The wiki has recently been updated. Please contact me by talk page or email if you encounter any issues.

User:Masako/sandbox: Difference between revisions

From FrathWiki
Jump to navigationJump to search
Line 1: Line 1:
= Introduction =
= Introduction =


Kala is a personal [[Wikipedia:Constructed_language|conlang]] (actually more of an [[Wikipedia:Artistic_language|artlang]]), not attached to any [[Wikipedia:Worldbuilding|conworld]] or conculture.  
Kala is a personal [[Wikipedia:Constructed_language|conlang]] (actually more of an [[Wikipedia:Artistic_language#Personal_languages|artlang]]), not attached to any [[Wikipedia:Worldbuilding|conworld]] or conculture.  


== Borrowing ==
== Borrowing ==

Revision as of 10:46, 2 March 2016

Introduction

Kala is a personal conlang (actually more of an artlang), not attached to any conworld or conculture.

Borrowing

Kala borrows extensively from various natural languages. This is a very small sample of borrowings:

  • kalato speak, talk, converse; from Arabic takallama
  • naI, me; from Arabic ʾanā
  • patoduck (Anatidae); from Spanish pato
  • tsenkaorange; from Chinese chéng
  • uasito take, get, acquire; from Lakota wasichu
  • myontato allow, permit; from Finnish myöntää
  • ato be, exist, yes; from Japanese aru

Phonology

Consonants

Consonants
Labial Alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal
central lateral
Nasal m (m) n (n) ɲ (ny)
Plosive p~b (p) t~d (t) k~g (k) ʔ (`)
Affricate ts (ts) t͡ɬ~tl (tl) t͡ʃ (ts)
Continuant s (s) l~ɾ (l) ʃ (s) h~ɦ (h)
Semivowel j (y) w (u)
  • Prenasalized: /ᵐp ⁿt ᵑk/
  • Labialized:/pʷ kʷ mʷ nʷ ʃʷ hʷ t͡ʃʷ/
  • Palatalized: /pʲ kʲ mʲ hʲ/

Note: Because of its small phoneme inventory, Kala allows for quite a lot of allophonic variation. For example, /p t k/ may be pronounced [b d ɡ] as well as [p t k], /s l h/ as [ʃ ɾ ɦ], and /t͡s t͡ɬ/ as [t͡ʃ t͡l]; also, vowels may be either long or short.

Vowels

Vowels
Front Central Back
Close i~ɪ (i) u~u: (u)
Mid e~ɛ (e) o~o: (o)
Open a~a: (a)

Kala has five vowels /i/, /e/, /a/, /o/ and /u/. Each occurs in both stressed and unstressed syllables. Phonetic nasalization occurs for vowels occurring between nasal consonants or when preceding a syllable-final nasal, e.g. tsunka [ˈt͡ʃũᵑka] ('bug').

Diphthongs

Phonetically, Kala has only two diphthongs, both falling; [aɪ̯] and [aʊ̯], but there are five syllables that can be analyzed as rising diphthongs; [wa], [we], [ja], [je], and [jo]. The two triphthongs [waɪ̯] and [jaʊ̯] are very rare but should be noted as possible.

syllable structure

Kala syllable structure is ((N)C)(y, u)V(F). As in most languages, CV is the most common syllable type, accounting for the majority of Kala lemmas. The finals /n/, /m/, and /k/ are grammatical and indicate adverbs, plurals, and negatives, respectively. They only occur word finally. So, nkapa (alcohol) is permitted, but nakpa is not; kyopo (fear) is acceptable, but koypo is not, etc. There is a limited set of syllables allowed by Kala phonotactics, similar to Japanese or Chinese. They are listed here: Syllable Chart, or here.

  • /l/ cannot occur word initially (except in loan words and toponyms).

allophony

stress

grammar

noun phrases

The basic noun phrase in Kala is PREPOSITION DETERMINER NOUN DESCRIPTIVE-VERB RELATIVE-CLAUSE. Depending on context, this is fairly predominant with a few exceptions.

determiners

Determiners includes various particles, demonstratives, and quantifiers. Most nouns will have exactly one of these. They usually come before their nouns. Possessives are semantically determiners, but they can't carry determiner inflections, so an appropriate particle or pronoun is brought in for them.

demonstratives

Kala makes a three-way distinction. Typically there is a distinction between proximal or first person (objects near to the speaker), medial or second person (objects near to the addressee), and distal or third person (objects far from both).

  • itla (i-) - this (near me)
  • uatla (ua-) - that (near you)
  • yetla (ye-) - that (over there)

The demonstratives can be prefixed to any noun to show deixis.

  • imita taha
PROX-dog be.big
This dog is big.
  • uaneko tamyo ka
MED-cat 2pl.POSS Q
Is that cat (near you) yours?
  • yetsakam ahi
DIST-house-PL be.small
Those houses (over there) are small.

quantifiers

pronouns

Kala agent pronouns are often omitted when the person is obvious from context. Kala personal pronouns also encode clusivity. That is, there are different words for "we" depending on whether the speaker is including the person spoken to or not. Pronouns do not inflect for gender; if gender is significant, one can use words like naka, tlaka, nahi, tahi (the woman, the man, the girl, the boy), etc.

nkalo
Agent Patient Reflexive Possessive Reciprocal
1sg na ena na'i nayo
2sg ta eta ta'i tayo
3sg ha eha ha'i hayo
4sg tla etla tla'i tlayo
1pl
1pl exclusive
nam
na'am
enam
ena'am
nami
na'ami
namyo
na'amyo
nanku
na'anku
2pl tam etam tami tamyo tanku
3pl kam ekam kami kamyo kanku
4pl tlam etlam tlami tlamyo tlanku


examples

correlative pronouns

verbs

tense

Kala has three simple tenses; past, present, and future. Present tense is unmarked. However, past and future tenses can be modified to include immediate future ("is about to..."), distant future ("will...in a long while"), recent past ("just ..."), and remote past ("...a long while ago"). These distinctions are made with the augmentative and diminutive endings -ha and -hi.

  • The present tense can show immediacy by using the adverb ima, "now; at this time":
ima mita ina - now dog eat - The dog is eating right now.
  • If a temporal adverb is used, the tense suffix may be omitted.
Example: yomaye nam ina - yesterday 1pl eat - We ate yesterday.
eme
Kala gloss English
Present mita ina dog eat The dog eats.
Past mita inaye dog eat-PST The dog ate.
Recent Past mita inayehi
or -hye
dog eat-REC The dog just ate.
Remote Past mita inayeha dog eat-REM The dog ate a long while ago.
Future mita inatli dog eat-FUT The dog will eat.
Immediate Future mita inatlihi
or -tlai
dog eat-IMM The dog will eat soon.
Distant Future mita inatliha dog eat-DIS The dog will eat a long while from now.

aspect

The progressive, also called the continuous [CONT], this is used to express an incomplete action or state in progress at a specific time. It is marked with -nko, from nkoso - "to continue; proceed; progress". The perfecive aspect indicates that an action is completed [PFV]. It is often translated by the English present perfect (have done some-thing). It is marked with -pua, from opua - "to end; finish; complete". The inchoative aspect refers to the beginning of a state [INCH]. It is marked with -mu, from mula - "to begin; start; initiate". The frequentative aspect refers to a repeated action [FREQ]. It is marked with -nua, from nua - "frequent; often; regular".

ti'a
Kala gloss English
Progressive mita inanko dog eat-CONT The dog is eating.
Perfective mita inapua dog eat-PFV The dog has eaten.
Inchoative mita inamu dog eat-INCH The dog begins to eat.
Frequentative mita inanua dog eat-FREQ The dog eats often.

mood

The negative mood (always marked finally) is indicated by the suffix –k or –nke (when the last syllable contains /k/).

  • mita inayek - dog eat-PST-NEG - The dog did not eat.
  • mita mokunke - dog sleep-NEG - The dog does not sleep.

copula

adjectives

adpositions

syntax

The basic structure of a Kala sentence is:

AGENT--PATIENT--VERB (or SOV)

The agent is the person or thing doing the action described by the verb; The patient is the recipient of that action. The importance of word order can be seen by comparing the following sentences:

  • mita tlaka anya - dog man see - The dog sees the man.
  • tlaka mita anya - man dog see - The man sees the dog.

In both sentences, the words are identical, the only way to know who is seeing whom is by the order of the words in the sentence.

adverbial

coordinate

relative

derivation

compounding

sample texts

lexicon