prefix ''gi-'', rather than the masculine prefix ''ki-'', is used whenever animals are personified, regardless of the syntactical gender of the animal.
The persistence of verbs like these is the main reason why Late Andanese has so many noun classes for specific animals such as turtles and rabbits with few nouns but larger numbers of verbs.
 
====Gender differences in private verbs====
The masculine prefix ''ki-'' is also the prefix for weapons and violent actions.  By contrast, the feminine prefix ''hi-'' pairs mostly with intransitives and passive objects, particularly those associated with the ocean.  This means that, in many sentences where the subject is a male human being, speakers must actually insert an extra syllable to clarify that the subject is ''not'' acting violently against the object.  For example, the prebasic root '''kapu''' means "hand, arm, fingers" (more precise definitions require compounds).  Given the same noun '''kilatu''' "man" used above, and the neuter noun '''ilati''' "door", one can say
:'''Kilatu kiilati ''kikapu''.'''
::The man ''punched'' the door.
 
Whereas the same verb, paired with the feminine subject '''hinuma''' "nurse", gives
:'''Hinuma hiilati ''hikapu''.'''
::The nurse ''opened'' the door.


;NOTE, FIND A BETTER EXAMP, THIS JUST MEANS 'HAND' AND COULD ALSO MEAN TO CLOSE.


;NOTE, AGAIN, VERB TENSE MARKERS ARE NOT YET ON HERE.
Note that the neuter prefix ''gi-'', rather than the masculine  ''ki-'' or feminine ''hi-'', is used whenever animals are personified, regardless of the semantic gender of the animal.


===Use of classifiers to derive new words===
===Use of classifiers to derive new words===
New words can be formed by copying a word from one class to another.  For example, in Old Andanese, the word for vampire was '''kʷĭhe'''.  It was used without a classifier prefix, as were many words for living beings (gender at this time was often marked by internal consonant changes). However, today, the commonest use of this morpheme is in the word pair '''gipihi''' "sharp tooth; canine" and '''kipihi''' "to bite".  Thus, a noun for a living being became a noun for just a distinctive trait of that being by simply changing the classifier prefix.
New words can be formed by copying a word from one class to another.  For example,   there exists a  word pair of '''gipihi''' "sharp tooth; canine" and '''kipihi''' "to bite".  Thus, a noun became a verb by simply changing the classifier prefix.
 
 
 
For some classes, the entire vocabulary can be assumed to be copiable.  For example, ''gi-'' nouns denoting body parts pair with ''ki-'' verbs denoting the action of striking or hitting something with that body part, and with ''mi-'' denoting that body part served as food.  For example, the word for thigh is '''gihuliti''', but somebody's thighs served for dinner could be called


:''' ''Yahuliti'' yatulihi.'''
For some classes, the entire vocabulary can be assumed to be copiable.  As above, ''gi-'' nouns denoting body parts pair with ''ki-'' verbs denoting the action of striking or hitting something with that body part, and with ''mi-'' denoting that body part served as food.  
::''Human thigh'' with spices.


Likewise, the word for a young student, '''tukuu''', is actually derived from the word for book, '''ikuu''', by changing the prefix from '''i-''', which denotes handheld objects, to '''tu-''', which marks the "human children" noun class.
Likewise, the word for a young student, '''tukuu''', is actually derived from the word for book, '''ikuu''', by changing the prefix from '''i-''', which denotes handheld objects, to '''tu-''', which marks the "human children" noun class.


===Derivation of words===
===Derivation of words===
Andanese is a head-initial language.  That is, within a word, a classifier prefix will come first, giving the general broad meaning of the word, and each additional morpheme will narrow the meaning of the word further.  This is the opposite of surrounding languages like [[Pabappa]], where it is the last morpheme within a word that defines the general broad meaning of the word.  Thus Andanese could be described as having a '''taxonomic vocabulary'''.  Someone hearing the first part of a word will not know its precise meaning, but will have a general idea of what it might be.  But someone hearing only the end of a word will have no idea which of the many possible categories of the vocabulary the word belongs to.
Andanese is a head-initial language.  That is, within a word, a classifier prefix will come first, giving the general broad meaning of the word, and each additional morpheme will narrow the meaning of the word further.  This is the opposite pattern of most surrounding languages.  Thus Andanese could be described as having a '''taxonomic vocabulary'''.  Someone hearing the first part of a word will not know its precise meaning, but will have a general idea of what it might be.  But someone hearing only the end of a word will have no idea which of the many possible categories of the vocabulary the word belongs to.


An exception to the rule that compounds are head-initial (i.e. "ears of corn") is that if one party is animate,  the animate partner goes first (i.e. "horse ears").
An exception to the rule that compounds are head-initial (i.e. "ears of corn") is that if one party is animate,  the animate partner goes first; thus from '''vukia''' "horse" comes '''vukiakiki''' "horse ears".<ref>this probably isn't the reason why gold switched, though, because in goldnthe anime pRtenr oftem.goes last</ref>
:this probably isn't the reason why gold switched, though, because in goldnthe anime pRtenr oftem.goes last




Line 205: Line 155:


====Polysemy====
====Polysemy====
Classifier prefixes with more than one meaning will have more than one titular word.  Since ''ka-'' also means "insect", one finds words such as '''kahunu''' and '''kakui''', both meaning "insect, arthropod" with little difference in meaning.
Classifier prefixes with more than one meaning will have more than one titular word.  Since ''ka-'' also means "insect", the word '''kakui''', meaning "insect, arthropod" is also considered a titular word.


===Dropping of classifiers in compounds===
===Dropping of classifiers in compounds===
Compounds of two nouns generally drop the classifier from the second noun, using semantics to disambiguate the possible meanings.  For example, '''hikala''' "seashell" and '''lakala''' "bear" share the same two-syllable root, and have different noun classifier prefixes.  But the compound word '''pugikala''', which adds ''pugi'' "claw", can only be used to mean "bear's claw" because seashells do not have claws.  If a speaker did want to specifically say "seashell claw", then they would use ''pugi puhikala''.
Compounds of two nouns generally drop the classifier from the second noun, using semantics to disambiguate the possible meanings.  For example, '''hikala''' "seashell" and '''lakala''' "bear" share the same two-syllable root, and have different noun classifier prefixes.  But the compound word '''pugikala''', which adds ''pugi'' "claw", can only be used to mean "bear's claw" because seashells do not have claws.  If a speaker did want to specifically say "seashell claw", then they would use ''pugi puhikala''.


This rule has some exceptions.  However, dropping the second classifier is mandatory when it is the same as the first classifier or of the same syntactic field.  In fact, some teachers insert implicit null morphemes before every element of a compound which agree with the first classifier, saying that, for example, there exists a secondary Andanese word '''pukala''' "bear" alongside '''lakala''', which is never used in bare form but is called up when speakers create compound words such as the above ''pugikala''.  This theory is one way of explaining the limits of which compounds are allowed to drop the classifiers and which are not.  However, a dictionary based on this theory would list over a million words, most of which would be unusable duplicates of others.
Dropping the second classifier is mandatory when it is the same as the first classifier or of the same syntactic field.  In fact, some teachers posit null morphemes before each element of a compound, each of which agrees with the outermost classifier.  Thus, for example, they analyze ''pugikala'' "bear claw" as ''pugi pukala'', and claim this proves that there exists a secondary Andanese word '''pukala''' "bear" alongside '''lakala''', which is never used in bare form but is called up when speakers create compound words such as the above ''pugikala''.  This theory is one way of explaining the limits of which compounds are allowed to drop the classifiers and which are not.  However, a dictionary based on this theory would list over a million words, most of which would be unusable duplicates of others.
 
:''Note, the morphemes here may be in the wrong order, since animacy overrides head-first construction.''


====Locative and thematic classifiers====
====Locative and thematic classifiers====
Line 216: Line 168:


====Animacy====
====Animacy====
Inanimate objects have classifiers that do not change for syntactical active or passive roles.  Since the subject of a sentence is always animate, however, their classifier is always buried underneath another classifier that repeats the subject's classifier.  Note that there are no sound changes; if two vowels come together, they are still pronounced as separate syllables.  Since this happens also to the verb, often a sentence will consist of three alliterative words.  Thus one can say
Inanimate objects have classifiers that do not change for syntactical active or passive roles.  Since the subject of a sentence is always animate, however, their classifier is always buried underneath another classifier that repeats the subject's classifier.  Note that there are no sound changes; if two vowels come together, they are still pronounced separately.  Since this happens also to the verb, often a sentence will consist of three alliterative words.  Thus one can say


:'''Tulata ''tuinuhu'' tuyula.'''
:'''Tulata ''tuinuhu'' tuyula.'''
::The student threw ''the torch''.
::The student threw ''the torch''.
Here, while a clever student might expect the sequence /tui/ to contract to /pi/, as it historically did in many similar constructions, the morphemes remain separate and the sequence is thus pronounced [twi].


===List of common noun classifiers===
===List of common noun classifiers===
All noun classifiers are one syllable long, but some classifiers can precede other classifiers and create what are effectively two-syllable prefixes.
All noun classifiers are one syllable long, but some '''second-order classifiers''' can follow other classifiers and create what are effectively two-syllable prefixes.  Even so, the outermost classifier in such a word is the one that echoes throughout the sentence, not the pair.


'''a''' : roads, streets
'''a''' : roads, streets
Line 228: Line 182:
'''la''' : large land animals
'''la''' : large land animals


'''i''' : handheld objects; small land animals<ref>Not historical ... extracted from ''iku'' "bird" ... therefore, this does not have cognates even in other Andanic languages</ref>
'''i''' : handheld objects  


'''ha''' : to worship; needle
'''ha''' : to worship; needle
Line 246: Line 200:
'''sa''' : love
'''sa''' : love


'''pa''' : words loaned from early Pabappa
'''pa''' : some words for loose-fitting clothes


'''ta''' :
'''ta''' :
Line 266: Line 220:
'''pu''' : succulent fruit (doublet of ''tu-''); sexual reproduction, obscene body parts (only when following another classifier such as ''li-'')
'''pu''' : succulent fruit (doublet of ''tu-''); sexual reproduction, obscene body parts (only when following another classifier such as ''li-'')


'''hu''' : fire; celestial objects; insects; clothes, "shaped like"; hair of the head, back of body;  
'''hu''' : fire; celestial objects; insects; tight clothes, "shaped like"; hair of the head, back of body;  


'''tu''' : small plants; children; blood, bodily humors
'''tu''' : small plants; children; blood, bodily humors
Line 284: Line 238:
'''ki''' : verbs of motion (corresponds to li- body parts); weapon, claw; men, boys; darkness, night, sleep; some buildings
'''ki''' : verbs of motion (corresponds to li- body parts); weapon, claw; men, boys; darkness, night, sleep; some buildings


The sequences /ja ji ju va vi vu/ have taken on the role of pseudo-classifiers, since even in Classical Andanese their pronunciation was already monosyllabic:
The sequences /ja ji ju va vi vu/ have taken on the role of pseudo-classifiers, since in initial position they are monosyllabic:


'''ya''' : some grasses; pineapple, large fruit; meat, food
'''ya''' : some grasses; pineapple, large fruit; meat, food
Line 297: Line 251:


'''vu''' : rain, water; horse, rideable animal
'''vu''' : rain, water; horse, rideable animal
If the root begins with a single vowel, the syllables /yi yu vi vu/ reverse to [iy iv uy uv].  If the root word begins with /i/ or /u/ followed by another vowel, however, the normal pronunciation is restored.
However, sequences such as /sia/, /nua/, and so on have not been reanalyzed as single syllables, and cannot serve as classifier prefixes. 


Note that there are many monosyllabic morphemes that are not classifiers.  For example '''i''' means "shoulder", but is only used with a body part classifier ''li-''.
Note that there are many monosyllabic morphemes that are not classifiers.  For example '''i''' means "shoulder", but is only used with a body part classifier ''li-''.


===Consonant-based gender system===
===Consonant-based gender system===
Andanese inherited the consonant-based gender system from [[Tapilula]]. Due to sound changes, the consonants do not line up well with those of the parent language.  An Andanese innovation was that, despite the Andanese language in general being nonfusional, gender markers used as prefixes affected the words they attached to.  This is actually not truly an innovation, because the Tapilula language did this as well, but Andanese got rid of other such fusional aspects of grammar while retaining the gender "shaping" process.  The genders are:
Andanese inherited the consonant-based gender system from [[Tapilula]].   The genders are:


'''li''': Babies; also used for humans of indeterminate age and gender (accusative is '''na-''')
'''li''': Babies; also used for humans of indeterminate age and gender (accusative is '''na-''')
Line 319: Line 277:




As in Gold, all objects found in bodies of water are grammatically feminine even if syntactically masculine. <ref>c.f. Russian </ref>
As in Gold, all objects found in bodies of water are grammatically feminine even if syntactically masculine.   


A very small number of words show relics of the older internal mutations that marked gender in the [[Tapilula]] language.  In Tapilula, classifier prefixes were dropped in more situations than they are in Late Andanese, and therefore the existence of separate stems for different genders was necessary.  In Late Andanese, only a few such words survive, such as the word pair '''kilitu''' "king" and '''hiihu''' "queen".  Even though the stems are different, the use of the prefixes is mandatory in Late Andanese.
A very small number of words show relics of the older internal mutations that marked gender in the [[Tapilula]] language.  In Tapilula, classifier prefixes were dropped in more situations than they are in Late Andanese, and therefore the existence of separate stems for different genders was necessary.  In Late Andanese, only a few such words survive, such as the word pair '''kilitu''' "king" and '''hiihu''' "queen".  Even though the stems are different, the use of the prefixes is mandatory in Late Andanese.
Line 328: Line 286:
The masculine noun class, marked by ''ki-'' in an agent role, is the most changeable of all noun class prefixes, and the only one whose accusative form depends on the noun class of the agent acting on them.  That is, a man stung by a bee will have a different noun class than a man kissed by a woman, in addition to the noun class of the agent which is stacked before the noun class for the man.  Furthermore, the agent and patient forms merge before certain vowel-initial words, and the agent form also takes a special form when the patient is male.
The masculine noun class, marked by ''ki-'' in an agent role, is the most changeable of all noun class prefixes, and the only one whose accusative form depends on the noun class of the agent acting on them.  That is, a man stung by a bee will have a different noun class than a man kissed by a woman, in addition to the noun class of the agent which is stacked before the noun class for the man.  Furthermore, the agent and patient forms merge before certain vowel-initial words, and the agent form also takes a special form when the patient is male.


The various interrelated patterns are all due to etymology.  The original form of the agent prefix was ''tə-'' in pre-Andanese, and the patient was ''hə-''.  In early Old Andanese, this vowel either disappeared or changed to /i/, depending on environment.  The /t/ soon changed to /k/, part of a general shift.  Then, the sequences ''ki hi'' merged as '''s-''' before a vowel-initial word.  But this change did not spread to words that became vowel-initial later on, meaning that only a few vowel-initial Late Andanese words trigger this assimilation.  Any sequence of /hihi/ resulting from these shifts reduced to /hi/ by a general process of analogy.  This occurred when a verb had a female agent and a male patient, but only when one of the other rules above did not force the inner /hi/ to contract into /s/.
The various interrelated patterns are all due to etymology.  The original form of the agent prefix was ''tə-'' in pre-Andanese, and the patient was ''hə-''.  In early Old Andanese, this vowel either disappeared or changed to /i/, depending on environment.  The /t/ soon changed to /k/, part of a general shift.  Then, the sequences ''ki hi'' merged as '''s-''' before a vowel-initial word.  But this change did not spread to words that became vowel-initial later on, meaning that only a few vowel-initial Late Andanese words trigger this assimilation, one of which is /atu/ "soldier"Further, although the nominative forms for male and female soldiers are both /satu/, in the accusative they diverge, with the masculine form being still /satu/ while the feminine form changes to /miatu/.
 
Any sequence of /hihi/ resulting from these shifts reduced to /hi/ by a general process of analogy.  This occurred when a verb had a female agent and a male patient, but only when one of the other rules above did not force the inner /hi/ to contract into /s/. Lastly, the /hi/ disappears before many consonant-initial roots, as it evolved early on into a simple /h/ in these positions, which was then dropped. 


Sample sentences below illustrate the different possibilities:  
Sample sentences below illustrate the different possibilities:  
:''' Satu'''.
::Male soldier.


:'''Kuinau ''kusatu'' kutakiu.'''
:'''Kuinau ''kusatu'' kutakiu.'''
Line 337: Line 300:
:'''Hiuma ''hisatu'' hitakiihumi.'''
:'''Hiuma ''hisatu'' hitakiihumi.'''
::The woman hit ''the soldier''.
::The woman hit ''the soldier''.
:''' Kikapi'''.
:Male farmer.
:''' Nianu ''nikapi''  nipihi.'''
::The snake bit ''the male farmer''.
: '''  Hikapi'''.
:: Female farmer.
: ''' Nianu ''nimikapi'' nipihi.'''
::The snake bit ''the female farmer''.


===Second order classifiers and length of words===
===Second order classifiers and length of words===
Line 347: Line 322:
Some syllables can be used either as primary or secondary classifiers.  For example, the ''-lu-'' above can only appear after a human gender classifier, but ''ku-'' "arrow, sharp object" often follows ''i-'' "handheld object" to create words for handheld weapons such as '''ikukivuni''' "slicing knife" and '''ikukuhigi''' "one-handed sword".
Some syllables can be used either as primary or secondary classifiers.  For example, the ''-lu-'' above can only appear after a human gender classifier, but ''ku-'' "arrow, sharp object" often follows ''i-'' "handheld object" to create words for handheld weapons such as '''ikukivuni''' "slicing knife" and '''ikukuhigi''' "one-handed sword".


===Private verbs===
===Other information about private verbs===
:''Note, it is most likely that the private verbs will depend on only the agent, and will exclude humans and inanimate objects. Therefore the scope is much less.''
Andanese preserves the '''private verbs''' of its parent language Tapilula, whereas in the Gold branch of the family they disappeared early on.   
Andanese preserves the '''private verbs''' of its parent language Tapilula, whereas in the Gold branch of the family they disappeared early on.   


Line 358: Line 334:
Through cultural osmosis, similar phenomena later reappeared in some of the Gold languages, particularly those most closely culturally linked to the Andanese.
Through cultural osmosis, similar phenomena later reappeared in some of the Gold languages, particularly those most closely culturally linked to the Andanese.


===Archaic traits===
===Syllable harmony===
Andanese has many unsuual traits.<ref>Search  "GRAND TRUTH PARTICLE.doc" for sentences like ''These were by about 3000 AD reduced to five: Sweet, Sleep, Feminine, Masculine, and Child. '' (Five genders.)</ref> For example, in an early stage of the language, ''ala'' meant "children, people falling down, animals, abstract concepts, claw-hands raised, swinging arms".  Unusual meaning shifts also have taken place; '''aka''' means "baby; blocking" and is not considered to simply be a set of two homophonous words.
Due to historical sound changes such as /pua > pʷo > po > pu/, where a syllable beginning in a vowel was completely swallowed up by the syllable preceding it, Late Andanese evolved '''syllable harmony''' where most related languages evolved consonant harmony or nothing at all.  Any ''a'' in the second element of a compound word could  change to match the syllable of the classifier prefix. Often, a further classifier prefix will then appear before the original one since the meaning of the word has changed.  Alternatively, the second word will agree with the first syllable of the first word in the compound rather than its classifier, since the grammar allows for the reinterpretation of originally independent words as sequences of classifier and short word.


There was also a "grand truth particle" at the beginning of every sentenceIt was omitted if the sentence was true, but ''la'' if the sentence was false; if false, any ''-a-'' in the sentence (that is, /a/ with no consonant) would change to ''-la-'' to match the truth particle.
This process is not used in people's names, as scholars consider the use of such vacuous alliteration to be a sign of an unintelligent mind, but it is used in common nouns and some place namesFor  example, a road to a store may be called '''ipunapupu''', the compound of '''i-''' "road, open place", '''puna''' "store", and '''aa''' "road".  Note also that the two main morphemes are in the reverse order from what one might expect, because the reinterpretation of the word for store as a one-off classifier makes the compound head-final instead of head-initial as in most compounds.  This process is reminiscent of [[Play_language#The_four_types_of_compounds|Play's head-initial compounds]], which also require a closing morpheme to restore the "proper" morpheme order.


Body parts and certain other inalienables change depending on the gender of the referentHowever, whole syllables change, not just consonants.
==Orthography and contact with other languages==
Andanese has had several scripts.  Commonly, the Andanese wrote their language with one of a series of artistic syllabaries, each with 30 glyphs, which were based on square tiles.  Of these, the commonest one was based on squares with 90° and 45° angles inside themIn multi-line texts, the boundaries of the squares would often be omitted, resulting in a shape that resembles a  Tangram puzzle.


==Orthography==
===Main scripts===
Andanese has had several scripts.   
The primary script was derived from the 100-glyph [[Tapilula]] syllabary in a highly irregular manner.  As the phonology contracted and evolved, many glyphs were dropped while others were repurposedThe actual original glyph derivations are:


The oddest looking one, "batam" (an exonym), was not a script at all but a means of drawing objects with the angular shapes of one of the many 30-letter syllabaries. It is similar to ASCII art.  Thus, "words" made from Batam were generally more than one line long and were absurdly long even comapred to the rest of Andanese.  Nevertheless, Batam inspired the symbololology of earlyt Khull;s, so e.g. a l;etter that looked like a pineapple came to mean "pineapple", meaning that Khulls ironically developed words even shorter than it had already had by using the same method ANdfanese had used to make its already long words even longer.
  [see red notebook]


Far more commonly, though, the Andanese wrote their language with one of a series of artistic syllabaries, each with 30 glyphs, which are based on square tiles.  Of these, the commonest one was based on squares with 90° and 45° angles inside them.  In multi-line texts, the boundaries of the squares would often be omitted, resulting in a shape that resembles a  Tangram puzzle.
Because most of the glyphs were taken from the top half of the syllabary, many  of the original shapes did not appear, and the glyphs were smoothed into simpler designs over time.


==Adoption of foreign loanwords==
During the last few decades when Late Andanese still had tones, they were spelled with the inherited glyphs for /va vi vu ya yi yu/When Andanese lost its tones, these glyphs did not recover their original values, nor were new glyphs created, and therefore these sequences continued to be spelled with digraphs /ua ui uu ia ii iu/.
In its early history, Andanese adopted many loanwords from surrounding languages, mostly of the [[Gold language|Gold]] and [[Haswaraba]] familiesBy the time of Late Andanese, however, the speakers of the language lived only in [[Paba]] and therefore were surrounded by speakers of [[Babakiam]].  Andanese thus began to loan words from Babakiam, and stopped adopting words from all of the other languages, including the other branches of Andanese.


During this time, many foreign peoples settled in Paba, bringing languages such as [[Khulls]] into contact with Andanese.  However, the Andanese generally distrusted these peoples, preferring to associate with Pabaps, and therefore few words from these languages were adopted into AndaneseAn additional factor was the great divergence in phonology between Andanese and Khulls: whereas Andanese has only 30 syllables, Khulls has 35 consonants, 5 vowels, and 5 tones, and allows consonant clusters whereas Andanese does not. Although Babakiam's phonology is also quite different from that of Andanese, the two languages can more easily share vocabulary without merging many sounds into one.
====Phonetic scope====
The syllabary had only thirty glyphs because Andanese teachers considered their language to have only thirty syllablesAt first, these thirty syllables included two sets of symbols for /a i u/ depending on whether the hiatus was primordial or of recent origin; this had important effects on grammar.  They had no glyphs for /sa si su/ because they considered these syllables to be underlyingly /hia hii hiu/But the final stage of the language shifted the spelling to a phonetic one incorporating the earlier hiatus  glyphs as /s/ glyphs. (That is, the glyphs themselves were repurposed, meaning that /a₂ i₂ u₂/ came to spell /sa si su/.)


===Loanwords from Babakiam===
The teachers never considered the sequences /wa wi wu/ or /ya yi  yu/ to be proper syllables, even though they behaved as such in  the grammar.  Neither were sequences such as /mia mii miu/, whose surface pronunciation was monosyllabic, incorporated into the syllabary with single glyphsThe earlier practice of not writing /s/ made sense from this standpoint, as the distribution of /sa si su/ is very similar to that of sequences like /mia mii miu/ and unlike that of more common syllables like /ka ki ku/; the change happened when the script came to be widely used to write foreign languages in which /s/ was a common sound and could not be analyzed as an allophone of an underlying CV sequence.
Unusually, loans from Babakiam are generally borrowed using a different phoneme matching than other loanwords.  Baba's /f/, /b/, and /p/ are all merged as /p/, and long vowels are borrowed as doublesOften, these words are assigned the classifier prefix '''pa-''' regardless of meaning, which means that most Babakiam loanwords begin with /pa/.  Babakiam's /ž/ phoneme is borrowed as /l/.  All final consonants are dropped.


Bābā's loanwords into Andanese can be said to sound like an exaggerated form of Babakiam, characterized by words like '''papupipa''' "room, chamber" from Bābākiam ''pubipa''. 
===Minor scripts===
 
====Batam====
====Boomerang loans====
The oddest looking script, "batam" (an exonym), was not a script at all but a means of drawing objects with the angular shapes of one of the many 30-letter syllabaries.  
As Andanese culture died off, some words that Andanese had borrowed from Babakiam were loaned back into Babakiam, creating a large assortment of lexical doublets.  For example, Babakiam ''aafa'' "dark-haired" was borrowed into Andanese as '''paaapa''', and then borrowed back into Babakiam in this same form.  Babakiam generally borrows Andanese /l/ as /ž/, which means that /ž/ in boomerang loans is preserved intact.  The classifier prefix ''pa-'' is sometimes retained, as Babakiam often does with other prefixes.
 
==Diachronics==
===Persistence of Andanese words in other languages===
===Old Andanese===
Late Andanese loanwords in other languages are mostly pronounced according to the closest match in the recipient language's spelling, since knowledge of the original language disappeared shortly after the war in 4175.   Thus the pronunciation of these words varies from language to language even as the spelling has remained the same for thousands of years.  However, no tradition has introduced phonemes not directly descended from one of the 30 syllables in the Late Andanese language.
[[Old Andanese]]: /p m f t n l k g h q kʷ ŋʷ w qʷ/ for consonants, /a e i o u/ on two tones for vowels.  Note that Old Andanese preserved the lack of /s/ passed down from the parent language.  /f/ is usually analyzed as /hʷ/, and /w/ as /gʷ/, which means that all the fricatives (/h hʷ g gʷ/) are laryngeals.  (The letter "g" always indicates a fricative; ġ is used for the stop in related languages but does not occur in any stage of Andanese.)
 
Old Andanese /p/ and /t/ became voiced between vowels.
 
The language is very guttural compared to its neighbors; after all, it has 9 dorsal conosnants, but only 3 coronals and 2 labials (though five of the dorsals are labialized).  However, the syllable strucutre is almost entirely CV, so it does not sound quite as aggressive as one might think.
 
Note, also, that /l/ patterns grammatically as a dorsal consonant, which means that from the viewpoint of the Old Andanese speakers, there were only four non-dorsal consonants in the language: /p m t n/.
 
 
====Loss of tone====
The loss of tone was actually the very last change that occurred in Late Andanese.  Even as late as 3700 AD, there were still minimal pairs of words that differed only in tone, although the tones carried a very minor functional load since only one syllable per word could carry stress and many words were very long.  The final loss of tone came in part due to influence from the speakers of [[Babakiam]], whose nation was also home to most Andanese at the time.
 
==Culture and habitat==
Historically, the Andanese people have preferred to form parasitic colonies within other nations rather than to form nations of their own.  Early on, they established settlements in the empires of [[Thaoa]], [[Paba]], and [[Subumpam]].
 
===Physical attributes===
The Andanese people were the shortest people in the world, and also among the thinnest, giving them a body size far smaller than that of even their neighbors the [[Paba]]ps and the various dark-skinned aboriginal tribes living further west.  Their hair and skin color was variable, as they  had absorbed some local peoples, but did not travel much, meaning that the foreign genes for dark skin did not diffuse to the wider Andanese population.
 
The original Andanese settlers never had red hair, but some of the children of mixed marriages did. This is because red hair only appeared when the blonde hair genes combined with Andanese genes affecting the production of pigmented hair.<ref>That is, "redness" and "blonde hair" were controlled by different genes, as on Earth.</ref>  Since these children tended to identify as Andanese themselves, red hair came to be seen as an Andanese trait, although it was very rare even among them.  With the dispersal of the Andanese identity into surrounding tribes, red hair has become slightly more common but no longer marks out any specific tribe.
 
===Early Andanese settlement in Paba===
The Andanese invaded Paba early on.  But rather than start a war, they settled and established themselves as parasites who resisted Paba's attempts to encourage all people to work and live cooperatively.  While Pabaps worked and shared the fruits of their labor, Andanese people stole from the Pabaps, with weaker Andanese people relying mostly on stealth and stronger ones relying on brute force.
 
====Parasitic growth====
Andanese people living in Paba practiced what they called '''handful culture''', meaning that anything they were able to grab with their hands was considered their property.  Beginning around the year 1300, Paba transformed itself into a pacifistic economic power and reduced the size of its military and police force.  This worked well for the Andanese, who were very militaristic.  The Andanese people realized that they no longer would need to worry about famine since they could simply raid a farm whenever they needed food, or steal merchandise from a store.  The Pabaps realized they had a problem, and decided to enact a new tax to pay for food and merchandise in order to compensate people who had been raided by the nomadic Andanese gangs.  This new tax reduced the Pabap peoples' living standards, but increased their physical safety, as the Andanese people realized that victims they allowed to live would be restored to a state of lootability in the close future.
 
===Early Andanese settlement in Thaoa===
Thaoa was a kingdom that broke away from Paba in the year 1085 and became markedly more militaristic.  Unlike Paba, they banned all ethnic minorities from even living on Thaoa land, except as slaves.  But the Andanese were able to get the better of even the Thaoans because the Andanese were willing to endure a standard of living far worse than that of the average Thaoan, to the point of living in unheated caves even during winter in order to hide out from the police and army. In fact, the Andanese were able to exploit one of Thaoa's few weaknesses: the Thaoan military was so strong that they went hundreds of years between battles, and nearly always won on those rare occasions that they fought.  Thus, the average Andanese lived a harsher and more rugged life than even the soldiers in the Thaoan army, and found that they were able to get away with provocative behavior such as stealing Thaoan civilians' food and belongings and retreating into the mountains before the army or police could catch up with them.
 
===Early Andanese settlement in Subumpam===
In [[Subumpam]], Andanese people mostly moved to areas where Pabap people were the majority, and stayed within the Pabap cities rather than forming settlements in the wilderness.  As in Paba, the Andanese people in eastern Subumpam preyed on the Pabaps, robbing them of food and belongings in order to ensure that Andanese people would never need to work.  The Pabaps adapted to the presence of the Andanese by introducing communal ownership of land and property, so that Pabap people who were attacked by the Andanese could replace their stolen belongings by borrowing replacements from the wider community.
 
==History===
The Andanese arrived on the continent of Rilola around the year 0, at the same time as the other [[Tapilula]] tribes such as the ancestors of the Poswobs, Pabaps, Moonshines, and so on.  They were a distinct people however, and kept to themselves.  In fact, the Tapilula people branched early on into two groups: the Andanese, and everybody else.  The other tribes are merely subdivisions of the "Gold" tribe that includes all non-Andanese.
 
By the 1900s, Andanese were most strongly settled along the south coast where the weather was warmest.  For the most part, Andanese did not build their own nations; they settled in the nations of other people such as the [[Pabappa|Pabap]] people and the [[Subumpam|Subumpamese]].  They lived in the same territories as Pabaps but for the most part lived in separate cities. 
 
====Cultural traits of the early Andanese people====
The Pabap and Andanese cultures were a lot alike, and despite having different religions the people worshipped the same gods.  However, the Andanese were a very violent people, and stood out sharply from the pacifistic Pabaps they lived among.  Young Andanese boys were trained from an early age how to hold a sword, and by the time they could read they were strong enough to kill wild animals and defend their elderly and infirm relatives from hostile would-be invaders.  By contrast, young Pabap boys were trained from an early age how to plant trees and vegetable gardens, and how best to decorate their property with the most beautiful flowers they could afford so that people walking through their property could enjoy the scenery and stop to rest after they had eaten their fill of the fruits and vegetables in their Pabap neighbors' garden.   
 
The Andanese and Pabaps generally did not battle ''each other'', however, so having vampires living amongst them was not a problem for Pabaps.
 
====Early Andanese language====
See [[Old Andanese]].
 
====Later history====
Eventually, the Andanese settled amongst the Pabaps to such an extent that they redefined themselves as merely a tribe of Pabaps, or sometimes a collection of tribes.  This identity was still held to when the [[Swamp Kids]] took over the reins of power in the year 4149 and renamed their new empire '''Anzan''' after the Andanese that many of them were descended from.
 
===Culture===
Like other human peoples, the Andanese originated in the tropics, eating a diet of fish combined with tropical fruits such as pineapples and coconuts.  Around the year 4175, they were on the losing side of a war which left them with no land to call their own.  But some brave Andanese explorers spoke of an uninhabited island, thousands of miles to the north and still teeming with fish and plenty of land to live on.  And so the Andanese people who had only ever known tropical rainstorms and blistering heat came to live in the coldest part of the Northern Hemisphere, the icecapped island of Xema.  Others stayed behind and tried to hide behind their skills at learning new languages and creating new political parties; in 4165 the [[Bubbles]] were formed, and despite being Andanese in origin, the public face of the party was the Poswobs and Pabaps.
 
Andanese people tended not to build nations of their own; instead, they settled in nations developed by other peoples, chiefly the [[Pabappa|Pabap]] people and the [[Subumpam|Subumpamese]] living along the south coast of Rilola.  They also settled in [[Thaoa]]; in fact, Thaoa had more Andanese people per capita than any other nation to its west.  Soon the Andanese actually became a majority.  But the Andanese people here were more intimiately linked with Andanese people living in other nations, and did not generally seek to actually take over the Thaoa government.
 
Andanese share with Moonshines the unusual trait of having women be strongly taller than men from puberty onward.  However the Andanese in general are much shorter people than Moonshines; in fact they are the shortest people in the world.  This has led to much lower rates of outmarriage with other peoples and therefore much less spreading out of the tall-female trait into surrounding cultures.
 
Even though Andanese women are much taller than their men, this did not lead to the Andanese becoming a peaceful, feministic society the way the Pabaps and Poswobs around them did.  Arguably the constant feeling of helplessness experienced by adult Andanese men who could barely get through a day without injuring themselves on a solid object intended to be harmless, combined with the fact that they were the shorter sex among the shortest human tribe in the world, made Andanese men feel as though they were simply "waste" people whose lives had little worth and led to them frequently starting fights with other Andanese men, and even Andanese women.  Andanese women, despite being stronger than their men, rarely participated in violence either against other Andanese or against foreign peoples or animals.  Likewise Andanese people generally wore clothing that was intended to protect them from injury by sharp objects, rather than to keep them warm during winter.  Thus the throat and wrists were generally covered even if areas closer to the heart were not.


When the Andanese nation was destroyed, many Andanese families literally climbed up palm trees and began to live like monkeys.  They in essence became "soft monkeys" in the sense that they did not have the strong muscles or sharp teeth that their lifestyle required but tried to compensate by building sharp knives and other tools to protect them from nature.  But even here there was no relief, because the species of monkeys that dominated their territory actually fed on humans, even if only as a minor part of a primarily fruit-based diet.  Some Andanese decided to simply accept their fate and live amongst the monkeys that were eating them, but even with a high birthrate these people became fewer and fewer in number over time.  Even after the monkeys were defeated in a war of their own, the "tree people" never resettled the land or established a new human nation.  The remainder of the Andanese people had scattered by this time, either to other nations or to assimilate as a new tribe of Pabap speakers called Sonsona.  These people were still very weak, and no longer warlike, but they had a strong tribal identity despite lacking a language of their own, and therefore they are the only surviving descanedants of the Andanese today.
The [[Palli]] language, spoken by the southern half of the defeated [[Thaoa]] people, lost most of its inherited vocabulary and replaced them with loanwords from Late Andanese.


==Notes==
==Notes==
[[Category:Teppala]]
[[Category:Teppala]]
[[Category:Languages of Teppala]]

Latest revision as of 08:36, 10 November 2022

Around the year 4178, the last surviving Andanic language went extinct as an everyday spoken language, but its ornate writing system survived, and thus knowledge of the language persisted for thousands of years. This preserved language is called Late Andanese.

scratch pad

itanu inuhahuka inuimina inuihupuhu inuihuhatahupuunatata "road to a camp fire wood store"

The above sentence could be loosely glossed as "ROAD STORE WOOD FIRE CAMP", but this does not explain why the words get longer towards the end, and why the increase is much greater on the last word than on the steps in between. Andanese does not need twelve syllables to say "camp"; the last word in the clause must carry the agreement morphemes of all the others, and these are piled on both sides of the root.

Phonology

Late Andanese has only 12 phonemes: the consonants /l h k m n ŋ s p t/ and the vowels /a i u/. And of these, the consonants /s/ and /ŋ/ are rare because they originated primarily from sequences rather than single phonemes. Vowel sequences are allowed, but final consonants are not. Thus there are only 30 syllables in the language.


Consonants

Consonant frequency

The frequency of consonants is roughly the same as their order in the syllabary: /l h k m n p t ŋ s/. However, as detailed below, some consonants are more common before certain vowels, and it is more accurate to think of syllables rather than consonants and vowels as the minimal phonemic unit.

Vowels

Andanese has three vowels: /a i u/. There are no tones, stress, or length distinctions. Unlike most of the languages of the Gold family, Andanese is vowel-strong: its vowels influence the pronunciation of preceding and following consonants, but the consonants have no influence on the pronunciation of the vowels. Vowels can only change due to the influence of adjacent vowels: an /i/ or /u/ before another vowel (even the same vowel) will contract into a semivowel. Other than this, there are no significant allophones of the three vowels.

Another way that Andanese differs from the Gold languages is that, although /a/ is the commonest vowel, it is noticeably less common by comparison to the other two vowels than it is in most Gold languages. The three vowels are all well represented in Andanese speech.

Phonology and vocabulary

With only 30 syllables in the language, many sentences, even with basic vocabulary items, are highly repetitive. For example hahaha is the word for "hat, cap, headgear" (ha- "shaped like" + haha "hair of the head").

Consonants besides /l h k/ are less frequent. The overrepresentation of the consonants /l h k/ (in roughly that order) is part of the reason why Andanese words are often so long even compared to other languages such as Play that have small phonologies.

Expressive wordplay

Another reason why words in Late Andanese are so long is the use of extensive wordplay.

Boys' names

Boys' names were often extremely long-winded due to the deliberate repetition of similar-sounding words and syllables. Simple reduplication was avoided, but since the language had many homophones, long sequences of identical syllables piled up even so. By omitting classifier prefixes, the inventory of homophones increased vastly, with dozens of possible interpretations for each morpheme. Thus, even two boys with the same name could claim unique derivations.

The repetitive names were inspired by the Late Andanese process of syllable harmony, in which a whole syllable in a word changes to that of its classifier prefix. But, as with reduplication, Andanese names avoided syllable harmony, since parents considered it unimpressive to coin a name whose internal assonance was simply due to their language's grammar. Instead, every morpheme in each name was a proper word on its own and could be interpreted in a fixed number of ways.

One common name, Kukukukukukuku, was an extreme example, since it consisted of the thematic syllable /ku/ seven times in a row. All speakers recognized kuku as their word for "leg, run", but from there, thousands of possible interpretations of the name opened up, since, with classifier prefixes removed, the same kuku could also mean "canopy; place high up", "night", or "road"; meanwhile, the remaining ku could also mean "forest", "hand", "arrow (weapon)", or "to expose, show publicly". Thus one boy with the name Kukukukukukuku might tell friends his name meant "running down trails at night" (kuku kukuku kuku; trails being roads in a forest) while another would say that it meant "sitting on a treetop with arrows in hand" (kuku kukuku ku ku, treetops being high places in a forest). Stress was placed only on the final syllable, irrespective of morpheme boundaries, just as in the language as a whole.

A more common name template would intersperse a dominant thematic syllable with several minor ones. Kaaaaaaya was a common boy's name, where the thematic syllable is /a/ and the two minor syllables are /ka/ and /i/. Haaaaaaaaaaa was also a very common boy's name. Taaaamaaaaaamaaaaa was less common. Aaaaaaaaaaatataaaa is another common boy's name. Each /a/ is a separate syllable. The longest boy's name in common use was Kakakaaakakatakakakakakakakakaka.

Some names had two thematic syllables; an example of this was Matamataamatatataamaataaaatata. The bare /a/ was common enough in the language as a whole that its repetition here was not considered to create a third thematic syllable.

Some names had abbreviated forms. For example, Lilalaayilalalalalalaa was another very long boy's name, but could be abbreviated to Lalaaalai.

A classroom of children would often sit quietly when a teacher called out their names, as though hearing lottery numbers read off, since a boy with a lengthy name would need to listen for quite a while to know whether the student being called up to the front of the class was him or his friend whose name differed only on the thirteenth syllable.

Girls' names

Girls' names typically did not use repeating syllables, and were thus typically shorter, but parents often chose long names that did not use repetition. The number of girls' names in use was always much greater than the number of boys' names, and many parents coined novel names never used before.

Other proper names

The same pattern of repeating syllables showed up in placenames such as Mipatatatatai (Land of the Ruling Children of Tata) and party names such as Laaatilalatitiaa (Rusted Pearls).

Allophony and sandhi

NOTE: This section was much longer until Nov 10 2022. See edit history for details.

Late Andanese has a much quicker speech tempo than the Play language that occupies the same territory. This is because the most common consonants in Play are the bilabials /p b m/ and the most common vowel is the open low vowel /a/; the time required to move the lips slows down the pronunciation of syllables containing these sounds, and sets the rhythm for the language, so other syllables are also pronounced slowly.

By contrast, the most common consonant sounds in Andanese are /l h k/, and therefore can be paired with any vowel without slowing down the speech tempo. Although Andanese has labial consonants /p m/, they are not nearly so common as in Play, and so the tongue and lips move independently during the pronunciation of each syllable far more often than in Play.

Allophones of vowel sequences

The vowel sequences ia ii iu are pronounced [ya yi yu], and this is reflected in Romanization. Likewise, ua ui uu are pronounced [wa wi wu.] In the case of ii and uu, the on-glide is weak but still distinctly present, and the second vowel is lowered slightly. Note that the Romanization here uses v for IPA /w/ and y for IPA /j/.

Triple vowel sequences are resolved by starting from the right. For example, tiui can only be [tiwi], never [*tyui]. And kiiii is [kyiyi].

The vowel [a] changes very little in any context. The sequence aa is simply [a:], and the sequences ai au raise the /a/ only slightly. It is common to find long sequences of vowels, including /a/, especially in derived words. Here, the same rules are followed, and thus aaa, aaaa, and so on, are simply more lengthened versions of /a/, with the speaker's volume and pitch the same throughout the entire vowel sequence. Thus Late Andanese aa resembles Play /ā/ more than it resembles Play /aa/.

Allophones of VC sequences

When a syllable beginning with /h/ follows a syllable beginning with a stop, the [h] spreads backwards over the vowel of that preceding syllable, making it voiceless. Thus, puha sounds like [pu̥a] or [pʰwa].

The VC sequence -um- is pronounced like a syllabic [] unless the preceding consonant is another labial.

The VC sequence -un- is pronounced like a syllabic [] unless the preceding consonant is one of t n l.

The VC sequence -uŋ- is pronounced as a syllabic [ŋ̍] unless the preceding consonant is one of k ŋ.

Grammar

Andanese uses prefixes for inflection, and bound postbases for derivation. There are no exceptions to this pattern, despite the strong influence of the many Gold-family languages surrounding Andanese, which use suffixes and infixes, but never prefixes. There are still some infixes inherited from Old Andanese, but because they can only infect the prefixes, Late Andanese is best described as a language that has tables of related prefixes, with one row of prefixes for each infix, rather than a system combining prefixes and infixes.

There is no morphological distinction between nouns and verbs in Andanese, but there is a fixed word order of Subject-Object-Verb, showing that verbs and nouns cannot be considered the same part of speech. In this respect Andanese resembles isolating languages such as Chinese and to a lesser extent English.


Classifier prefixes

All words have classifiers, except for a few that are sometimes considered to have a null morpheme as their classifier. For example, the classifier gi- means "humans, human body parts". Its accusative form is na-. However, classifiers for inanimate objects do not have distinct forms for their accusatives.


Repetition of classifier prefixes across nouns and verbs

Note that classifiers stack on top of the verbs. That is to say, any verb in the sentence will take a prefix agreeing with the noun classifier of the subject, unless it happens to already have the same classifier.

However, humans are spread across several classifiers, which mark different genders and ages of humans. These all share the same verbs, so it is not necessary to repeat the classifier before the object if the subject and object of a sentence are both human. For example, hinuhuki means teacher (adult female), tukuu means student (young child of either gender),[1] and tuupi means to kiss. The accusative form of hi- is mi-. Thus one can say

Tukuu minuhuki tututami.
The student kicked the teacher.

Stacking of classifiers

Classifiers stack across nouns that are dependent on other nouns. For example, kuha means "(a) piece", and sikupi means "wood", so one can speak of

Kuha kusikupi.
A piece of wood.

However, when a two-word phrase such as this is used as an object in a longer sentence, the subject's classifier is repeated only on the first word:

Hinuma hikuha kusikupi hikigi.
The woman scratched the piece of wood.

This can be thought of more clearly with an analogy to mathematics. The sentence above can be visualized in three dimensions, with kusikupi as a branch off of hikuha, or as an equation:

Hinuma hi(kuha ku(sikupi)) hi(kigi).
Words with no classifiers

A small number of words have no classifier prefix. Many of these are proper nouns or loanwords. These are treated as if their first syllable were a classifier prefix, and therefore behave exactly the same as other words except that they usually seem to be in the wrong noun class.

Private verbs

Note, it is most likely that the private verbs will depend on only the agent, and will exclude humans and inanimate objects. Therefore the scope is much less.

Andanese preserves the private verbs of Tapilula, which also persisted in the Gold language but were dropped in all of Gold's descendants, along with the classifier system itself. Private verbs are morphemes, usually only one or two syllables long, whose meaning depends on the preceding classifier prefixes. For example, there exists a verb vutami "to gallop, run quickly on all fours". This verb begins with vu-, which is also the first syllable of the word for horse. Thus, if a horse is the subject, there is no need to repeat its classifier prefix before the verb; the classifier prefix is already there. If a man is galloping, one would say

Kilatu kivutami.
The man is galloping.
The man is running like a horse.

Whereas if the subject is a horse or a similar animal, the sentence would instead be

Vuhapi vutami.
The horse is galloping.
The horse is running like a horse.

Without the need for an extra syllable before the verb. Likewise, if the situation were reversed, one could say

Vuhapi vugitami.
The horse is running like a human (on its hind legs only).

The persistence of verbs like these is the main reason why Late Andanese has so many noun classes for specific animals such as turtles and rabbits with few nouns but larger numbers of verbs.


Note that the neuter prefix gi-, rather than the masculine ki- or feminine hi-, is used whenever animals are personified, regardless of the semantic gender of the animal.

Use of classifiers to derive new words

New words can be formed by copying a word from one class to another. For example, there exists a word pair of gipihi "sharp tooth; canine" and kipihi "to bite". Thus, a noun became a verb by simply changing the classifier prefix.

For some classes, the entire vocabulary can be assumed to be copiable. As above, gi- nouns denoting body parts pair with ki- verbs denoting the action of striking or hitting something with that body part, and with mi- denoting that body part served as food.

Likewise, the word for a young student, tukuu, is actually derived from the word for book, ikuu, by changing the prefix from i-, which denotes handheld objects, to tu-, which marks the "human children" noun class.

Derivation of words

Andanese is a head-initial language. That is, within a word, a classifier prefix will come first, giving the general broad meaning of the word, and each additional morpheme will narrow the meaning of the word further. This is the opposite pattern of most surrounding languages. Thus Andanese could be described as having a taxonomic vocabulary. Someone hearing the first part of a word will not know its precise meaning, but will have a general idea of what it might be. But someone hearing only the end of a word will have no idea which of the many possible categories of the vocabulary the word belongs to.

An exception to the rule that compounds are head-initial (i.e. "ears of corn") is that if one party is animate, the animate partner goes first; thus from vukia "horse" comes vukiakiki "horse ears".[2]


The genitive prefix si- is never used within a word; this appears only heading up a full, independent word describing a definite object. Thus, all compounds are single words, and some can be very long. In general, Andanese words are often highly precise, and may seem long even given the small phonology of the language. Many words have been lengthened by adding additional morphemes over time, even when such lengthening was not necessary to prevent collision of the word with a homophone. For example, the word for heart, as inherited from Old Andanese, is vi. But this is padded with the body part classifier prefix li- on one side and the precising morpheme tu "blood" on the other; thus, the resulting word livitu can be analyzed as "body part heart of blood".

Reassignment of classifier prefixes

Words have moved from one class to another over time. For example, the inherited root word for snow, reflected in Late Andanese as gina, is now indifferent to the form of precipitation and only pairs with classifiers: gagina "snow"; vugina "rain".

Classifier prefixes and titular words

Classifier prefixes cannot be used as words of their own. Every classifier prefix has at least one titular stem, whose meaning simply repeats one of the meanings of the classifier prefix so that it can be used as an independent word. For example, the classifier prefix sa- means "love", but the proper verb for "to love" is sanala. Thus one would say

Kikuhigi nanuma kitasanala.
The soldier loved the nurse.

Likewise, ka- means "tree", but the full form of the word for tree is kakupi.

Polysemy

Classifier prefixes with more than one meaning will have more than one titular word. Since ka- also means "insect", the word kakui, meaning "insect, arthropod" is also considered a titular word.

Dropping of classifiers in compounds

Compounds of two nouns generally drop the classifier from the second noun, using semantics to disambiguate the possible meanings. For example, hikala "seashell" and lakala "bear" share the same two-syllable root, and have different noun classifier prefixes. But the compound word pugikala, which adds pugi "claw", can only be used to mean "bear's claw" because seashells do not have claws. If a speaker did want to specifically say "seashell claw", then they would use pugi puhikala.

Dropping the second classifier is mandatory when it is the same as the first classifier or of the same syntactic field. In fact, some teachers posit null morphemes before each element of a compound, each of which agrees with the outermost classifier. Thus, for example, they analyze pugikala "bear claw" as pugi pukala, and claim this proves that there exists a secondary Andanese word pukala "bear" alongside lakala, which is never used in bare form but is called up when speakers create compound words such as the above pugikala. This theory is one way of explaining the limits of which compounds are allowed to drop the classifiers and which are not. However, a dictionary based on this theory would list over a million words, most of which would be unusable duplicates of others.

Note, the morphemes here may be in the wrong order, since animacy overrides head-first construction.

Locative and thematic classifiers

Assignment of newly coined words to classifiers usually corresponds either to the place ("locative classifiers") or the purpose of the object ("thematic classifiers"). For example, a spoon is not an edible object, but it is frequently found with them, and therefore the word for spoon, miguha is in the food class rather than the handheld object class; iguha instead means "shovel".

Animacy

Inanimate objects have classifiers that do not change for syntactical active or passive roles. Since the subject of a sentence is always animate, however, their classifier is always buried underneath another classifier that repeats the subject's classifier. Note that there are no sound changes; if two vowels come together, they are still pronounced separately. Since this happens also to the verb, often a sentence will consist of three alliterative words. Thus one can say

Tulata tuinuhu tuyula.
The student threw the torch.

Here, while a clever student might expect the sequence /tui/ to contract to /pi/, as it historically did in many similar constructions, the morphemes remain separate and the sequence is thus pronounced [twi].

List of common noun classifiers

All noun classifiers are one syllable long, but some second-order classifiers can follow other classifiers and create what are effectively two-syllable prefixes. Even so, the outermost classifier in such a word is the one that echoes throughout the sentence, not the pair.

a : roads, streets

la : large land animals

i : handheld objects

ha : to worship; needle

ka : trees; some flying insects

u : water, liquid; land features (e.g. "beach", "hill")

ma : some grasses

ga : winter, things encountered in winter

na : accusative of gi- (all senses)

li : rung, plank, flat surface(?); water, liquid (alternates with u-)

sa : love

pa : some words for loose-fitting clothes

ta :

pi : pregnant women, babies, and couples

hi : tree (bark); worm; the ocean(?); bowl, cup, dish; adult women (nominative); men, boys (accusative)

ti : foot, motion

si : genitive prefix (etymologically hi-i-; often seen in contracted form as s- before vowel-initial words)

gi : protective objects; sharp, firm, protective; humans, human body parts; birds[3]

mi : food; breast, nourishment; adult women (accusative)

ni : young girls; place of X, generic placenames; snakes

pu : succulent fruit (doublet of tu-); sexual reproduction, obscene body parts (only when following another classifier such as li-)

hu : fire; celestial objects; insects; tight clothes, "shaped like"; hair of the head, back of body;

tu : small plants; children; blood, bodily humors

su : oceanography (hi- + -u-)

lu : some body parts (a 2nd-order classifier that often comes between the gender marker and the root word)

ku : arrow, sharp weapon

mu :

gu : breast, nurture, nutrition (secondary use only)

nu : fruit[4]; buildings

ki : verbs of motion (corresponds to li- body parts); weapon, claw; men, boys; darkness, night, sleep; some buildings

The sequences /ja ji ju va vi vu/ have taken on the role of pseudo-classifiers, since in initial position they are monosyllabic:

ya : some grasses; pineapple, large fruit; meat, food

yi :

yu :

va :

vi : eye, vision, knowledge

vu : rain, water; horse, rideable animal

If the root begins with a single vowel, the syllables /yi yu vi vu/ reverse to [iy iv uy uv]. If the root word begins with /i/ or /u/ followed by another vowel, however, the normal pronunciation is restored.

However, sequences such as /sia/, /nua/, and so on have not been reanalyzed as single syllables, and cannot serve as classifier prefixes.

Note that there are many monosyllabic morphemes that are not classifiers. For example i means "shoulder", but is only used with a body part classifier li-.

Consonant-based gender system

Andanese inherited the consonant-based gender system from Tapilula. The genders are:

li: Babies; also used for humans of indeterminate age and gender (accusative is na-)

pi: Pregnant; adult males and adult females together; parents, childbirth

ki: Men and boys (contracts to k- before a vowel; accusative is variable, alternating between hi- or s- when acted on by "weak" agents and a contracted form of the prefix of the agent itself for "strong" agents)[5]

tu: Young children (accusative is ti-)

ni: Young girls

hi: Adult women (accusative is mi-)

There is no neuter gender and no epicene gender. Note that females have two genders (three if "pregnant" is considered to be exclusively feminine) but males have one. This is a trait common to many languages of the area, and was present in their common parent language of Tapilula.


As in Gold, all objects found in bodies of water are grammatically feminine even if syntactically masculine.

A very small number of words show relics of the older internal mutations that marked gender in the Tapilula language. In Tapilula, classifier prefixes were dropped in more situations than they are in Late Andanese, and therefore the existence of separate stems for different genders was necessary. In Late Andanese, only a few such words survive, such as the word pair kilitu "king" and hiihu "queen". Even though the stems are different, the use of the prefixes is mandatory in Late Andanese.

Marking the accusative case

Late Andanese marks the accusative case by changing the noun classifier prefix, if there is one. For example, the noun classifier for adult women is hi- in the nominative (agentive) case, but this changes to mi- when a woman is the patient (direct object) of a verb. This case is still padded with the additional case marker of the agent itself, meaning that there will be two noun classifiers stacked together. This is the normal case in Andanese when an noun is the direct object of a verb. Note that inanimate nouns and most non-human animate nouns do not have separate forms of their noun class prefix for showing the accusative.

The masculine noun class, marked by ki- in an agent role, is the most changeable of all noun class prefixes, and the only one whose accusative form depends on the noun class of the agent acting on them. That is, a man stung by a bee will have a different noun class than a man kissed by a woman, in addition to the noun class of the agent which is stacked before the noun class for the man. Furthermore, the agent and patient forms merge before certain vowel-initial words, and the agent form also takes a special form when the patient is male.

The various interrelated patterns are all due to etymology. The original form of the agent prefix was tə- in pre-Andanese, and the patient was hə-. In early Old Andanese, this vowel either disappeared or changed to /i/, depending on environment. The /t/ soon changed to /k/, part of a general shift. Then, the sequences ki hi merged as s- before a vowel-initial word. But this change did not spread to words that became vowel-initial later on, meaning that only a few vowel-initial Late Andanese words trigger this assimilation, one of which is /atu/ "soldier". Further, although the nominative forms for male and female soldiers are both /satu/, in the accusative they diverge, with the masculine form being still /satu/ while the feminine form changes to /miatu/.

Any sequence of /hihi/ resulting from these shifts reduced to /hi/ by a general process of analogy. This occurred when a verb had a female agent and a male patient, but only when one of the other rules above did not force the inner /hi/ to contract into /s/. Lastly, the /hi/ disappears before many consonant-initial roots, as it evolved early on into a simple /h/ in these positions, which was then dropped.

Sample sentences below illustrate the different possibilities:

Satu.
Male soldier.
Kuinau kusatu kutakiu.
The bee stung the soldier.
Hiuma hisatu hitakiihumi.
The woman hit the soldier.
Kikapi.
Male farmer.
Nianu nikapi nipihi.
The snake bit the male farmer.
Hikapi.
Female farmer.
Nianu nimikapi nipihi.
The snake bit the female farmer.

Second order classifiers and length of words

Because of the noun classifier prefixes, most word bases are at least three syllables long. Exceptions are of two kinds: some very common words are used with no classifiers, and some words have classifiers but stems that are only one syllable. Generally these result from recent sound contractions.

Often, a classifier prefix is not sufficient to precisely define a word, and the word will take a second-order classifier between its "exposed" classifier that interacts with the grammar and the rest of the root word. For example, many body parts are classified under the prefix lu-, such as gilulali "head", which breaks down as gi- + -lu- + lali.

Most words of this type acquired their second-order classifiers only fairly recently, when drastic sound changes led to phonological collision of words even within the same noun class.

Some syllables can be used either as primary or secondary classifiers. For example, the -lu- above can only appear after a human gender classifier, but ku- "arrow, sharp object" often follows i- "handheld object" to create words for handheld weapons such as ikukivuni "slicing knife" and ikukuhigi "one-handed sword".

Other information about private verbs

Note, it is most likely that the private verbs will depend on only the agent, and will exclude humans and inanimate objects. Therefore the scope is much less.

Andanese preserves the private verbs of its parent language Tapilula, whereas in the Gold branch of the family they disappeared early on.

Private verbs are verbs whose meaning is entirely dependent on the noun classes of the subject and object that precede it. For example, if the subject is "boy" and the object is "orange", a verb spelled lua could mean "to eat". If the subject is "boy" and the object is "girl", the same verb would mean "to kiss". The only commonality between the two verbs is that they both involve the mouth, and indeed, lua is derived from the word for mouth. The opposite meanings can be indicated by stacking additional classifier prefixes. For example, nulua means unambiguously "eat" in any context, so one can say

Kupu kihipu kitanulua .
The boy ate the girl.

This could be translated narrowly as "The boy mouthed the girl like she was a fruit."

Through cultural osmosis, similar phenomena later reappeared in some of the Gold languages, particularly those most closely culturally linked to the Andanese.

Syllable harmony

Due to historical sound changes such as /pua > pʷo > po > pu/, where a syllable beginning in a vowel was completely swallowed up by the syllable preceding it, Late Andanese evolved syllable harmony where most related languages evolved consonant harmony or nothing at all. Any a in the second element of a compound word could change to match the syllable of the classifier prefix. Often, a further classifier prefix will then appear before the original one since the meaning of the word has changed. Alternatively, the second word will agree with the first syllable of the first word in the compound rather than its classifier, since the grammar allows for the reinterpretation of originally independent words as sequences of classifier and short word.

This process is not used in people's names, as scholars consider the use of such vacuous alliteration to be a sign of an unintelligent mind, but it is used in common nouns and some place names. For example, a road to a store may be called ipunapupu, the compound of i- "road, open place", puna "store", and aa "road". Note also that the two main morphemes are in the reverse order from what one might expect, because the reinterpretation of the word for store as a one-off classifier makes the compound head-final instead of head-initial as in most compounds. This process is reminiscent of Play's head-initial compounds, which also require a closing morpheme to restore the "proper" morpheme order.

Orthography and contact with other languages

Andanese has had several scripts. Commonly, the Andanese wrote their language with one of a series of artistic syllabaries, each with 30 glyphs, which were based on square tiles. Of these, the commonest one was based on squares with 90° and 45° angles inside them. In multi-line texts, the boundaries of the squares would often be omitted, resulting in a shape that resembles a Tangram puzzle.

Main scripts

The primary script was derived from the 100-glyph Tapilula syllabary in a highly irregular manner. As the phonology contracted and evolved, many glyphs were dropped while others were repurposed. The actual original glyph derivations are:

[see red notebook]

Because most of the glyphs were taken from the top half of the syllabary, many of the original shapes did not appear, and the glyphs were smoothed into simpler designs over time.

During the last few decades when Late Andanese still had tones, they were spelled with the inherited glyphs for /va vi vu ya yi yu/. When Andanese lost its tones, these glyphs did not recover their original values, nor were new glyphs created, and therefore these sequences continued to be spelled with digraphs /ua ui uu ia ii iu/.

Phonetic scope

The syllabary had only thirty glyphs because Andanese teachers considered their language to have only thirty syllables. At first, these thirty syllables included two sets of symbols for /a i u/ depending on whether the hiatus was primordial or of recent origin; this had important effects on grammar. They had no glyphs for /sa si su/ because they considered these syllables to be underlyingly /hia hii hiu/. But the final stage of the language shifted the spelling to a phonetic one incorporating the earlier hiatus glyphs as /s/ glyphs. (That is, the glyphs themselves were repurposed, meaning that /a₂ i₂ u₂/ came to spell /sa si su/.)

The teachers never considered the sequences /wa wi wu/ or /ya yi yu/ to be proper syllables, even though they behaved as such in the grammar. Neither were sequences such as /mia mii miu/, whose surface pronunciation was monosyllabic, incorporated into the syllabary with single glyphs. The earlier practice of not writing /s/ made sense from this standpoint, as the distribution of /sa si su/ is very similar to that of sequences like /mia mii miu/ and unlike that of more common syllables like /ka ki ku/; the change happened when the script came to be widely used to write foreign languages in which /s/ was a common sound and could not be analyzed as an allophone of an underlying CV sequence.

Minor scripts

Batam

The oddest looking script, "batam" (an exonym), was not a script at all but a means of drawing objects with the angular shapes of one of the many 30-letter syllabaries.


Persistence of Andanese words in other languages

Late Andanese loanwords in other languages are mostly pronounced according to the closest match in the recipient language's spelling, since knowledge of the original language disappeared shortly after the war in 4175. Thus the pronunciation of these words varies from language to language even as the spelling has remained the same for thousands of years. However, no tradition has introduced phonemes not directly descended from one of the 30 syllables in the Late Andanese language.

The Palli language, spoken by the southern half of the defeated Thaoa people, lost most of its inherited vocabulary and replaced them with loanwords from Late Andanese.

Notes

  1. "book" in dict, though
  2. this probably isn't the reason why gold switched, though, because in goldnthe anime pRtenr oftem.goes last
  3. NOTE. "humans, human body parts; birds" was originally listed under li but this is an error. Both come from Tapilula nʷə-, meaning that humans and birds were considered as one even then. There is alternate form of the prefix, nu-, which is the basis of the accusative.
  4. "Large enough to be eaten one at a time, but small enough to hold in one hand."
  5. from təlin "penis"