Béu : Chapter 3 : The Verb: Difference between revisions

From FrathWiki
Jump to navigationJump to search
 
(329 intermediate revisions by the same user not shown)
Line 2: Line 2:
Welcome to      <big> '''béu'''</big>
Welcome to      <big> '''béu'''</big>


== ... The Verbal Moods==


..


When people speak they have different intentions. That is they are trying to achieve different things by speaking ... maybe they are trying to convey information, or wanting somebody to do something, or not to do something, or they are just expressing their feelings about something. All these are examples of what is called moods. Different languages have different methods of coding their moods. Also the various moods of a languages cover a different semantic range compared to other languages.


There are 7 moods in '''béu''' ... 3 expressing themselves by changes to the root verb and 4 by periphrasis.  
== ..... Person/Tense/Evidence==


..
..


[[Image:TW_687.png]]
Also called the '''r'''-form or the indicative.


..
..
To make a verb in the indicative mood, you must first deleted the final vowel from the base form. Then add affixes that indicate "agent", "indicative mood", "tense", "evidentiality" and "perfectness". We will refer to these as slots 1, 2, 3, 4 and 5 respectively. All these affixes together are known as the verb tail. The "agent", "indicative mood", "tense" are mandatory ... however one tense, the aortist is a null morpheme.


..
..


What are considered moods are shown by a green circle.
=== ... Seven Persons===


..
..


Slot 1 is for the agent
..
..


[[Image:TW_688.png]]
One of the 7 vowels below is must be added. These indicate the doer..


How the different moods and forms interact are shown above. [this will be explained in full later]
[[Image:TW_109.png]]
 
Notice that there are 2 entries that represent the 1st person plural subject (i.e. we). The top one represents first person inclusive and the bottom one represents first person exclusive. 
 
Some people might have difficulty remembering whether to use '''ai''' or '''au'''. The diagram below might help some ...


..
..


=== ... The Infinitive===
[[Image:SW_08.png]] ............... [[Image:SW_09.png]]


..
..


The '''maŋga''' is "the infinitive"
Mathematically it is as if ... '''ai''' = me + you ... and ... '''au''' = me + they ....... (sort of)


This is the base form of the verb ... not considered a mood. '''maŋga''' corresponds to what is called the "infinitive" in some languages or the "masDar" in Arabic.
The vowels of the first person plural inclusive pronoun '''magi''' are reflected in the infix -'''ai'''-.  


About 32% of multi syllable '''maŋga''' end in "a".
As are the  vowels of the first person plural exclusive pronoun '''manu''' reflected in the infix -'''au'''-.


About 16% of multi syllable '''maŋga''' end in "e", and the same for "o".
..


About 9% of  multi syllable '''maŋga''' end in "au", and the same for "oi", "eu" and "ai".
Note that the '''ai''' form is used when you are talking about generalities ... the so called "impersonal form" ... English uses "you" or "one" for this function.


[[Image:TW_626.png]]
The above defines the "person" of the verb. Then follows an "r" which indicates the word is an verb in the indicative mood. For example ...


Note that no '''maŋga''' end in "i", "u", "ia" and "ua"
'''doika''' = to walk


"i" is reserved for marking verb chains, which will be explained later.
'''doikar''' = I walk


"u" is used for the imperative mood ... i.e. for commanding people.
'''doikair''' and '''doikaur''' = we walk


"ia" is used for a past passive participle. For example ...
'''doikir''' = you walk


'''yubako''' = to strengthen
'''doiker''' = you walk


'''yubakia''' = strengthened ... as in '''pazba dí r yubakia''' => "this table is strengthened"
'''doikor''' = he/she/it walks


"ua" could be called the future passive participle I guess. For example ...
'''doikur''' = they walk


'''ndi r yubakua''' => these ones must be strengthened
..


To form a negative infinitive the word '''jù''' is placed immediately in front of the verb. For example ...
=== ... The R-form===


'''doika''' = to walk
..


'''jù doika''' = to not walk .... not to walk
One mood


..
..


=== ... The indicative===
Slot 2 is for the indicative mood marker.


..
..


Also called the R-form.
At this point we must introduce a new sound and a new letter.
 
 
[[Image:TW_355.png]]


..


To make a verb in the indicative mood, you must first deleted the final vowel from the infinitive. Then add affixes that indicate "agent", "indicative mood", "tense", "evidentiality" and "perfectness". We will refer to these as slots 1, 2, 3, 4 and 5 respectively.
This letter has not been mentioned so far because it doesn't occur in any words as such. It only occurs in grammatical suffixes and it indicates the indicative mood.
 
If you hear an "r" you know you are hearing the main verb of a clause.


..
..


==== .. Slot 1====
=== ... Five Tenses===


..
..


Slot 1 is for the agent
Slot 3 is for tense markers. There are 5 tense markers in '''béu'''
 
..
..


One of the 7 vowels below is must be added. These indicate the doer..
1)  '''*doikaro''' => '''doikar''' = I walk (habitually)


[[Image:TW_109.png]]
This could be called "the open tense" ... timewise there are no limits to an action marked in this way. Also called "the timeless tense". A sort of habitual tense. Often used for generic statements. For example ...


Notice that there are 2 entries that represent the 1st person plural subject (i.e. we). The top one represents first person inclusive and the bottom one represents first person exclusive. 
'''ngur jwadoi''' = "birds fly"


Note that the '''ai''' form is used when you are talking about generalities ... the so called "impersonal form" ... English uses "you" or "one" for this function.
Actually you can say this tense has an underlying  '''o''' which appears again if there is an '''n''' or '''s''' in slot 4.


The above defines the "person" of the verb. Then follows an "r" which indicates the word is an verb in the indicative mood. For example ...
2) '''doikaru''' = I will walk


'''doika''' = to walk
This is the future tense


'''doikar''' = I walk
3) '''doikari''' = I walked


'''doikair''' and '''doikaur''' = we walk
This is the past tense. This means that the action was done before today (by the way ... the '''béu''' day starts at 6 in the morning).


'''doikir''' = you walk
4) '''doikare''' = I walked


'''doiker''' = you walk
This is the near-past tense. This means that the action was done earlier on today (a good memory aid is to remember that '''e''' is the same vowel as in the English word "day")


'''doikor''' = he/she/it walks
5) '''doikara''' = I am walking


'''doikur''' = they walk
This is the present tense ... it means that the action is ongoing at the time of speaking.


..
..


==== .. Slot 2====
It can be seen that '''béu''' is more fine-grained, tense-wise than most of the world's languages ... http://wals.info/chapter/66 and http://wals.info/chapter/67


..
..


Slot 2 is for the indicative mood marker.
=== ... Evidentials===


..
..


At this point we must introduce a new sound and a new letter.
Two Evidentials


..


[[Image:TW_355.png]]
Slot 4 can have one of the evidential markers '''a''', '''a''', '''n''', '''s''' or it can be empty.
Actually the first '''a''' defines the subjects attitute rather than any evidentiality, however all 4 are usually just called evidential markers.


..


This letter has not been mentioned so far because it doesn't occur in any words as such. It only occurs in grammatical suffixes and it indicates the indicative mood.
There are three markers that cites on what evidence the speaker is saying what he is saying. However it is not mandatory to stipulate on what evidence you are saying what you are saying. In fact most occurrences of the indicative verb do not have an evidence marker.


If you hear an "r" you know you are hearing the main verb of a clause.
The markers are as follows ...


..
1) -'''n'''


==== .. Slot 3====
For example ... '''doikorin''' = "I guess that he walked" ... That is the speaker worked it out from circumstances/clues observed.


..
I will mention '''waron''' here. It means "I think so" and is nearly as common an answer as '''aiwa''' "yes"


Slot 3 is for tense markers. There are 5 tense markers in '''béu'''
2) -'''s'''  


..
For example ... '''doikoris''' = "They say he walked" ....... That is the speaker was told by some third party(ies) or overheard some third party(ies) talking.


1) '''*doikaro''' => '''doikar''' = I walk (habitually)
3) -'''a'''  


{|
For example ... '''doikoria''' = "he walked, I saw him" ...... That is the speaker saw it with his own eyes.
|-
! tunheu-n ||align=center| doik-a-r-∅ || fafalaja || gò || nambo-n || ny-á-r-∅
|-
| townhall-{{small|DAT}} |align=center|walk-{{small|1SG-IND-AOR}}  || "every afternoon" || {{small|CONJ}}  || home-{{small|DAT}} || return-{{small|1SG-IND-AOR}}
|} ==> I walk to the townhall every afternoon and then return home


Note that the above evidential only co-occurs with the past tense and near-past tense. Actually when used with the near-past tense, '''*ea''' => '''ia''' so the distinction between "past" and "near-past" is lost for this evidential.


I call this the aortist tense. The word comes from Ancient Greek and means "indefinite" as it was the unmarked tense/aspect. (Actually thIs unmarked form had a past & nondurative meaning in Ancient Greek). I call this form aortist because it is usually represented by a null morpheme. In '''béu''' it has a sort of timeless tense (sometimes it is habitual) used for generic statements. For example ...
Now there is a forth possibility for this slot ... and it is not actually an evidintial. Furthermore it has the same form as 3).


'''pyár jwadoi''' = "birds fly"
4) -'''a'''  


Actually you can say this tense has an underlying  '''o''' which appears if there is an '''n''' or an '''s''' in the evidentiality slot.
For example ... '''doikorua''' = "he intends to walk" ... the agent in this case must be a sentient being of course.


{|
This evidential marker only co-occurs with the future tense.
|-
! tunheu-n ||align=center| doik-o-r-o-s  || fafalaja || gò || nambo-n || ny-o-r-o-s
|-
| townhall-{{small|DAT}} |align=center|walk-{{small|3SG-IND-AOR-EV2}}  || "every afternoon" || {{small|CONJ}}  || home-{{small|DAT}} || return-{{small|3SG-IND-AOR-EV2}}
|} ==> They say he/she walks to the townhall every afternoon and then returns home


If the speaker doesn't know the evidential or deems it unimportant then this slot can be left empty. According to corpus studies in '''béu''', 60% - 70% of '''r'''-form have nothing in this slot.


2) '''doikaru''' = I will walk
..


This is the future tense
So the complete verb prefix system is ...


3) '''doikari''' = I walked
[[Image:TW_980.png]]


This is the past tense. This means that the action was done before today (by the way ... the '''béu''' day starts at 6 in the morning).
..


4) '''doikare''' = I walked
It can be seen that the '''béu''' evidentiality inventory is quite substantial compared to other languages ... http://wals.info/chapter/78


This is the near-past tense. This means that the action was done earlier on today (a good memory aid is to remember that '''e''' is the same vowel as in the English word "day")
Also it appears that 4 or 5 categories being appended to the verb is typical of languages of the world. See ... http://wals.info/chapter/22  [If I have understood the chapter properly]


5) '''doikara''' = I am walking
..


This is the present tense ... it means that the action is ongoing at the time of speaking.
=== ... For brevity===


..
..


It can be seen that '''béu''' is more fine-grained, tense-wise than most of the world's languages ... http://wals.info/chapter/66 and http://wals.info/chapter/67
We have seen that in the verb tail, '''o''' is not pronounced if it comes final (the aortist tense).
 
The reason for this is brevity of speech.
 
For brevity of writng, every occurrence of '''o''' is not written (in the verb tail). For example ...


..
..


==== .. Slot 4====
[[Image:TW_795.png]]


..
..


Slot 4 is for the evidential markers (well three out of five are evidential markers)
== ... Probability/Aspect/Negation==


..
..


There are three markers that cites on what evidence the speaker is saying what he is saying. However it is not mandatory to stipulate on what evidence you are saying what you are saying. In fact most occurrences of the indicative verb do not have an evidence marker.
We have already covered the 4 slots for "agent", '''r''', "tense" and "evidentiality" at the end of the verb. As well as the nuances given by these suffixes, there are particles which add further information to the basic verb. These are called (near-standers ?). These particles occur in three pre-verbal slots.


The markers are as follows ...
The two particles in the first slot show probability.  


1) -'''n'''
The seven particles in the second slot have to do with aspect in some way. Aspect can be tricky.


For example ... '''doikorin''' = "I guess that he walked" ... That is the speaker worked it out from circumstances/clues observed.
In the third slot, only one particle : the negating particle ''''''.


2) -'''s'''
..


For example ... '''doikoris''' = "They say he walked" ....... That is the speaker was told by some third party(ies) or overheard some third party(ies) talking.
=== ... Two probability particles  ===


3) -'''a'''
..


For example ... '''doikria''' = "he walked, I saw him" ...... That is the speaker saw it with his own eyes.
[[Image:SW_051.png]]


Note that the above evidential only co-occurs with the past tense and near-past tense. Actually when used with the near-past tense, '''e.a''' => '''ia'''. Hence when this evidential is used, we loose the distinction between "past" and "near-past".
..


Now there is a forth possibility for this slot ... and it is not actually an evidintial. Furthermore it has the same form as 3).
'''lói''' = probably


4) -'''a'''  
'''màs''' = possibly


For example ... '''doikorua''' = "he intends to walk" ... the agent in this case, of course, must be a sentient being (i.e. human).
If nothing is in this slot, one assumes probability is 100% ... the option to challenge the underlying premise is never really considered.


Note that the above only co-occurs with the future tense.  
The probability distribution for '''lói''' centres around 85 %.


5) -'''ø'''  
The probability distribution for '''màs''' centres around 50 %.


This is the null morpheme. If the speaker doesn't know the evidential or deems it unimportant, then the null morpheme is used. According to corpus studies in '''béu''', 60% - 70% of indicative mood verbs have the null morpheme.
One can indicate a probability distribution centred around 15 % by using '''lói''' + '''bù'''. For example ... '''lói bù doikor''' = He/she probably doesn't walk.


..
..


It can be seen that the '''béu''' evidentiality inventory is quite substantial compared to other languages ... http://wals.info/chapter/78
=== ... Two habituality particles ===


..
..


==== .. Slot 5====
[[Image:SW_052.png]]


..
..


This slot can have the "perfect aspect marker" '''yə''' or not (you can call the second case the null morpheme choise ... if you want)
Every verb can be considered to have a default probability distribution over time.
 
[[Image:TW_984.png]] .... By the way, don't worry too much about the time scale in these sketched.


..
..


The perfect tense, logically doesn't differ that much difference from the past tense,. but it is emphasizing a state rather than an action. It represents the state at the time of speaking as the outcome of past events. We have this aspect in English and it is realized as "have -en".  
'''timpa''' and '''nko''' have very simple default probability shapes. But the typical (possible) probability distribution for '''kludau toili''' is more complicated.


For example if you wanted to talk to John and you went to his office, his secretary might say "he has gone to lunch, this emphasizes the absence of John as opposed to "he went for lunch". The latter is just an action that happened in the past, the former is a present state brought about by a past action.  
..


For another example ... "she read the book on geometry"
[[Image:SW_001.png]]


This doesn't specify whether she read it all the way thru or whether she  just read a bit of it. Whereas ...
Likewise the typical (possible) probability distribution for '''bunda tìa'''.


"she has read the book on geometry", implies she read the book all the way thru, but more importantly the connotation is that at the present time she has knowledge of geometry.
We can group all verbs into 3 classes occording to their probability distribution over time.


..
1) Punctual event ... '''timpa'''
 
2) Steady state ....... '''nko'''


The perfect marker -'''yə''' was probably derived from '''ìa''' "to finish/to complete" in its verb chain form. It has been suggested that it could have been derived from '''yái''' "to have/to possess" in its verb chain form but this is now considered very unlikely. The perfect aspect occurs in roughly half of the languages of the world ... http://wals.info/chapter/68
3) Process ............ '''kludau toili''' or '''bunda tìa'''


..
Now every verb (actually "very situation" would be more acurate) have a range of typical probability distributions associated with them. However the '''béu''' aspect markers IMPOSE a typical probability distributions on any verb they touch.


Also it appears that 5 categories being appended to the verb is typical of languages of the world. See ... http://wals.info/chapter/22  [If I have understood the chapter properly]
For example the particle '''awa''' imposes a probability distribution quite similar to '''kludau toili''' on ANY verb that it come in contact with.


..
'''awa*''' gives a "habitual but irregular" (maybe best translated as "now and again" or "occasionally" or even "not usually") meaning to the verbal block.  


=== ... The imperative===
The particle '''bolbo*''' is similar to '''awa''' in a way. However it implies quite a bit of regularity. Maybe the regularity implied by ...


..
[[Image:TW_985.png]]


You use the following forms for giving orders ... for giving commands. When you use the following forms you do not expect a discussion about the appropriateness of the action ... although a discussion about the best way to perform the action is possible.
'''bolbo''' gives a "habitual and regular" (best translated as "normally" or "usually" or "regularly") meaning to the verbal block.  


..
..


For non-monosyllabic verbs ...
We saw earlier that of the five tenses. The first is a sort of habitual tense. For example ...


The final vowel of the '''maŋga''' is deleted and replaced with '''u'''.
'''doikar''' = I walk (with a sort of habitual meaning) ... OR ... I can walk (with a sort of potential meaning)


'''doika''' = to walk
'''beucar''' = I am sick ... OR ... I am prone to sickness


'''doiku''' = walk !
So we have a sort of habitual meaning without needing to use either  '''awa''' or '''bolbo'''.


..
However, if we wanted to restrict the habitualness to either the past or the future,  '''awa''' or '''bolbo''' is needed. For example ...


For monosyllabic verbs -'''hu''' is appended.
'''bolbo doikari''' = I used to walk (to school)


'''gàu''' = "to do"
'''awa beucaru''' = I will be sick (when I start the chemotherapy)


'''gauhu''' = "do it" ... often '''''' is added fot extra emphasis.
'''awa''' or '''bolbo''' most often co-occur with tense (2) and tense (3). It is quite rare to have the right circumstances to use '''awa''' or '''bolbo''' with the other three tenses.


'''só gauhu''' = do it !
..


One verb has an irregular form.
'''*''' '''awa''' is possibly related to the verb '''awata'''  which means "to wander". '''bolbo''' is possibly related to the verb '''bolbolo''' which means "to roll". [by the way '''boloi''' means "to turn over" (as in "to turn over a mat"). '''boloi''' also means revolution [ '''boloi peugan''' means "social revolution" or '''boloi tun''' means "political revolution" ... i.e. the French Revolution ]. '''gwò''' is possibly related to the verb '''gwói''' which means "to pass (by)".


'''jò''' = "to go"
..


'''ojo''' = "go" ... actually a bit abrupt, probably expressing exasperation, veering towards "fuck off" ... '''jò''' itself can be used as a very polite form.
=== ... Three aspect particles ===


..
..


The imperative cab be directed at second person singular or second person plural. When addressing a group and issuing a command to the entire group you sort of let your eyes flick over the entire group. When addressing a group and issuing a command to one person you keep your eyes on this person when issuing the command ... maybe saying their name before the command ... probably preseded by '''só''' which is a vocative marker as well as being an emphatic particle.
Three aspect and a negating particle
 
..


[ Note ... I think that in English, the infinitive usually has "to" in front of it, in order to distinguish it from the imperative. In '''béu''' too there is a need to distinguish between these two verb forms. However as the imperative occurs less often than the infinitive, I have decided to mark the imperative. ]
[[Image:SW_053.png]]  


..
..


=== ... The prohibitive===
With the three particles '''pín''', '''gwò''' and '''juku''', the fifth tense (present tense) never co-occurs.


..
..


This is also called the negative imperative. Semantically it is the opposite of the imperative. It is formed by putting the particle '''kyà''' before '''maŋga'''.
Maybe the best way to approach '''pín''' and '''gwò''' is to consider process verb like "read the book" or "build a house" '''*'''


'''kyà doika''' =  don't walk
Well you could say ...


That is pretty much all there is to say about it.
'''bù bundar tìa''' = "I don't build houses" ... which would put you out of the running.


..
But if you said '''bundar tìa''' ... and you were expected to build a house, one of the following might be applicable ...


=== ... The optative===
1) '''hogi bù bundar tìa''' = I still haven't started to build the house


..
2) '''pín bundar tìa''' = I am in the process of building a house


This form expresses a wish or hope of the speaker ...  but there is no appeal for the addressee to act. Also it is not <u>really</u> giving information as such. It is more about letting the speaker express his emotions [ maybe "ventative would be a suitable name for it  :-) ]
3) '''gwò bundar tìa''' = I have built the house


The form is introduced by the particle '''fò'''. This particle has no other uses. It always comes utterance initial.
It is (2) and (3) we are interested in at the moment.  


It expresses wishful thinking. For example ... '''fò blèu doika''' = "Oh to be able to walk" ... '''fò sàu jini''' = "I wish I was clever"
Notice that '''bù bundara tìa''' = "I am not building a house" can be true when (2) is true. Remember that tense 5 refers to the EXACT time of speaking.


This form is used for curses and benedictions ... by frequency of usage the former outnumber the latter by about 10 to 1. For example ...  
[[Image:SW_056.png]]


'''fò gò diablos ò ʔaworu''' = "May the Devil take him"
..


There are some formula type expressions that are used in certain situations/ rituals that use this form.. For example '''xxx''' = "God save the king"
In English, it is a bit of a mouthful to say "I am in the process of building a house". So you can see that '''pín''' is a useful little particle when you want to be specific in this particular situation. However '''pín''' is the rarest out of '''pín''', '''gwò''' and '''juku'''.


The most common use of '''''' is the greeting '''fò fales sàu gipi''' "may peace be upon you"
[Is '''pín''' also a preposition meaning during ... preceding a noun which is a period of time ?]


The verb form in this construction is usually '''maŋga'''. Most often hopes and wishes are for the future, but sometimes they are orientated towards the past (I suppose they should be called regrets in these cases). For example ...
..


"If only you had arrived yesterday"
Lets talk about '''gwò''' now.


In these cases the R-form is used after the particle '''gò'''.


"If only you had arrived yesterday" => '''fò gò diriyə jana'''
As we can see in (3), '''gwò''' is linked to the idea of completion. It is also linked to the idea of having done something at least once (to have "experienced" some action, in other words). For example ...


The table below shows the optative construction ... either with the particle '''''' plus '''maŋga''' <u>OR</u> with the particles '''fò gò''' plus the R-form.
'''gwò jàr glasgoh''' = "I've been to Glasgow" as opposed to '''jari glasgoh''' = I went to Glasgow


[[Image:TW_688.png]]
As I said above, the present tense never co-occurs with '''pín''', '''gwò''' and '''juku'''. However the other 3 tenses are possible  ...


..
'''gwò jaru glasgoh''' = I will have been to Glasgow
 
'''gwò jari glasgoh''' = I had been to Glasgow (with reference time sometime before today)


=== ... The suggestive===
'''gwò jare glasgoh''' = I had been to Glasgow  (with reference time earlier today)


..
'''gwò''' could be called an experiential/resultative perfect. '''béu''' also has a resultative perfect expressed with the copula '''sàu''' and the suffix -'''in'''.


We have come across '''kái''' before. In chapter 2.10 we saw that it was a question word meaning "what kind of". It normally follows a noun being an adjective. For example ...
The aspect distinctions available in '''béu''' are pretty fine-grained in some areas. Maybe if '''béu''' were to become a natlang, many of the fine-grain distinctions I have given it would fall by the wayside.


'''báu kái''' = what type of man ?
..


'''ò r báu kái''' = what type of man is he ?
And now it's time to introduce '''juku'''. When '''gwò''' expresses the experiential idea (as it does above) '''juku''' expresses the non-experiential idea ...


'''ò r deuta kái''' = what type of soldier is he ?
'''juku jare glasgoh''' = I had never been to Glasgow (with reference time, earlier today)


'''nendi kái''' = this is what type ?
'''juku jari glasgoh''' = I had never been to Glasgow (with reference time, before today)


But just as a normal adjective can be a copula complement, so can '''kái'''.
'''juku jaru glasgoh''' = I will never go to Glasgow (with reference time, before today)


'''ò r kái''' = what type is he ?
'''juku''' like '''gwò''' is most often referenced to NOW. Hence ...


'''nendi r kái''' = this is what type ?
'''juku jàr glasgoh''' = I have never been to Glasgow.


'''ʃì r kái''' = what type of thing is it ?
..


However when you see '''kái''' utterance initial you know that it has a slightly different function : it is introducing the "soliciting opinion" mood. For example ...
It is useful to compare the usage of '''juku''' against the usage of '''bù'''.This can best be explained by taking a punctual verb such as '''timpa'''. For example, suppose we were discussing "John hitting Paul yesterday afternoon". That particular instance of "hitting" can be negated with ''''''. However suppose it is wished to widen what is negated. Suppose that you want to say that there has been no instances of "John hitting Paul" (up until the present time of course), then you would use '''juku''' to negate the proposition. This is equivalent to "never" in English and I consider it an aspect particle.


'''kái wìa nyáu nambon jindi''' = How about we go home now ? <u>OR</u> Let's go home now.
'''jonos polo bù timpori''' = John did not hit Paul


Now ... as with the "optative", the "soliciting opinion" mood is usually orientated towards the future and uses '''maŋga'''. However their are circumstances where you solicit opinion about past events [for example a group of detectives on a crime scene discussing the possible steps taken by the perpetrator]. In these circumstances the R-form would be used preceded by the particle '''''' ...  [see the table in the section above]
'''jonos polo juku timpori''' = John never hit Paul .... Notice that both '''timpori''' or '''timpore''' could be used. It depends upon what has been said before.


The <u>main</u> thing about this mood is that the speaker is asking for feedback/advice/approval or disapproval. But it overlaps with the field "gently suggesting a course of action" somewhat.
'''bù''' is purely negation. It has no aspect to it.


..
[Note 1 ... The way '''juku''' negates '''gwò''' keeping the same aspect is similar to the way 没 méi (or 没有 méiyǒu) negates 了 le the perfect aspect particle, in Mandarin. 不 [bù] not being involved, just as '''bù''' isn't involved in '''béu'''. ]


=== ... The interrogative===
[Note 2 ... One little thing you should be aware off. I have equated '''juku''' with "never". Taking more strictly it should be equated with "have never". Let me expand on this ...


..
a) "he has never worked" => '''juku kodor'''.


Also called Polar Questions. A polar question is a question that can be answered with "yes" or "no".
b) "he doesn't work" or "he never works" => '''bù kodor''' .... in this one "never" in English is equivalent to the timeless tense plus the normal negator ... '''juku''' doesn't make an appearance ]


..
..


To turn a normal statement ( i.e. with the verb in its R-form) into a polar question the particle '''ʔai?''' is stuck on at the very end.
So to restate the '''béu''' aspect system ...
 
It has its own symbol (and I transcribe it as '''ʔai?''') because it possesses its own tone contour.


I have mentioned this particle in chapter 1 (if you look back you can see its exact tone contour). Here is its symbol again ... [[Image:TW_399.png]]


And here is an example of it in action ...
'''juku kludar toili dè''' = I have never read that book ... not one word


[[Image:TW_492.png]] ... '''jono jaŋkori ʔai?''' = Did John run ?
'''pín kludar toili dè''' = I have not completed that book (but I have read some of it)


..
'''gwò kludar toili dè''' = I have read that book .............. every word


'''ʔai?''' is neutral as to the response expected ... well at least in positive questions.


To answer a positive question you answer '''ʔaiwa''' "yes" or '''aiya''' "no" (of course if "yes" or "no" are not adequate, you can digress ... the same as any language).  
It is not really felicitous to say '''*bù kludar toili dè'''. However if you dropped the object, then '''bù kludar''' is acceptable.


Here is an example of a positive question ...
'''bù kludar''' => "I don't read" or "I never read" or even "I can't read" [This can be regarded as an event with a probability distribution over time, similar to '''nko'''. That is it is a sort of generic steady state event. For these sort of events '''bù''' is the normal negator]


'''glá r hauʔe ʔai?''' = Is the woman beautiful ?
"I don't intend to read this book" would be '''bù kludarua toili dè''' [And I think that exhausts everything I could want to do regarding "a/the book"]


If she is beautiful you answer '''ʔaiwa''', if not you answer '''aiya'''<sup>*</sup>.
In a similar way constructions like "horses never fly" '''*kài fanfa juku ngur''' are frowned upon. "horses don't fly" '''kài fanfa bù ngur''' is considered more felicitous.


..
..


To answer a negative question it is not so simple. '''ʔaiwa''' and '''aiya''' are deemed insufficient to answer a negative question on their own. For example ...
To restate the system yet again'''**''' ...


'''glá bù r hauʔe ʔai?''' = Is the woman not beautiful ?
{| border=1
 
  |align=center| '''gwò kodor'''
If she is not beautiful, you should answer '''bù hauʔe'''<sup>**</sup>, if she is you can answer either '''hù hauʔe''' or '''glá r hauʔe'''  
  |align=left| he has worked
 
  |align=center| '''juku kodor'''
I guess a negative question expects a negative answer, so a positive answer must be quite accoustically prominent (that is a short answer ("yes" or "no") is not enough)
  |align=left| he has never worked
  |-
  |align=center| '''gwò kodori'''
  |align=left| he had worked
  |align=center| '''juku kodori'''
  |align=left| he had never worked
  |-
  |align=center| '''gwò kodore'''
  |align=left| he has worked (earlier today)
  |align=center| '''juku kodore'''
  |align=left| he hasn't worked (so far) today
  |-
  |align=center| '''gwò kodoru'''
  |align=left| he will have worked
  |align=center| '''juku kodoru'''
  |align=left| he will never have worked
  |}


..
..


We have mentioned '''''' already ... in the above section about '''seŋko'''. This is the focus particle. It has a number of uses. When you want to emphasis one word in a clause, you would stick '''''' in front of it<sup>***</sup>.
These three aspect particles also occur quite frequently in fronted adverb clauses. In these, '''pín''', '''gwò''' or '''juku''' are followed by an base form (plus any other bits and pieces relevant to the clause), then the main clause follows. English has similar. Here are three examples from English, illustrating the possible uses of these fronted adverb clauses ...


Another use for '''só''' is when hailing somebody .... '''só jono''' = Hey Johnny
1a) '''pín doika ... ''' : Walking dejectedly home, Peter noticed a sudden movement in the hedgerow.


You can also stick it in front of someone's name when you are talking to them. However it is not a "vocative case" exactly. Well for one thing it is never mandatory. When used the speaker is gently chiding the listener : he is saying, something like ... the view you have is unique/unreasonable or the act you have done is unique/unreasonable. When I say unique I mean "only the listener" hold these views : the listener's views/actions are a bit strange.
1b) '''tìa pà pín bunda''', I HAD TO LOOK AFTER TWO DAUGHTERS


When stuck in front of a non-multi-syllable verb you get an imperative. For example ... '''só nyáu''' = Go home
2a) '''gwò doika ... ''' : Having walked all the way home in the rain, Peter was ready for a hot bath and a cosy night in, in front of the TV.


'''''' can also be used to highlight one element is a statement or polar question. For example ...
2b)'''gwò''' TO TAKE CITY, HE BURNT IT : urbem captem incendit


Statement ... '''bàus glán nori alha''' = the man gave flowers to the woman
3) '''juku jò ... ''' : Never having gone to Casablanca before, Peter soon got lost in a warren of small streets just north of the Bazaar.


Focused statement ... '''bàus só glán nori alha''' = It is the woman to whom the man gave flowers.<sup>****</sup>
These type of fronted adverb clauses are considered good style. One comes across them quite often. Notice that the tense of the whole sentence is determined by the main clause.


Unfocused question ... '''bàus glán nori alha ʔai?''' = Did the man give flowers to the woman ?
..


Focused statement ... '''bàus só glán nori alha ʔai?''' = It is to the woman that the man gave flowers ?
Note ... '''pín''' can also stand before a noun, a noun that represents a period of time. In which case it means "during". Or is can stand before a base verb, in which case it is equivalent to "while" or "during". Or it can appear in an active predicate, where it specifies a certain aspect type.


..
..


Any argument can be focused in this way.
NOTE TO SELF ... does '''pín''' cover all occurrences of "while" and "when" in English ?


..
..


<sup>*</sup>These words have a unique tone contour as well ... at least when spoken in isolation. I suppose I should have given these two words a symbol each ... if I wanted to be consistent.
'''*''' I do not consider "read" and "build" in themselves to be process verbs, they are sort of open-ended affairs. But for "read the book" and "build a house" there is a definite completion time ... and completion state, implied.
 
<sup>**</sup>Mmm ... maybe you could answer '''ʔaiwa''' here ... but a bit unusual ... not entirely felicitous.
 
<sup>***</sup>In English, when you want to emphasis a word, you make it more accoustically prominent : you don't rush over it but give it a very careful articulation. This is iconic and I guess all languages do the same. It is a pity that there is no easy way to represent this in the English orthography apart from increasing the font size or adding exclamation marks.


<sup>****</sup>English uses a process called "left dislocation" to give emphasis to an element in a clause.
'''**''' You can't have too much of a good thing.


..
..


The other type of question ... the content question was covered in the last chapter.
=== ... Aspectual operators ===


..
..


=== ... The conflative===
Two overlapping-action particles


..
..


Also called the i-form.  [By the way "conflative" is my term ... I thought I would join in the fun and make up a silly name myself]
[[Image:SW_054.png]]  
 
I will only touch on this here. Nearer the end of this chapter there is a section that goes into this in a lot more detail. OK one quick example ...
 
to walk = '''doika'''
 
road = '''komwe'''
 
to follow = '''plèu'''
 
to whistle = '''wiza'''
 
From the above we could make three short sentences.
 
John walked  => '''jono doikori'''
 
John followed the road => '''jonos komwe plori'''
 
John whistled => '''jono wizori'''


..
..


However as all three verbs seem to take part in the same action they can be combined. The first verb in the combination is normal (whether it is r-form, u-form, s-form or in fact '''manga''').
I call '''ʔés''' and '''hogi''' "overlap words".


The following verbs in the combination take a special ending ... -'''i''' for multi-syllable words and the schwa '''ə''' for mono-syllable words. So we get the form ...
Sometimes referred to as  "aspectual operators" or "aspectual particles" in the Western Linguistic Tradition.


John walked along the road whistling => '''jono doikori komwe plə wiʒi'''
Most languages have equivalents to these two particles ...


..
..


== ..... Verbs & Valency==
{|border=1
 
|align=center| English
..
|align=center| already
 
|align=center| still
In the Western Linguistic Tradition we talk about intransitive verbs (for example "sleep") that have one participant (also called an "argument"). Also we talk about transitive verbs (for example "cut") which have two. Then we have ones with three arguments which are called ditransitive verbs. But I hate this nomenclature ... from now on I will talk about one-place, two-place and three-place verbs. And I will write it in shorthand form ...
|-
 
|align=center| German
 
|align=center| schon
[[Image:TW_659.png]]
|align=center| noch
 
|-
 
|align=center| French
Actually you can have [0000] as well. For example ....
|align=center| déjà
 
|align=center| encore
..
|-
 
|align=center| Mandarin
{|  
|align=center| yîjing
|-
|align=center| hái
! jono-s || jene-n || laigau || haun-o-r-a || eŋglaba-tu
|-
|-
|align=center| Dutch
| John-{{small|ERG}} || Jane-{{small|DAT}}|| calculus || teach-{{small|3SG-IND-PRES}} || English-{{small|INST}}
|align=center| al
|} ==> John is teaching calculus to Jane in English
|align=center| nog
 
|-
..
|align=center| Russian
 
|align=center| uže
The main thing is that you have to distinguish between the various arguments somehow. English uses position with respect to the verb to distinguish between the "doer" and the "done-to". The prepositions "to" and "in" are used to show what the roll of the other two arguments have. As you can see ... '''béu''' uses suffixes to distinguish between all the different arguments.
|align=center| eščë
 
|-
For various reasons ... it is not necessary to talk about [0000] in the grammer of a language.
|align=center| Serbo-Croatian
 
|align=center| već
[000] should be taken into consideration. However it is [00] and [0] that need the most attention. The rest of this section is about [00] and [0].
|align=center| još
 
|-
..
|align=center| Finnish
 
|align=center| jo
And now to introduce the S A O convention, this is just a way used to refer to the arguments of [0] and [00] ... the one argument of [0] is referred to as the S argument ... the argument of [00] in which the success of the action most depends is referred to as the A argument ... the argument of [00] which is most affected by the action is called the O argument.
|align=center| vielä
 
|-
..
|align=center| Swedish
 
|align=center| redan
O was probably chosen from "object", A from "agent" and S from "subject" ( I find this useful to keep in mind as a memory aid). However O does not "mean" object and A does not mean agent and S does not mean subject. I (and many other linguists) use the word subject to refer to either A or S. Easier to talk about "subject" that to talk about "A or S" all the time.
|align=center| än(nu)
|-
|align=center| Indonesian
|align=center| sudah
|align=center| masih
|-
|align=center| '''béu'''
|align=center| '''ʔés'''
|align=center| '''hogi'''
  |}


..
..


Now in English certain verbs appear to be  [0] in some situations and [00] in others. These are called ambitransitive verbs. For example ...
'''hogi''' indicates ...


..
1) An activity is ongoing.


1) The door opened
2) The activity must stop some time in the future, possibly quite soon.


2) The old woman opened the door
3) There is a certain expectation<sup>*</sup> that the activity should have stopped by now.


..
'''ʔés'''  indicates ...


We could say that the above are two different takes on the same situation. In (1) we have  [0] and "door" = S ...  in (2) we have  [00] and "door" = O. So we can say "open" is an ambitransitive verb of type S=O.
1) An activity is ongoing.


..
2) The activity was not ongoing some time in the past, possibly quite recently.


3) The old woman knitted
3) There is a certain expectation<sup>*</sup> that the activity should not have started yet.
 
4) The old woman knitted a sweater


..
..


We could say that the above are two different takes on the same situation. In (3) we have  [0] and "old woman" = S ...  in (4) we have  [00] and "old woman" = A.  
<sup>*</sup> Inevitably a connotation of "contrary to expectation" will develope to a certain degree. This is because if the situation was according to expectation often nothing would need be utterred. Hence '''hogi'''
and '''ʔés''' are often found in contrary to expectation situation which in turn colours their meaning.


..
..


RMW Dixon calls "knit" is an ambitransitive verb of type S=A. I do not. I just say that "knit" is [00] but sometimes the O argument can be dropped if it is considered trivial. I don't think we are disagreeing about anything ... just I am using a different terminology ... well I want to be able to say that '''béu''' has no ambitransitive verbs.
[[Image:SW_046.png]]  
Well ... anyway, putting aside what Dixon calls ambitransitive verbs of type S=A ... lets get back to (1) and (2). In '''béu''' "open" '''mapa''' is [00]. So how would we express (1) in '''béu'''. Well ...  as in (4) where we consider the O totally unimportant and just drop it to get (3) ... why not simply drop the A. Well the problem is that the subject (S or A) must be represented in slot "1" of the indicative verb. How should we know what to put in here ( see Ch3.1.2.1 ) ?  Well we could take the 3 person plural suffix -'''u'''- ... chances are that it is a 3rd person agent and the plural is more generic than the singular. This is what Russian does to make a sort of a passive. '''béu'''s solution is to use a schwa. We don't know which of the 7 markings for person to use ... so everything sort of collapses in ... to the schwa ... an impersonal schwa.


..
..


[[Image:TW_664.png]]
A very interesting thing about the overlap couplet is how they are negated cross-linguisticly. Either the particle can be negated or the verb can be negated. The first case I represent with a bar over the operator+verb. The second case with a bar over the verb only.
"the door opened" => "the door was opened"  = '''pintu mapəri''' (Actually I do not think the schwa symbol is distinct enough ... from now on I will use a dash)  =>  '''pintu map-ri'''


..


Here are some examples of this construction [ I will call it the schwa construction from now on ]
Notice ... compared to the positive case, if the operator+verb is negated ... the line that represents onset/cessation of activity is moved to the other side of the dashed line representing "now".


'''beuba sw-r dían''' = "The language of '''béu''' is spoken here"
Notice ... compared to the positive case, if the verb is negated ... then the yellow place becomes white and the white space becomes yellow.


'''pí gaudoheu dè_sweno g-r''' = "In this factory telephones are made"
..


'''toilia bù ost-r pí duka dí''' = "Books are not sold in this shop"
[[Image:SW_007.png]] .... [[Image:TW_996.png]]
 
'''pintu by-ru mapa''' = '''pintu r mapua''' = the door has to be opened
 
'''pintu bl-r mapa''' = the door can be opened ...........  [ to understand this example and the one above it ... see Ch 4.7 ]
 
'''hala dè nyal-ryə''' = that rock is eroded .......... '''nyale''' = to erode, to wear


..
..


Note ... the schwa can not support any tone. And as it is only used in the grammer and not in any base words as such it was not introduced in Chapter 1 (as '''r''' was not). The schwa is in fact a cross ...
As you see by above ... by changing whether the negator act on the operator+verb or whether only on the verb give diametrically opposite meanings.


[[Image:TW_724.png]]
Note that there are 4 possible negative cases to choose from and a language only needs 2. A language (to cover all negative cases) should be either "(a) (b) type" or "(c) (d) type" or " (a) (c) type" or "(b) (d) type"


Also note ... Some people pronouns "schwa" + "syllable final rhotic" as  "ø". These people also tend to give "ø" the proper tone. However the majority pronoun a schwa followed by a rhotic appoximant with neutral tone.
Cross linguistically there are interesting variations. All Slavic languages prefer verb negation, hence they are (c) (d) types.


..
In German, only (a) and (c) are allowed in positive declarations.  


Now "door" is a man-made object and probably it exists in a place with many people around. So it is reasonable to expect there to be ''human volition'' involved when it opens. But what about when we get out into nature. When we see a river freezing. There is no agent to be seen behind this "freezing" ... it just happens. For this reason the verb "to freeze" '''doska''' is [0].
Nahuatl has negation of the operator so is (a) (b) type.


But now we have become clever ... we hold dominion over nature. Hence we need to derive a word for freeze that is [00]. And that deriration is arrived at by appending -'''n''' (historically this was -'''nau''' and even further back it was the independent word '''náu''' "to give")
English is a bit tricky ... it has suppletion and uses "not yet" for situation (c) and "no longer" for situation (d). Now in English "yet" means pretty much the same as "still". I believe "yet" was the original particle but "still" over time largely usurped it in the positive case. However the form "not yet" ... if taken at face value would seem to negate the operator. But it doesn't. Logically it would make more sense if we said "yet not" instead of "not yet" [i.e. we have situation (c) rather than (b)]. I am sure there is a perfectly good explanation for this reversal but unfortunately I do not know it ... anyway ... nothing to worry about too much. [ The form "not work yet" seems more logical in its word order ... how can "not" in "not yet work" have "work" under its scope but not "yet" ... but apparently that is the way it works ]


Hence ...
In '''béu''', '''bù''' negates the verb and comes immediately before the verb. It has scope only over the verb, rather than the whole verb phrase.


'''moze doskori''' = the water froze
----


'''moze doskaniri''' = You froze the water
{|
 
|-
..
! hogi || kod-a-r-a || dían
|-
| still || work-{{small|1SG-IND-PRES}}  || here
|} ==> I am still working here


Actually any [0] can take this suffix and become [00]. Here are a few more examples ...


..
{|
|-
! ʔés || kod-a-r-a || dían
|-
| already || work-{{small|1SG-IND-PRES}}  || here
|} ==> I already work here
 
----
 
{|
|-
! hogi || bù || kod-a-r-a || dían
|-
| still || not || work-{{small|1SG-IND-PRES}}  || here
|} ==> I don't work here yet
 


{| border=1
{|  
   || '''pyà'''
|-
  || to fly
! ʔés || bù || kod-a-r-a || dían
  ||  '''pyàn'''  
|-
  || to throw
| already || not || work-{{small|1SG-IND-PRES}}   || here
  |-
|} ==> I no longer work here
  ||  ''''''
 
  || to go
----
  ||  '''jón'''
 
  || to send
However although '''hogi bù''' and '''?é bù''' are possible, they are rarely encountered. Usually the terms '''jù dìa''' and '''uhoge''' are used. The provenance of these two terms is interesting ...
  |-
 
  ||  ''''''
'''''' means zero and is also used for negating nouns. '''dìa''' is a verb with quite a norrow meaning. It is what the sun does when it is revealing itself first thing in the morning.
  || to come
 
  ||  '''tèn'''
I guess '''jù dìa''' is an idiomatic expression.
  || to summon
 
  |-
'''''' means "long" [not to be confused with '''''' the 13th '''pila?o'''). '''hoge''' means "longer". So '''uhoge''' means "no longer".
  || '''bái'''
 
  || to rise
So the actual system for these two negatives are ...
  ||  '''báin'''
 
  || to raise
{|
  |-
|-
  ||  '''kàu'''
! jù dìa || kod-a-r-a || dían
  || to descend
|-
  ||  '''kàun'''
|  "not yet" || work-{{small|1SG-IND-PRES}}   || here
  || to lower
|} ==> I don't work here yet
  |-
 
  ||  '''dài'''
 
  || to die
{|  
  ||  '''dàin'''
|-
  || to kill
! uhoge || kod-a-r-a || dían
  |-
|-
   || '''slài'''
| "no longer" || work-{{small|1SG-IND-PRES}}   || here
  || to change
|} ==> I no longer work here
  || '''slàin'''
 
  || to change
----
  |-
 
  || '''diadia'''
These operators are usually used to specify overlap with present time ... (I call the present time, NOW, in the diagrams). I would think this is true of every language (notice that the above examples the tense is always -'''a'''). However it is a trivial matter to reference the time of onset/cessation of activity to a different time ... you just change the tense.
   || to happen
  |'''diadian'''
  || to cause
  |}


..
..


Six  [00] can also take -'''nau''' as well and become [000].  They are ...
== ... Verbal Moods==


..
..


{| border=1
When people speak they have different intentions. That is they are trying to achieve different things by speaking ... maybe they are trying to convey information, or wanting somebody to do something, or not to do something, or they are just expressing their feelings about something. All these are examples of what is called moods. Different languages have different methods of coding their moods. Also the various moods of a languages cover a different semantic range compared to other languages.
  ||  '''flò'''
 
  || to eat
There are 6 moods in '''béu'''. The prohibitive, indicative, optative, imperative, suggestive and interrogative ... 2 of these are represented by changes to the root and 4 by adding particles.
  ||  '''flòn'''
 
  || to feed
Two verb forms ... the inflinitive and the conflative ... do not represent moods, but I present them here along with the moods. These both are represented by changes to the root.
  |-
  ||  '''bwí'''
  || to see
  ||  '''bwín'''
  || to show
  |-
  ||  '''háu'''
  || to learn
  ||  '''háun'''
  || to teach
  |-
  || '''glù '''
  || to know
  || '''glùn'''
  || to inform
  |-
  || '''pòi'''
  || to enter, to join
  || '''pòin'''
  || to put in
  |-
  || '''féu'''
  || to exit, to leave
  || '''féun'''
  || to take out
  |}


..
..


(Note : '''fyá''' "to tell" means basically the same as '''glùn''' but is less formal )
[[Image:SW_189.png]]


..
..


We have discussed '''mapa''' and '''doska''' so far. The first is considered basically [00] and the second one basically [0]. There is a third type of verb ... for this type it is hard to say if it is more basic as [0] or more basic as [00]. So these verbs have <u>two</u> basic forms. For example ...
How the different moods and forms interact are shown above. This will al be explained later.


..
..


'''cwamo hulkori''' = the bridge broke
=== ... The base form===
 
'''deutais cwamo helkuri''' = the soldiers broke the bridge


..
..


Actually for the first example .. the chances are that the breakage was due to wear and tear caused by human activity. But the important thing is that it is non-volitional. Also there might have been no humans around when the bridge actually did break. So we can talk about the bridge breaking by itself ... as if by an act of nature. And another example ...
About 32% of multi syllable '''maŋga''' end in "a".


..
About 16% of multi syllable '''maŋga''' end in "e", and the same for "o".


'''jono wiltore''' = John woke up (earlier today)
About 9% of  multi syllable '''maŋga''' end in "au", and the same for "oi", "eu" and "ai".


'''jenes jone woltore ''' = Jane woke up John  (earlier today)
[[Image:TW_626.png]]


..
Note that no '''maŋga''' end in "i", "u", "ia" and "ua"


There are about 40 of these pairs. If the [0] has '''u''' the [00] will have '''e''' ...  if the [0] has '''i''' the [00] will have '''o'''.
"i" is reserved for marking verb chains, which will be explained later.


So lets summarize these three typre of verb ...
"u" is used for the imperative mood ... i.e. for commanding people.


..
"ia" is used for a past passive participle. For example ...


[[Image:TW_665.png]]
'''yubako''' = to strengthen


..
'''yubakia''' = strengthened ... as in '''pazba dí r yubakia''' => "this table is strengthened"


----
"ua" could be called the future passive participle I guess. For example ...


So to wrap it all up about verbs and arguments ...
'''ndi r yubakua''' => these ones must be strengthened


No verbs are ambitrasitive. They are either [0] or [00]. However it is easy to drop the A or the O argument from a [00] clause if either of them is considered trivial or is unknown.
To form a negative base form the word '''jù''' is placed immediately in front of the verb. For example ...


Now in '''béu''' any [00] can be given a [0] meaning ( grammatically the structure is still [00] ) by making the the O argument '''tí''' ... meaning himself, herself, yourself etc. etc. However only animate A arguments do this. Hence ...
'''doika''' = to walk


'''bàus tí timpori''' = the man hit himself  ................. acceptable
'''jù doika''' = to not walk .... not to walk


'''*pintus tí mapori''' = the door opened itself ...... unacceptable
..


In English there are two ways to report on a door opening without mentioning any agent ... "the door opened" and "the door was opened"
=== ... The imperative===
 
In '''béu''' only one ... '''pintu map-ri''' ... which is just a two place clause with the A argument dropped. Comparable to how "the old woman knitted" is a two place clause with the O argument dropped.


..
..


In '''béu''' you can make a passive participle by suffixing -'''ia'''.
You use the following forms for giving orders ... for giving commands. When you use the following forms you do not expect a discussion about the appropriateness of the action ... although a discussion about the best way to perform the action is possible.
 
If you come across something broken and you know it was broken by human volition ... you would call it '''helkia'''.
 
If you come across something broken and you did not know how it was broken ... you would call it '''hulkia'''.
 
If you come across something frozen you would call it '''doskia'''. There is no such word as '''*doskania'''.


..
..


In '''béu''' you can make the so called obligation participle by suffixing -'''ua'''.
For non-monosyllabic verbs ...


If you come across something that had to be broken ... you would call it '''helkua'''.
The final vowel of the '''maŋga''' is deleted and replaced with '''u'''.


If you come across something that had to be frozen ... you would call it '''doskanua'''.
'''doika''' = to walk


There is no such words as '''*doskua''' or '''*hulkua'''
'''doiku''' = walk !


..
..


The above method of presenting a verb like '''mapa''' all hint at human volition. To get rid of this connotation (to suggest that the event happened naturely) we must use '''lài''' "to become" plus an adjective. This is demonstrated below ...
For monosyllabic verbs -'''hu''' is appended.
 
'''gàu''' = "to do"


Consider '''geuko''' = "to turn green" ... [00] ... derived from '''gèu''' "green"
'''gauhu''' = "do it" ... often '''''' is added fot extra emphasis.


'''só gauhu''' = do it !


1) '''báu lí gèu''' = The man became green .. ........................ ''natural''
One verb has an irregular form.


2) '''báu  geuk-ri''' = The man was made green .................... ''human volition''
'''''' = "to go"


3) '''báus tí geukori''' = The man made himself green
'''ojo''' = "go" ... actually a bit abrupt, probably expressing exasperation, veering towards "fuck off" ... '''jò''' itself can be used as a very polite form.


..
..


Now consider '''mapa''' = "to open" ... [00]
The imperative cab be directed at second person singular or second person plural. When addressing a group and issuing a command to the entire group you sort of let your eyes flick over the entire group. When addressing a group and issuing a command to one person you keep your eyes on this person when issuing the command ... maybe saying their name before the command ... probably preseded by '''''' which is a vocative marker as well as being an emphatic particle.


[ Note ... I think that in English, the infinitive usually has "to" in front of it, in order to distinguish it from the imperative. In '''béu''' too there is a need to distinguish between these two verb forms. However as the imperative occurs less often than the infinitive, I have decided to mark the imperative. ]


1) '''pintu lì mapia''' = the door became opened = the door opened .......... ''natural'' ................ [ here the agent could be anything ... the wind ... or even some fairy '''cái'''  ... use your imagination ]
..


2) '''pintu map-ri''' = the door was opened ............................................... ''human volition'' .... [ this one implies that the agent was human but is either unknown or unimportant and the action  deliberate ]
=== ... The prohibitive===
 
Note ... there is no (3) here as a door is non-human.


..
..


In either of the (1)'s '''wistia''' "deliberately/carefully" or '''wistua''' "accidently/carelessly" can be added after'''*''' '''lì'''. This automatically makes Agent => Human
This is also called the negative imperative. Semantically it is the opposite of the imperative. It is formed by putting the particle '''kyà''' before '''maŋga'''.


The same for the (2)'s, but the incidence of '''wistua''' should greatly excede the incidence of '''wistia''' as "intention" is the default for this construction.
'''kyà doika''' =  don't walk


With (3) the connotation of intent is so strong that '''wistia'''/ '''wistua''' could be considered a bit infelicitous ... not impossible but indicative of a very unusual situation.
That is pretty much all there is to say about it.


'''*''' or '''wistiwe''' or '''wistuwe''' if not immediately after the verb. [by the way ... '''wisto''' = mind/brain]
..
 
=== ... The interrogative===


..
..


== ..... Short Verbs==
The interrogative, also called a polar question. This is a question that can be answered with "yes" or "no".


..
..


In a previous lesson we saw that the first step for making an R-form is to delete the final vowel from the '''maŋga'''. However this is only applicable for multi-syllable words.  
To turn a normal statement ( i.e. with the verb in its '''r'''-form) into a polar question the '''r''' is simply changed into '''?'''.


With monosyllabic verbs the rules are different. For monosyllabic verbs the R-form suffixes are simply added on at the end of the infinitive.


'''swó''' = to fear  ... '''swo.ar''' = I fear ... '''swo.ir''' = you fear ... '''swo.or''' = she fears ... 
And here is an example of it in action ...


Many '''béu''' speakers pronounce a glottal stop between the two parts, especially if they are speaking forcefully.


In my transcription a dot is inserted between the base and the suffixes. In the '''béu''' writing system the two vowels are simple written alongside.
[[Image:SW_195.png]] ... '''lea r tiji''' = Lea's small  [[Image:SW_190.png]] ... '''lea sòr tiji''' = Lea is small    [[Image:SW_191.png]]  ... '''lea so?o tiji''' = Is Lea small ?


..
..


[[Image:TW_725.png]]
Polar questions also exhibit a certain pitch contour ... the pitch rises towards the end of the utterance. There is a symbol to show this utterance pitch contour ... [[Image:SW_192.png]]
 
However the '''béu''' question mark is never used when it is obvious that we have a question. But sometimes a single name, noun or adjective can constitute a question by itself. In these cases the special symbol is used.
 
[[Image:SW_193.png]] ... Lea ?


..
..


For a monosyllabic verb ending in '''ai''' or '''oi''', the final '''i''' => '''y''' for the R-form.
The interrogative is neutral as to the response expected ... well at least in positive questions.
 
To answer a positive question you answer '''ʔaiwa''' "yes" or '''aiya''' "no" (of course if "yes" or "no" are not adequate, you can digress ... the same as any language).  


'''gái''' = to ache, to be in pain ... '''gayar''' = I am in pain ... '''gayir''' = you are in pain ...  
Here is a positive question ...


For a monosyllabic verb ending in '''au''' or  '''eu''', the final '''u''' => '''w''' for the R-form.
'''glá so?o hauʔe''' = Is the woman beautiful ?


'''ʔáu''' = to take, to pick up ... '''ʔawar''' = I take ... '''ʔawir''' = you take ...  
To which you answer '''ʔaiwa''' "yes" or '''aiya''' "no". [Actually these two words have their own unique intonation pattern ... at least when said in isolation (see CH1 : Some interjections) ]


..
..


However 37 monosyllabic '''maŋga''' are exceptions : they pattern exactly the same as poly-syballic verbs.
To answer a negative question it is not so simple. '''ʔaiwa''' and '''aiya''' are deemed insufficient to answer a negative question on their own. For example ...
 
'''glá bù so?o hauʔe''' = Isn't the woman beautiful ?
 
If she is not beautiful, you should answer '''bù sòr'''<sup>*</sup>, if she is you can answer either '''sòr''' or '''soro''' or '''sòr hau?e'''


..
..


{| border=1
We have mentioned '''''' already ... in the above section about '''seŋko'''. This is the focus particle. It has a number of uses. When you want to emphasis one word in a clause, you would stick '''''' in front of the word<sup>**</sup>.
  |align=left| '''ʔái''' = to want
 
  |align=left|
Another use for '''''' is when hailing somebody .... '''só jono''' = Hey Johnny
  |align=left|
 
  |align=left|
You can also stick it in front of someone's name when you are talking to them. However it is not a "vocative case" exactly. Well for one thing it is never mandatory. When used the speaker is gently chiding the listener : he is saying, something like ... the view you have is unique/unreasonable or the act you have done is unique/unreasonable. When I say unique I mean "only the listener" hold these views : the listener's views/actions are a bit strange.
  |-
 
  |align=left| '''mài''' = to get
'''''' can also be used to highlight one element is a statement or polar question. For example ...
  |align=left| '''myè''' = to store
  |align=left|
  |align=left|
  |-
  |align=left| '''yái''' = to have
  |align=left|
  |align=left|
  |align=left|
  |-
  |align=left| '''''' = to go
  |align=left|
  |align=left|
  |align=left| '''jwòi''' = to to pass through, undergo, to bear, to endure, to stand
  |-
  |align=left| '''féu''' = to exit
  |align=left| '''fyá''' = to tell
  |align=left| '''flò''' = to eat
  |align=left|
  |-
  |align=left| '''bái''' = to rise
  |align=left| '''byó''' = to own
  |align=left| '''blèu''' = to hold
  |align=left| '''bwí''' = to see
  |-
  |align=left| '''gàu''' = to do
  |align=left|
  |align=left| '''glù''' = to know
  |align=left| '''gwói''' = to pass by
  |-
  |align=left| '''día''' = to arrive / reach
  |align=left|
  |align=left|
  |align=left| '''dwài''' = to pursue
  |-
  |align=left| '''lài''' = to become
  |align=left|
  |align=left|
  |align=left|
  |-
  |align=left| '''cùa''' = to leave / depart
  |align=left|
  |align=left|
  |align=left| '''cwá''' = to cross
  |-
  |align=left| '''sàu''' = to be
  |align=left|
  |align=left| '''slài''' = to change
  |align=left| '''swé''' = to speak, to say
  |-
  |align=left| '''kàu''' = to fall
  |align=left| '''kyò''' = to use
  |align=left| '''klói''' = to like
  |align=left| '''kwèu''' = to turn
  |-
  |align=left| '''pòi''' = to enter
  |align=left| '''pyá''' = to fly
  |align=left| '''plèu''' = to follow
  |align=left|
  |-
  |align=left| '''té''' = to come
  |align=left|
  |align=left|
  |align=left| '''twá''' = to meet
  |-
  |align=left| '''wè''' = to think
  |align=left|
  |align=left|
  |align=left|
  |-
  |align=left| '''náu''' = to give
  |align=left| '''nyáu''' = to return
  |align=left|
  |align=left|
  |-
  |align=left| '''háu''' = to learn
  |align=left|
  |align=left|
  |align=left|
  |}
 
..


For example ... '''pòr nambo''' = he/she enters the house ... <u>not</u> *'''poyor nambo'''
Statement ... '''bàus gláh nori alha''' = the man gave flowers to the woman


The above are also among the most common verbs as well. If you are serious about learning '''béu''' you should try and memorize them as soon as possible.
Focused statement ... '''bàus só gláh nori alha''' = It is the woman to whom the man gave flowers.


..
Unfocused question ... '''bàus gláh no?i alha''' = Did the man give flowers to the woman ?


== ..... Copulas==
Focused statement ... '''bàus só gláh no?i alha''' = It is to the woman that the man gave flowers ?


..
..


There are two copula's ... '''sàu''' "to be" and '''lài''' "to become". You will see that they were listed among the 37 special short verbs. However they pattern differently from the other 35 as we shall see.
Any argument can be focused in this way. ['''béu''' also has a means of "fronting" to emphasize an element in a sentence. This is discussed elsewhere]
 
The three components of a copular clause have a strict order ... "copular subject" => "copula" => "copula complement" ... the same order as English in fact.
 
The copula subject is always unmarked.


..
..


However the indicative mood is not derived from the infinitive by the usual method. As you might remember the first 3 slots are mandory in the indicative form (the aortist tense being a null morpheme).
<sup>*</sup>Mmm ... maybe you could answer '''ʔaiwa''' here ... but a bit unusual ... not entirely felicitous.


But for '''sàu''' and '''lài'''  things are radically different. Below are the indicative forms for  '''sàu''' and '''lài'''.
<sup>**</sup>In English, when you want to emphasis a word, you make it more accoustically prominent : you don't rush over it but give it a very careful articulation. This is iconic and I guess all languages do the same. It is a pity that there is no easy way to represent this in the English orthography apart from increasing the font size or adding exclamation marks.


..
..


[[Image:TW_656.png]]
=== ... The suggestive===


..
..


Note that the third column (under '''lài''') are grammatically all R-form's ... even though they don't actually have any rhotic sound.


We have come across '''kái''' before. In chapter 2.10 we saw that it was a question word meaning "what kind of". It normally follows a noun being an adjective. For example ...


For '''sàu''' in the aortist tense, '''r''' is the complete copula. It is a clitic attached the the last vowel of the copula subject (however it is always written as a separate word). For example ....
'''báu kái''' = what type of man ?


'''tomo r tumu''' = Thomas is stupid
'''òn rò báu kái''' = what type of man is he ?


It takes the tone of the copula subject (if the copula subject has one).
'''òn rò deuta kái''' = what type of soldier is he ?


If the copula subject ends in a consonant then '''''' is used.  For example ....
'''dí kái''' = this is what type ?


'''géus rò solki''' = the green one is smoothe
But just as a normal adjective can be a copula complement, so can '''kái'''.


Evidentials can be added as normal to these forms. For example ...
'''òn rò kái''' = what type is he ?


'''jene gáu rìs hauʔe''' = "They say old Jane used to be beautiful"
'''dí r kái''' = this is what type ?


'''jono jutu lòn gáu''' = "I guess big John is becoming old"  ... note that '''lón''' is considered mote appropriate than '''lán'''. If the timeframe of the action was a lot shorter then '''lá''' would be considered appropriate.
'''?ò r kái''' = what type of thing is it ?


..
However when you see '''kái''' utterance initial you know that it has a slightly different function : it is introducing the "soliciting opinion" mood. For example ...


It is only the R-forms of the copula's which are irregular. All other forms are perfectly normal.
'''kái àn nyairu tìah jindi''' => "how about we go home now" =>  "let's go home now"


..
Actually '''kái àn''' is sometimes rendered simply '''àn'''. Maybe you come across the two alternatives an equal amount of times.


'''sàu bòi''' = Be good ................................................................. U-form
Is there any difference between the two forms ? Well ... yes. '''kái àn''' is used when the proposed venture is connected to leisure and pleasure. '''àn''' is used in more work-a-day situations.


'''kodor sə kludado''' = He works as a clark ................................ I-form
Now ... as with the "optative", the "soliciting opinion" mood is usually orientated towards the future and uses '''maŋga'''. However their are circumstances where you solicit opinion about past events [for example a group of detectives on a crime scene discussing the possible steps taken by the perpetrator]. In these circumstances the '''r'''-form would be used preceded by the particle '''tà''' ... [see the table in the section above]
 
The <u>main</u> thing about this mood is that the speaker is asking for feedback/advice/approval or disapproval. But it overlaps with the field "gently suggesting a course of action" somewhat.


..
..


Note that for copular clauses, the subject noun or pronoun can never be dropped, because the person/number information is gone (that is ... there is no component to the left of the "r"). For a normal verb ... if the subject is 1st or 2nd person ... then the pronoun is invariably dropped. For 3rd person, whether you have a proper noun, pronoun or nothing depends upon discourse factors.
=== ... The conflative===
 
..


'''wài r wikai tè nù r yubau''' = "we are weak but they are strong"
Actually the verb itself is called an '''i'''-form verb. But a clause that has one or more '''i'''-form verbs is called a conflative clause.


'''ʃì r helkia''' = "it is broken" [ '''ʃì  hulki''' ]
I will only touch on this subject here ... in Ch 10 there is a section that goes into this verb form in exhaustive detail. But one quick example ...


..
..


Often the object of certain transitive verbs can be unceremoniously dropped if it is thought too trivial and/or unknown. For example '''solbara''' "I am drinking" is complete in itself [both in English and in '''béu'''] even though drink and '''solbe''' are transitive verbs. That is ... no need to say "I am drinking something" in English ... no need to say '''solbara efan''' in '''béu'''. 
'''jana jonos holdori nti flə sainyi uya''' => "yesterday John caught, cooked and ate three fish"


The copula subject in certain situations can also be dropped. The reason for this is the mirror image to the reason for dropping an object. Whereas the object is thought "too trivial" or "unknown" the copula subject is thought too  "all encompassing". (English usually uses a dummy subject, "it" in similar situations)
..


When '''béu''' has no copular subject ... '''sər''' [ written as '''s-r''' from now on ] is the form of the copula used. ( Notice that this is what the 3sg indicative impersonal form of '''sàu''' would be if it were conjugated as a regular verb )
yesterday = '''jana'''


As with English, this construction is often used for the weather ...
to catch = '''holda'''
 
to cook = '''ntu'''
 
to eat = '''flò'''


'''fona''' = rain : '''fonia''' = rainy/raining : '''fonua''' = dry (well not raining). So ...
three = '''uya'''


'''s-ra fonia''' = it's raining
fish = '''sainyi'''


..
..


Often when discussing the advisability of some course of action a construction containing '''s-r''+   one of the adjectives '''neʒi''', '''boʒi''' or '''fàin'''  +   '''gò''' are used. For example ...
'''totai timpə+ri jw+ daun''' = the child was hit and died (instantly) [Note to self : how to say "the child was hit and died later"]


..
'''totai''' = a/the child


{|
'''timpa''' = to hit
|-
! səra ||align=center| neʒi || gò || ny-e-r-u || jindi
|-
| "it is"-{{small|PRES}} ||align=center| necessary || align=center| {{small|CMPZ}} || return-{{small|2PL-IND-FUT}} || now
|} ==> It is necessary that you (pl) will return to home now ==> You (pl) must go home right now


'''jwòi''' = to undergo


{|
'''dàu''' = to die
|-
! bù || səra ||align=center| fàin || gò || sw-a-r-u || ifan || jindi
|-
| {{small|NEG}} || "it is"-{{small|PRES}} ||align=center| appropriate || align=center| {{small|CMPZ}} || speak-{{small|1SG-IND-FUT}} || anything || now
|} ==> It is inappropriate that I will say anything now ==> I shouldn't say anything now


'''dàun''' = to kill


{|
'''jwòi dàun''' = to be killed
|-
!  sər-u ||align=center| boʒi || gò || jubu || j-u-r-u
|-
|  "it is"-{{small|FUT}} ||  optimum || {{small|CMPZ}} || nobody || go-{{small|3PL-IND-FUT}}
|} ==> It will be best if nobody goes


..
..


'''neʒi''' = necessary ................. the action is a vital part of some larger scheme (that will achieve some goal)  ........... '''moze r neʒi LIFE.wo''' = water is necessary for life
In a conflative clause, the first verb is conjugated as normal. However the remaining verbs are in their '''i'''-form. That is ... the final vowel of the '''manga''' is deleted and replaced with "i". If the verb is monosyllabic, the final vowel is replaced with a schwa. Semantically the'''i'''-form verbs follow the first verb. That is '''nti''' means '''ntu.ori''' and '''flə''' means '''flori'''.


'''boʒi''' = best ........................... the action will yield more benefits than other actions (or no action at all). 
In conflative clauses, there can only be one subject but there can be more than one object. A conflative clause can consist of a mixture of H verbs and ɸ verbs. If the first verb is H then the subject is in its ergative form, otherwise it is in its base form. In the example given here, the three verbs have a definite time order, so the verb order is pretty much set. But we shall see in Ch 10 many examples where this is not the case.
 
'''fàin'''  = fitting/appropriate...... the action will be approved of by society at large (or at least the subsection society that is interested in the matter).


..


Note the two nouns ... '''neʒis''' = "a necessity and '''boʒis''' = "the optimum"
Note ... in this example, all three verbs are intransitive and have the same object. So '''léu sainyi uya''' can not come between any of the verbs, but must come either before them all or after them all ...
'''jana jonos sainyi  uya holdori nti flə''' ="yesterday John caught, cooked and ate ''the'' three fish"


..
..


For new situations ... '''l-r''' is used instead of '''s-r'''.  (Notice that '''l-r''' is what the 3sg indicative impersonal form '''lài''' would be if it were conjugated as a regular verb). For example ...
My motivation for having the conflative is to express meanings such as "through" or "into" by pure verbs ... i.e. "to go through", "to enter".  


'''l-ra fonia''' = it starting to rain
Also the '''béu''' verb tail can get pretty long so I didn't want it to be necessary to repeat it three or four times in quick succession.
 
Conflative clauses are very often used to describe situations involving motion. But no actual restrictions on what verbs can enter into a conflative clause (of course the verbs plus other arguments must represent a coherent subset of reality. That is the overall clause must make sense semantically).


The form '''l-r''' is quite a bit rarer than '''s-r'''.


..
..


OK ... So '''sàu''' and '''lài''' are the two copula's of '''béu'''.
To say that one activity happens totally within the time of an other activity, we use the conflative plus the particle '''pín''' which we met earlier in this chapter. For example ...


----
'''jonos lailore pín doiki''' = "John sang while walking earlier today"


There is another verb, that while not a copula, can function in a way similar to '''s-r'''.
'''jonos lailore pín doiki tunheun''' = "John sang while walking to the civic centre earlier today"


While '''s-r''' connects an attribute ( adjective ) to the universe at large (well at least attaches an attribute to the local environment) '''y-r''' connects a noun to the universe at large. '''y-r''' is actually the 3sg indicativeimpersonal form of the verb '''yái''' "to have on you".  
The whole constuctions (i.e. '''pín doiki''' and '''pín doiki tunheuh''') are equivalent adverbs.  


'''yái''' is often used to connect a human subject to a object (stupid English) object. For example ...
An adverb meaning "the '''r'''-form (matrix verb) happened during the time of the '''pín''' + -'''i''' verb".


'''jonos yór kli.o''' = John has a knike
..


'''yái''' can also be used to connect a location subject to any physical object. For example ...
=== ... The optative===


{|
..
|-
!  tunheu-s ||  y-o-r-e || yiŋki || hè || yildos
|-
|  "townhall"-{{small|ERG}} ||  "have"-{{small|3SG-IND-PST}} || "attractive girls" || alot || morning
|} ==> the townhall had many attractive girls this morning'''*'''


This usage can become impersonalized (i.e. the locative subject can be deleted) and the meaning then becomes ... the physical object exists somewhere in the Universe. For example ...
See Ch 4 : The particles '''àn''' and '''gò'''


'''y-r dèus''' = "there is a God" or "God exists"
..


This construction can be negated in two ways ...
== ..... Negativity==
 
'''bù y-r dèus''' = "there isn't a God" : '''y-r jù dèus''' = "there is no God"


..
..


Going back to the original example ...
'''béu''' has three particles/prefixes for expressing negativity.


Different particles for different parts of speech. Usually the particle is immediately to the left of the concept it modifies.


'''y-r yiŋki hè''' = "There are many attractive girls" ............................................................................... [ they exist somewhere ... somewhere in the Universe ]
..


The above can be modified ... below we modify it with an "adjective phrase of location" '''tunheuʔe''' and an  "adjective phrase of time" [Notice  the tense of '''y-r''' must be adjusted to agree on the last one ]
[[Image:SW_145.png]]


(1) '''y-re yiŋki hè tunheuʔe yildos''' = "there were many attractive girls at the townhall this morning".....  [ this changes the meaning from "Somewhere in the Universe" => "a more Specific Locality" ]
..


Which actually means exactly the same as (2) '''tunheus yore yiŋki hè yildos'''
'''bù''' negates the live verb (i.e. the verb in its r-form). We have encountered '''''' already in the section "probability/aspect/negation".


Which in turn means pretty much the same as the copula sentence  (3) '''yiŋki hè rè tunheuʔe yildos''' = "many attractive girls were at the townhall this morning" ... so three ways to say the same thing.
The verb in its u-form is negated by the particle '''kyà''' to the left of the '''maŋga'''. For example ...


But note ...
..


'''*tunheuʔe rè yiŋki hè yildos''' = "at the townhall this morning were many attractive girls"
'''sauhu bòi'''= be good


The above construction that is allowed in English is not allowed in '''béu''' ... well you can't say "green is the man" in English
However '''kyà sàu bòi''' = "don’t be good" instead of '''*bù sauhu bòi'''


..
..


== ..... Another passive==
The verb in its u-form can not be negated.


..
..


We have seen the impersonal passive above (where the vowel before the '''r''' becomes a schwa.
'''u'''- can connect to any adjective.  


However there is another passive form made with the verb '''jwòi''' "to undergo" plus the infinitive.
'''?ár wèu u.ai''' = I want a nonwhite car (I want a car, any colour but white)


'''bwari jono katala lazde''' = I saw John cutting the grass ....................... '''katala lazde''' is a '''saidau kaza''' ..... '''katala''' is a '''saidau baga'''
'''u'''- can on occasion be prefixed to nouns, the same as "non"- is used in English. However this construction is quite rare.  


'''bwari lazde jwola kata''' = I saw the grass being cut ............................. '''jwola kata''' is a '''saidau kaza'''
'''u'''- can connect to some verbs. The number of verbs it can connect to is limited ... about 20 or 30. Here are some examples ...


'''bwari lazde jwola kata hí jono''' = I saw the grass being cut by John .... '''jwola kata hí jono''' is a '''saidau kaza'''
..


Note ... although the '''''' suffix is probably connected to the second '''pilamo''' it should be recognized as a separate siffix here. If it was the '''pilamo''' we would have ... '''bwari lazde là jwòi kata'''
{| border=1
  |align=center| '''kunja'''
  |align=center| to fold
  |align=center| '''ukunja'''
  |align=center| to unfold
  |-
  |align=center| '''laiba'''
  |align=center| to cover
  |align=center| '''ulaiba'''
  |align=center| to uncover
  |-
  |align=center| '''tata'''
  |align=center| to tangle
  |align=center| '''utata'''
  |align=center| to untangle
  |-
  |align=center| '''fuŋga'''
  |align=center| to fasten, to lock
  |align=center| '''ufuŋga'''
  |align=center| to unfasten, to unlock
  |-
  |align=center| '''benda'''
  |align=center| to assemble, to put together
  |align=center| '''ubenda'''
  |align=center| to take apart, to disassemble
  |-
  |align=center| '''pauca'''
  |align=center| to stop up, to block
  |align=center| '''upauca'''
  |align=center| to unstop
  |-
  |align=center| '''senza'''
  |align=center| to weave
  |align=center|  '''uzenza'''
  |align=center| to unravel
  |-
  |align=center| '''fiŋka'''
  |align=center| to put on clothes, to dress
  |align=center| '''ufiŋka'''
  |align=center| to undress
  |}


'''bwari lazde kataya''' = I saw the grass that has been cut
..


'''bwari lazde katawa''' = I saw grass that must be cut = I saw that the grass must be cut
'''''' negates nouns. In the next chapter we will encounter it in the section on numbers. It means "zero".


'''lazde katawa bwari''' = I saw the grass that must be cut
It also negates  '''maŋga''' or dead verbs.


'''bwari lazde nài r katawa'''
It also negates clauses. For example ...


'''jù àn ?ár jò''' = "not that I want to go"
..
..


== ..... Six causative constructions==
Sometimes '''béu''' uses two of these three methods in the same sentence. I guess you could call this double negation. Double negation does NOT cancel, and it does NOT produce emphatic negation.


..
..


 
Here is an example of '''bù'''/'''jù''' double negation ... '''jenes bù mbor jù flò cokolata''' ... meaning "Jane lacks the willpower to resist chocolates".
"John made Jane drink the water" is an English causative construction ... [Note on terminology ... we  call "John" the "causer" and  "Jane" the "causee"]


..
..


In a similar manner to English ... '''béu''' uses '''gàu''' (meaning "to do" or "to make") as the neutral term for coding causation. For example ...
And here is an example of ''''''.-'''u''' double negation ...
 
(a) '''jonos gore solbe moze jenen''' = John made Jane drink water (earlier today)


..
..


{|
[[Image:SW_149.png]] ..................... [[Image:SW_148.png]]
|-
! jono- || g-o-r-e || solbe || moze || jene-n
|-
| John-{{small|ERG}} ||  "do"-{{small|3SG-IND-PST}} || drink.{{small|INF}} || water || Jane-{{small|DAT}}
|}


..
..


Note that the causee gets the dative affix. Also note that '''maŋga''' immediately follows '''gàu''', the '''maŋga''' object immediately follows '''maŋga'''. The causee can come anywhere but the string  '''gore solbe moze''' can not be broken. There are 3 possible places where '''jenen''' can appear.
'''mutu/umutu''' "important/unimportant" patterns with such antonym pairs as big/small ( '''jutu/tiji''' ) in that the two pole values together do not fill up the entire semantic space.


And another example ...
..
 
Sometimes you have a choice, as to which negative to use. As in English, where "I don't have a house" can also be exressed as "I have no house". in '''béu''' you can say '''bù byár tìa''' or '''byár jù tìa'''. For both languages the latter form comes across as being more vivid, carries greater emotion [I am not 100% sure why this should be so].


'''jonos gore náu onyo waulon jenen''' = John made Jane give the bone to a dog (earlier today)
..


Notice that we have two datives in this construction. The string '''gore náu onyo waulon''' can not be broken.
== ..... Six useful verbs==


..
..


This construction implies that the causer was present when the event happened. We call it a "direct" causative construction.
Six verbs of a kind


There is another causative construction which doesn't imply the causer was present when the event took place. In fact it implies that the causer took some action which at a <u>later</u> time made the causee do what they did. The two actions very probably being linked by some sort societal connection (via other people).
..


(b) '''jonos gore gò jenes solbore moze''' = John had Jane drink water  
{| border=1
  ||  '''bala'''
  || to open
  ||  '''kala'''
  || to shut/close
  |-
  ||  '''bana'''
  || to let go, to release, to free ...
  ||  '''kana'''
  || to connect, to make fast, to join
  |-
  || '''baza'''
  || to empty
  || '''kaza'''
  || to fill
  |}


The clause after '''gò''' ( i.e. '''jenes solbore moze''' )  has free word order. 
..


The indirect causative construction is iconic ...  separating the two verbs with '''gò''' reflects the separation of the two events ... both timewise and otherwise (i.e. there could have been a chain of protagonists involved).
And we have six common adjectives derived from the above ...


..
..


There are 4 other causitive constructions in '''béu''' ... '''gàu''' is neutral as to how the causee views the action they are made to do.
{| border=1
  ||  '''balya'''
  || open
  ||  '''kalya'''
  || shut/closed
  |-
  ||  '''banya'''
  || free, seperate
  ||  '''kanya'''
  || connected, joined
  |-
  ||  '''baʒya'''
  || empty
  ||  '''kaʒya'''
  || full
  |}


If the causee is reluctant ... we use '''tumai''' "to squeeze" or "to press" instead of '''gáu'''.
..


If the causee is eager ... we use '''náu''' "to give" instead of '''gáu'''. For example ...
{| border=1
  ||  '''balo'''
  || an key
  ||  '''kalo'''
  || a (window)shutter/valve
  |-
  ||  '''bano'''
  || padding
  ||  '''kano'''
  || link/connector
  |-
  ||  '''bazo'''
  || a void/vucuum
  ||  '''bano'''
  ||  fill
  |}


..


(c) '''jonos tumore solbe moze jenen''' =  "John made Jane drink water" or  "John forced Jane to drink water (earlier today)"


(d) '''jonos tumori gò jenes solbore moze''' = "John had Jane drink water" or   "John arranged that Jane had to drink the water" ... (the drinking occurred earlier today, the causing of the drinking ... yesterday or before)
The '''o''' suffix implies something solid. "connection", "association" or "relationship" would be covered by the '''manga''' ... '''kana'''.


(e) '''jonos nore solbe moze jenen''' = "John let Jane drink the water (earlier today)"
'''bazda''' = desert ?? : '''kazda''' = ocean " '''kanda''' = an intersection ?? : '''balda''' = a gap/opening


(f) '''jonos nori gò jenes solbore moze''' =  "John allowed Jane to drink water" or "John arranged for Jane to be able to drink water" ... (the drinking occurred earlier today, the arranging of the drinking ... yesterday or before)
'''bano''' originally padding to separate a warriors leather armour from his tunic.


..
..


Notice that in (a), (c) and (e) the '''maŋga''' must occur immediately after  '''gàu''',  '''tumai''' or '''náu'''. This is the same as the French, Italian or Spanish causative constuctions. Here is a French example ...
== ..... Valency==


..
..


:{|
In every language a particular verb can be associated with a number of nouns (we usually called these nouns arguments of the verb). For example ....
 
{|  
|-
|-
| je || ferai || manger || les || gâteaux || à || Jean
! jono-s || jene-h || slaigau || haun-o-r-a || eŋglaba-tu
|-
|-
| 1sgA || make+{{sc|fut}}+1sg || eat+{{sc|inf}} || the || cakes || {{sc|prep}} || Jean
| John-{{small|ERG}} || Jane-{{small|DAT}}|| calculus || teach-{{small|3SG-IND-PRES}} || English-{{small|INST}}
|} ==> I will make Jean eat the cakes
|} ==> John is teaching calculus to Jane in English


..
In the above example "teach" is associated with 4 nouns.


(a), (c) and (e) have what is called a compound causative verb. (i.e. one clause) ... (b), (d) and (f) are what are called periphrastic causative constructions. (i.e. two clauses)
Now things can get a bit confusing here. Some people hold that it is easy to distinguish between "core arguments" which are essential and "peripheral arguments" which simply add more information. But this is questionable. The consensus w.r.t. English seems to be that if an argument requires a preposition, then it is a "peripheral arguments", if no preposition required then it is a "core argument". A simple to implement system at the least.


..
In the above example "English" can be dismissed as a peripheral argument because of "using". But what about "Jane". In the above example Jane's roll in the clause is defined by the prefix "to". But what if "John is teaching calculus to Jane in English" is re-arranged as "John is teaching Jane calculus in English"?  Here you have three nouns not qualified by a prefix. In English "teach" is sometimes called a ditransitive verb (a verb that can take three essential (unmarked) arguments).  


It is possible for the indirect paraphrastic construction to give the embedded clause an impersonal form. For example ...
In '''beu''' no verbs are considered ditransitive ... Jane will always be marked by the dative suffix.  Now you might argue that every instance of teaching involves "somebody getting taught" ... well this is true, but it is also true that every instance of teaching involves some language being used. At the end of the day ... the English verb "teach" means ''exactly'' the same as its '''béu''' equivalent ( '''haun''' ). It is just that there are two different conventions for expounding an action (verb) in two different linguistic traditions. The '''béu''' linguistic tradition is the simplest :-)
 
The '''béu''' linguistic tradition divides all verbs in into two types .... H (transitive) and Ø (intransitive). In dictionaries all verbs are marked by the simbol H or Ø. H means a transitive verb ( called a "dash verb" ) and Ø means an intransitive verb ( called a "stroke verb" ). The rule is ...


'''jonos gori gò solb-re moze''' =  "John had the water drunk" or "John arranged for someone to drink the water"  ................. [notice : no causee]
..
..


[[Image:TW_652.png]]
A verb is H  if it is ever associated with a noun that has the ergative marker  "-'''s'''".
 
A verb is Ø if it is never associated with a noun that has the ergative marker  "-'''s'''".


..
..


In the above table, it can be seen that there are 6 causative constructions. There are 3 degrees of "volition" (the willingness of the causee) and 2 degrees of "directness" (did the causer act directly on the causee or through intermidiaries).
Now I will introduce the S A O convention which was devised by RMW Dixon. This convention is a useful way to refer to the arguments of transitive and intransitive verbs. The one argument of the intransitive verb is called the S argument. The argument of the transitive verb in which the success of the action most depends is referred to as the A argument. The argument of of the transitive verb is most affected by the action is called the O argument.
 
O was probably chosen from "object", A from "agent" and S from "subject" ( I find this useful to keep in mind as a memory aid). However O does not "mean" object and A does not mean agent and S does not mean subject. I (and many other linguists) use the word subject to refer to either A or S. Easier to talk about "subject" that to talk about "A or S" all the time.
 
[ In the '''béu''' linguistic tradition, the A argument is "the '''sadu''' noun", the O argument is the "the dash noun" and the S argument is the "the stroke noun".]


..
..


It is possibly to chain causative constructions together. For example ...
Now in English certain verbs appear to be Ø in some situations and H in others. These are called ambitransitive verbs.


..
..


'''jonos flòn jodoi''' = John feeds the animals.
1) The old woman knitted a sweater


'''g-r gò jonos flòn jodoi''' = John is made to feed the animals.
2) The old woman knitted


'''(nús) gùr gò jonos flòn jodoi''' = they make John feed the animals.
"knit" is regarded as a  "A=S ambitransitive". In (1) "old woman" is A ... in (2) "old woman" is S ... [ (2) is partially the reality described by (1) ]


'''gàu gò (nús) gùr gò jonos flòn jodoi''' = make them make John feed the animals.
..


'''by-r gàu gò (nús) gùr gò jonos flòn jodoi''' = it is necessary to make them make John feed the animals.
3) The old woman opened the door


'''(gís) byír gàu gò (nús) gùr gò jonos flòn jodoi''' = you must make them make John feed the animals.
4) The door opened
 
"open" is regarded as a  "O=S ambitransitive". In (3) "the door" is O ... in (2) "the door" is S ... [ (4) is not inconsistant'''*''' to being partially the reality described by (3) ]


..
..


And 2 of these 3 causative verbs can be given impersonal forms ....
In '''béu''', there are no "ambitransitives. "knit" is considered H but with the O argument being dropped when it is unimportant or unknown. Similarly "open" is also considered H but with the A argument dropped'''**''' when it is unimportant or unknown.
 
'''bala''' "to open" is always H in '''béu'''. In  English, "open" is sometimes transitive and sometimes intransitive.
 
Take '''pintu baləri***''' "the door opened". In English the proper analysis is "door" = "S argument". Well it is subject because it comes before the verb, and as it is the only argument it must be S.  
 
In '''béu''' the proper analysis is "door" = "O argument". We know '''bala''' "to open" is H becuse on occasion it can occur with A arguments. However in this case the only noun ('''pintu''') is not marked for the ergative hence it must be the "O argument".


'''jenen g-ryə doika''' or '''g-ryə doika jenen''' = "Jane has been made to walk" or "somebody has make Jane walk
'''pintu baləri''' could also be translated as "the door was opened".


'''jenen tum-ryə doika''' or '''tum-ryə doika jenen''' = "Jane has been forced to walk" or "somebody has forced Jane to walk
..


Now '''náu''' "to give" is a strange word in that it never takes an impersonal form (see the section above). Instead the word '''mài''' "to receive/get" is used.
'''*'''(4) leave open the question whether human action brought about the action or it was due to some other cause. This question could be answered by rewriting (4) as either "The door was opened" or "The door opened by itself".


'''jene moryə doika''' "Jane has been allowed to walk" ... [ as opposed to '''*jenen n-ryə doika'''  ]
'''**'''Actually it would be possble to drop A arguments in English if the imperative was not the base verb. For example in English "knit a jersey" is a command ... but if English ... say ... suffixed "ugu" for the imperative, then the command would be "knitugu a jersey". That would allow "knit a jersey" to be interpreted as "jersey being knitted".


We will learn more about '''mài''' Ch 4.6 and Ch 4.7.  
'''***'''We haven't come across the schwa before the "r" before. This will be explained very soon.


..
..


Another verb that we can mention here is '''penau''' meaning "to persuade, coax, convince, bring around, influence, sway"
So in '''béu''' …. each verb is either  H  or  Ø … no ambitransitives or ditransitives.
Also “the passive” is not talked about … rather it is just considered a particular case of “dropping”. And actually “dropping” is not considered a bit deal … just an very obvious thing to do.
 
..


'''penarua jene jonowo''' = "I intend to persuade Jane about John" = "I intend to bring Jane around to my way of thinking with respect to John"
Now one problem with dropping arguments is that the subject (S or A) must be represented in slot "1" of the indicative verb. How should we know what to put in here ( see Ch3.1.2.1 ). One solution could be to use the 3 person plural suffix -'''u'''- ... chances are that it is a 3rd person agent and the plural is more generic than the singular. This is what Russian does to make a sort of a passive. Another solution would be to use a vowel not already appropriated for pronoun agreement. This is what '''béu''' does. The schwa is inserted in the slot just before the "r".


'''(pás) penare jono jò nambon''' = "I got John to go home" =  "I persuaded John to go home" .... [Note ... the '''maŋga''' does not immediately follow for '''penau''' ]
Everything collapses in ... to the schwa ... an impersonal schwa.


'''(pás) penare jono gò baba yor jò nambon''' = "I persuaded John that father should go home"
..


Also '''penau''' says nothing about the success of the action ... unlike the 3 other verbs we have considered where success is assumed.
[[Image:TW_664.png]]
"the door opened" = "the door was opened"  = '''pintu baləri''' (Actually I do not think the schwa symbol is visually distinct enough ... from now on I will use a cross)  =>  '''pintu bal+ri'''


..
..


== ..... Two quotative verbs==
Here are some examples of this construction [ I will call it the impersonal construction from now on ]


..
'''beuba bl+r dían''' = "The language of '''béu''' is spoken here"


'''béu''' has two quotative verbs ... '''swé''' and '''aika'''.  What I mean by the term "quotative verb"is a verb which must'''*''' be accompanied by a <u>s</u>tring <u>o</u>f <u>d</u>irect <u>s</u>peach ["sods" from now on]
'''pí gaudoheu dè_blanyo g+r''' = "In this factory telephones are made"


'''swé''' = "say" and '''aika''' = ask .... ( that is to ask for information, to request something (to ask for) has a completely different root ... namely '''tama''' )
'''toilia bù ost+r pí duka dí''' = "Books are not sold in this shop"


I guess it is intransitive because the speaker never takes the ergative ending "'''s'''". The spoken to (if mentioned) takes the dative ending "'''n'''".
'''pintu by+r bala''' = '''pintu r balwa''' = the door has to be opened


[Some people would like to argue as to whether "sods" = an object or whether "sods" = a complement clause. I think this is not worth arguing about. It is similar to arguing about how many angels can stand on the end of a needle. ]
'''pintu mb+r bala''' = the door can be opened ........... [ to understand this example and the one above it ... see Ch 4.7 ]


There is an ordering restrictions for a clause formed around a quotative verb ... the "sods" must appear adjacent to '''swé''' or '''aika'''. It doesn't matter which comes first but they must be adjacent ... normally both elements are pronounced in the same intonation contour. A second restriction is that there must be a pause at the other end of the "sods" ... the opposite end from the quotative verb. For example ...
'''hala dè nyal+ryə''' = that rock is eroded .......... '''nyale''' = to erode, to wear


John said "Ai ... go away" => '''jono swori aiʔdo ... ojo''' where  '''aiʔdo''' is an interjection expressing frustration and '''ojo''' is quite a rough way to say "go away".
..


This can also be expressed as '''aiʔdo ... ojo swori jono''' or '''jono ... aiʔdo ... ojo swori''' or even '''swori aiʔdo ... ojo ... jono'''. The first two patterns are the most common followed by the third pattern and the fourth a distant last. Notice that the "sods" that I chose for demonstration purposes entails an internal pause.
Note ... the schwa can not support any tone. And as it is only used in the grammer and not in any base words as such it was not introduced in Chapter 1 (as '''r''' was not). The schwa is represented in fact by a cross in the '''béu''' writing system ...


If we introduced a dative element ...  
..


John said to Jane "Ai ... go away" => '''jono jenen swori aiʔdo ... ojo'''
[[Image:TW_909.png]]


The above would be the most common ordering of constituents ... but again quite a bit of freedom with respect to word ordering.  
Note ... Some '''béu''' speakers  pronounce "schwa" + "syllable final rhotic" as  "ø" or "ør". These people also tend to give "ø" the proper tone. However the majority pronoun a schwa followed by a rhotic appoximant with neutral tone.


The "sods" can be quite lengthy ... 2 or 3 or 4 clauses and follows as near as possible the speach pattern of the original speaker.
..
 
Now "door" is a man-made object and probably it exists in a place with many people around. So it is reasonable to expect there to be ''human volition'' involved when it opens. But what about when we get out into nature. When we see a river freezing. There is no agent to be seen behind this "freezing" ... it just happens. For this reason the verb "to freeze" '''doska''' is Ø.
 
But now we have become clever ... we hold dominion over nature. Hence we need to derive a word for freeze that is H. And that deriration is arrived at by appending -'''n'''.  


The '''béu''' orthography is a bit quirky when it comes to quotative verbs. In CH 1.8 we briefly mentioned the '''deupa'''. These are actually used to bracket any "sods". Also it is common to drop the actual quotative verb. (well after the time setting of the speach act(s) are revealed anyway). For example ...
Hence ...


[[Image:TW_746.png]]
'''doska''' = to freeze


The first one is graphically '''jono''' [ '''aiʔdo ... ojo''' ] ... (for an explanation of the graffic form of the interjection  '''aiʔdo''', look back to CH 1.2)
'''moze doskori''' = the water froze


The second one is graphically '''jono''' [ '''bàu nái''' ]
'''moze doskanaru''' = I will freeze the water


These would be read as '''jono swori aiʔdo ... ojo''' and '''jono aikori bàu nái''' (John asked "which man")
..


But how do we know that '''swé''' should be associated with one and '''aika''' to the other ? Simple ... if you have a question word within the '''deupa''' then you know you should pronounce '''aika''' ... if not you pronounce '''swé'''. We have encountered these question words already in CH 2.10. There are ten of them but the first two have two forms. Here they are again ...
Actually any Ø can take this suffix and become H. Here are a few more examples ...


..
..


{| border=1
{| border=1
   |align=center| '''nén nós'''
   || '''ngeu'''
   |align=center| what
   || to fly
  ||  '''ngeun''' 
  || to throw
   |-
   |-
   |align=center| '''mín mís'''
   || ''''''
   |align=center| who
   || to go
  ||  '''jón'''
  || to send
   |-
   |-
   |align=center|  '''láu'''
   ||  ''''''
   |align=center| "how much/many"
   || to come
  ||  '''tèn'''
  || to summon
   |-
   |-
   |align=center| '''kái'''
   || '''bái'''
   |align=center| "what kind of"
   || to rise
  ||  '''báin'''
  || to raise
   |-
   |-
   |align=center| ''''''
   || '''kàu'''
   |align=center| where
   || to descend
  ||  '''kàun'''
  || to lower
   |-
   |-
   |align=center| '''kyú'''
   || '''dàu'''
   |align=center| when
   || to die
  ||  '''dàun'''
  || to kill
   |-
   |-
   |align=center| '''sái'''
   || '''slài'''
   |align=center| why
   || to change
  ||  '''slàin'''
  || to change
   |-
   |-
   |align=center|  '''nái'''
   ||  '''diadia'''
   |align=center| which
   || to happen
   |-
   ||  '''diadian'''
  |align=center|  '''ʔai?'''
   || to cause
  |align=center| "solicits a yes/no response"
  |-
  |align=center|  '''ʔala'''
   |align=center| which of two
   |}
   |}


..
..


The only time that you hear these ten words and you are NOT being asked a question is when these words are in the same intonation contour as the verb "aika" in one of its forms.
And here are a few more examples ....


The only time that you see these ten words and you are NOT being asked a question is when these words are sandwiched between two '''deumai'''.


This is quite a bit different from English where question words have been appropriated to function as relativizers, complementizers and what have you (heads of free relative clauses).
{| border=1
 
  |align=center| '''ʔoime'''
In the above ... when pronouncing words ... '''swé''' or '''aika''' is inserted where the first bracket appears. It could equally well be that '''swé''' or '''aika''' is inserted where the second bracket appears. It is deemed to not really matter that much. However in carefull writting the proper position of the quotative verb can be indicated. For example ...
  |align=center| to be happy, happyness
 
  |align=center| '''ʔoimor'''
[[Image:TW_747.png]]
  |align=center| he is happy
 
  |align=center| '''ʔoimen'''
In the above a pause (gap) is visible just above the top '''deupa'''. From that it is logical to deduce that '''swé''' or '''aika''' should be inserted after the "sods". (from the word order and intonation rules given earlier). But most of the time ... when reading out loud ... people do not take much heed to whether the quotative verb is placed over the '''deupa damau''' or the '''deupa dagoi'''.
  |align=center| to make happy
 
  |align=center| '''ʔoimin'''
In a textblock, which you have a lot of dialogue it is common to colour code the "sods" with respect to the speaker. For example ...
  |align=center| pleasant
 
  |-
 
  |align=center| '''heuno'''
[[Image:TW_278.png]] Shown in better detail   => [[Image:TW_750.png]]
  |align=center| to be sad/sadness
 
  |align=center| '''heunor'''
 
  |align=center| she's sad
When this happens the '''deupa''' has no gold filling. It could be possible to drop the speakers name also once the colour coding scheme is established. This really depends upon how much dialogue is involved. Maybe each speaker would be mentioned again at the start of every textblock ... just to keep the protagonist <=> colour mapping alive in the readers mind.
  |align=center| '''heunon'''
  |align=center| to make sad
  |align=center| '''heunin'''
  |align=center| depressing
  |-
  |align=center| '''taude'''
  |align=center| to be annoyed
  |align=center| '''taudor'''
  |align=center| he is annoyed
  |align=center| '''tauden'''
  |align=center| to annoy
  |align=center| '''taudin'''
  |align=center| annoying
  |-
  |align=center| '''swú'''
  |align=center| to be scared, fear
  |align=center| '''swor'''
  |align=center| she is afraid
  |align=center| '''swún'''
  |align=center| to scare
  |align=center| '''swu.in'''
  |align=center| frightening, scary
  |-
  |align=center| '''centa'''
  |align=center| to be angry, anger
  |align=center| '''centor'''
  |align=center| he is angry
  |align=center| '''centan'''
  |align=center| to make angry
  |align=center| '''centin'''
  |align=center| really annoying
  |-
  |align=center| '''yode'''
  |align=center| to be horny, lust
  |align=center| '''yodor'''
  |align=center| she is horny
  |align=center| '''yoden'''
  |align=center| to make horny
  |align=center| '''yodin'''
  |align=center| sexy, hot
  |-
  |align=center| '''gái'''
  |align=center| to ache, pain
  |align=center| '''gayor'''
  |align=center| he hurts
  |align=center| '''gáin'''
  |align=center| to hurt (something)
  |align=center| '''gai.iin'''
  |align=center| painful
  |-
  |align=center| '''gwibe'''
  |align=center| to be ashamed/shame/shyness
  |align=center| '''gwibor'''
  |align=center| she is ashamed/shy
  |align=center| '''gwiben'''
  |align=center| to embarrass
  |align=center| '''gwibin'''
  |align=center| embarrassing
  |-
  |align=center| '''doimoi'''
  |align=center| to be anxious, anxiety
  |align=center| '''doimor'''
  |align=center| he is anxious
  |align=center| '''doimoin'''
  |align=center| to cause anxiety, to make anxious
  |align=center| '''doimin'''
  |align=center| worrying
  |-
  |align=center| '''ʔica'''
   |align=center| to be jealous, jealousy
  |align=center| '''ʔicor'''
  |align=center| she is jealous
  |align=center| '''ʔican'''
  |align=center| to make jealous
  |align=center| '''ʔicin'''
  |align=center| causing jealousy
  |}


..
..


'''*''' In the very first sentence of this section I said that "quotative verb"is a verb which must be accompanied by a "sods" ... not quite true. The determiners '''dí''' and '''dè''' can take the place of a "sods". In these constructions '''dí''' refers to a "sods" that will be revealed imminently ... '''dè''' refers to a "sods" that was spoken in the past.
'''jài ?oime''' is an adjective meaning happy by nature.


If Jane pronounces an opinion about something ... if John had pronounced roughly similar in the past ... it would be fitting to say '''jono swori dè'''.


If you are about to replay some utterance by John on a voice file, it would be appropriate to say '''jono swori dí''' just before playing the voice file.
Six H can also take -'''n''' as well.  They are ...


..
..


IMPORTANT ... The only time you hear direct speech is when '''swé''' or '''aika''' is present in one of its forms.
{| border=1
 
  ||  '''flò'''
..
  || to eat
  ||  '''flòn'''
  || to feed, feeding
  |-
  ||  '''heca'''
  || to see
  ||  '''hecan'''
  || to show, showing
  |-
  ||  '''háu'''
  || to learn
  ||  '''háun'''
  || to teach, tuition
  |-
  || '''nko'''
  || to know
  || '''nkon'''
  || to inform, informing
  |-
  || '''pòi'''
  || to enter, to join
  || '''pòin'''
  || to put in, insertion
  |-
  || '''féu'''
  || to exit, to leave
  || '''féun'''
  || to take out, extraction
  |}


== ..... Speech verbs==
..


..
In English, all the above except the last would be considered ditransitive verbs. "to take out" would not be considered ditransitive because one argument would be marked by the preposition "from". In '''béu''' they are all still H although they have undoubtedly one extra noun compared to their non-derived counter parts. Remember H and Ø were defined as ...
 
A verb is H  if it is  ever associated with a noun that has the ergative marker  "-'''s'''".


We have already briefly touched on serial verb chains. These are in fact the i-form of the verb.  
A verb is Ø if it is never associated with a noun that has the ergative marker  "-'''s'''".


The i-form of '''swé''' is often used to give strings of direct speech in conjuction with another verb. Usually this other verb denoted some time of speech event. There are around 50 speech-verbs in '''béu''' ... 
(Note : '''fyá''' "to tell" means basically the same as '''nkon''' but is less formal. Also '''gàu''' means basically the same as '''diadian''' but is less formal. )
'''melita''' "to agree" : '''noluja''' "to disagree" : '''malapa''' "to equivocate" : : '''oldo''' "to order/command" : '''endo''' "to introduce/recommend" : '''enji''' "to suggest" : '''ʔuaho''' "to exclaim" : '''ʔaume''' "to scream" : '''uhozo''' "to exhort/urge" : '''dauŋgo''' "to repeat/relay" : '''diŋkli''' "to discuss" : '''dawata''' "to haver" : '''daumpa''' : "to scold/berate" ... etc. etc.


..
..


'''uhozo''' is an S-verb meaning "to urge", "to exhort".
We have discussed '''bala''' and '''doska''' so far. The first is considered basically H and the second one basically Ø. There is a third type of verb ... for this type it is hard to say if it is more basic as Ø or more basic as H. So these verbs have <u>two</u> basic forms. For example ...
 
..


So you could use '''uhozo''' as the main verb in the clause and then can add the sods marked with '''swə''' ...
'''cwamo hulkori''' = the bridge broke


'''uhozora jenes jono ... gì r boimos swə''' => Jane is cheering on John shouting "you are the best"
'''deutais cwamo helkuri''' = the soldiers broke the bridge


..
..


{|
Actually for the first example .. the chances are that the breakage was due to wear and tear caused by human activity. But the important thing is that it is non-volitional. Also there might have been no humans around when the bridge actually did break. So we can talk about the bridge breaking by itself ... as if by an act of nature. And another example ...
|-
! uhoz-o-r-a || jene-s || jono || gì || r || boimos || swə
|-
|  "exhort"-{{small|3SG-IND-PRS}} || Jane-{{small|ERG}} || John || you || COP || the best || saying
|-
|}


..
..


'''dauŋgo''' is an S-verb meaning "to repeat, to relay"
'''jono wiltore''' = John woke up (earlier today)


'''tomos dauŋgore jene swə gì r boimos''' = Thomas repeated Jane saying "you are the best"
'''jenes jone woltore ''' = Jane woke up John  (earlier today)


..
..


The i-form of '''aika''' is also used fo give "sods". For example ...
There are about 40 of these pairs. If the Ø has '''u''' the H will have '''e''' ...  if the  Ø has '''i''' the H will have '''o'''.
 
So lets summarize these three typre of verb ...


'''daulau''' is an NS-verb meaning "to joke".
..


'''daulori jene jonotu aiki''' ''bla bla bla''  => Jane joked with John asking ''bla bla bla'' (well to make this a good example I would have to invent a quite involved scenario, but I hope you get the idea ... even with the ellipses)
[[Image:TW_825.png]]


..
..


The use of '''alki''' and '''swə''' in conjunction with one of the speech act verbs are an important structure in '''béu''' grammar and adds to the beauty and functionality of the language.
----


This structure is really only applicable to speech act verbs. If it was used with a non-speech-act verb it would sound a bit strange to the ear of a '''béu''' speaker. For example ...
So to wrap it all up about verbs and arguments ...


?'''jono doikori dunheun swə falaja r NICE sowe''' = John walked to the civic centre saying "what a beautiful morning"
No verbs are ambitrasitive. They are either Ø or H.  However it is easy to drop the A or the O argument from a H clause if either of them is considered trivial or is unknown.


..
Now in '''béu''' any H can be given a Ø meaning ( grammatically the structure is still H ) by making the the O argument '''tái''' ... meaning himself, herself, yourself etc. etc. However only animate A arguments do this. Hence ...


'''béu''' maintains a dichotomy between speech-verbs and thought-verbs. Speech-verbs can take a '''swə/aiki''' adjunct whereas thought-verbs can take a complement clause introduced by the particle '''gò''' in place of an O argument. We will go into this more in the next section.
'''bàus tái timpori''' = the man hit himself  ................. acceptable


This dichotomy is not total though. There is some merging between the two constructions.
'''*pintus tái balori''' = the door opened itself ...... unacceptable


For example ... '''ʕelo'''  "to hear" may be considered unique w.r.t. the constructions it can appear in ...
In English there are two ways to report on a door opening without mentioning any agent ... "the door opened" and "the door was opened"


'''ʕelari jwadoi''' = I heard some big birds
In '''béu''' only one ... '''pintu bal+ri''' ... which is just a H clause with the A argument dropped. Comparable to how "the old woman knitted"(as this would appear in '''béu''' of course) is a H clause with the O argument dropped.


'''ʕelari jono''' = I heard John
..


'''ʕelari jono swé bù ʔár jò''' = I heard John say "I don't want to go"   ....... OK I suppose we analyze '''swé bù ʔár jò''' as an adjuct similar to '''swə bù ʔár jò''' in '''jono nolujori swə bù ʔár jò''' "John disagreed saying "I don't want to go"
In '''béu''' you can make a "passive participle" by suffixing -'''ia'''.


'''ʕelari gò jono bù jorua''' I heard that the jono doesn't intend to go
If you come across something broken and you know it was broken by human volition ... you would call it '''helkia'''.


'''ʕelari swər bù jarua''' = I heard it said "I will not go" .............. And we can analyze this as an transitive verb where the object has been dropped.
If you come across something broken and you did not know how it was broken ... you would call it '''hulkia'''.


Also '''glùn''' "to inform, to tell" is both a thought-verb and a speech-verb. The informer is in the ergative, the informed the dative. The object can be an object (i.e. the news) or a complement clause (i.e. '''gò jono bù jorua''') or it can simply be missing (when we use '''glùn''' as a speech-verb) ... or should we consider that when it is used as a speech-verb that there is an object ... something generic like "the news" but that it can be dropped. Well neither answer is right in itself.  
If you come across something frozen you would call it '''doskia'''. There is no such word as '''*doskania'''.


..
..


== ..... Thought verbs==
In '''béu''' you can make the "general obligation participle" by suffixing -'''ua'''.


..
If you come across something that has to be broken ... you could refer to it as '''helkua'''.


Everybody carries an (imperfect) model of his environment in his mind and uses this model to plan his actions. The main reason HUMANS have been so successful compared to other animals is that we have a more complete model than ... say ... our primate cousins. The reason that out model is good is that we have LANGUAGE and so get information from our fellows. Probably the building of this MODEL and LANGUAGE were co-developements and could well be reflected in the size of the HUMAN BRAIN over the last few million years. I believe that this MODEL and LANGUAGE are intertwined and hence I don't think it is a good idea to consider either in isolation.
If you come across something that had to be frozen ... you could refer to it as '''doskanua'''.  


Now usually when we communicate ... we just talk about reality. For example ... JOHN IS TALL. We do not acknowledge the actual more complicated situation ... IN MY WORLD MODEL, JOHN IS TALL.
There is no such words as '''*doskua''' or '''*hulkua'''


But sometimes we do .... usually when we are talking about activities related to our mind ... like "thinking", "knowing" ... disseminating knowledge to our fellows "telling", "saying" ... gathering knowledge first hand "seeing", "hearing" ... trying to gather knowlege from our fellows "asking". All these bracketed verbs can take what are called complement clauses. When you see a complement clause you are seeing an admission that what we are talking about is not in fact REALITY, but some MODEL of REALITY. Maybe you could say that it is an admission that we are using META-DATA rather than DATA.
..


The 4 panels below might illuminate what I am trying to say. What is on the white background is REALITY. What is on a black background is part of a MIND MODEL . The script on an orange background is a speach act appropriate for the situation shown. The top panel is the way that we normally speak ... that is REALITY is presented directly with no referrence to any MIND MODEL.
The above method of presenting a verb like '''bala''' hints at human volition. To get rid of this connotation (to suggest that the event happened naturely) we must use '''tezau''' "to become" plus an adjective. This is demonstrated below ...


----
Consider '''geuko''' = "to turn something green" ... H ... derived from '''gèu''' "green"


[[Image:TW_744.png]]


----
1) '''báu tezori gèu''' = The man became green .. ........................ ''natural''


[[Image:TW_738.png]]
2) '''báu  geuk+ri''' = The man was made green .................... ''human volition''


----
3) '''báus tái geukori''' = The man made himself green ......... ''human volition''


[[Image:TW_739.png]]
..


----
Now consider '''bala''' = "to open" ... H


[[Image:TW_742.png]] ===> [[Image:TW_743.png]]


----
1) '''pintu tezori balya''' = the door became opened = the door opened .......... ''natural'' ................ [ here the agent could be anything ... the wind ... or even some fairy '''cái'''  ... use your imagination ]


Now it seems that the majority of languages have at least one way of bracketing off the META-DATA from DATA. English has two types of complement clause (CC from now on) ... one introduced by the complementizer "that" and the other introduced by a question word. These usually take the place usually taken by an O argument. '''béu''' has one CC which is introduced by the particle '''gò'''. Some of the thought-verbs that can take either a CC or an O argument are listed below ...
2) '''pintu bal+ri''' = the door was opened ............................................... ''human volition'' .... [ this one implies that the agent was human but is either unknown or unimportant and the action  deliberate ]


'''petika''' "to select/choose/pick/decide" : '''glù''' "to know" : '''wè''' "to be thinking about/consider/ponder" : '''celba''' "to remember" : '''dolka''' "to forget" : '''wespila''' "to understand" : '''glùn''' "to inform/tell" : '''celban''' "to remind" ... etc. etc.
Note ... there is no (3) here as a door is non-human.


'''béu''' does not have indirect speech as English has ... i.e. John said (that) that was stupid. In '''béu''' this would have to be framed as direct speech ... i.e. "this is stupid" said John (notice the change of reference for time and argument). Also ... "John asked whether I wanted to go" would be recast as "John asked "you want to go ?" "
..


The '''béu''' CC is exclusively used for thought-verbs ( IS THERE AN EXCEPTION TO THIS ?? )
In either of the (1)'s '''wistia''' "deliberately/carefully" or '''wistua''' "accidently/carelessly" can be added after'''*'''  '''tezori'''.  This automatically makes Agent => Human


..
The same for the (2)'s, but the incidence of '''wistua''' should greatly excede the incidence of '''wistia''' as "intention" is the default for this construction.
 
With (3) the connotation of intent is so strong that '''wistia'''/ '''wistua''' could be considered a bit infelicitous ... not impossible but indicative of an unusual situation.


== ..... The reciprocal construction==
'''*''' or '''wistiwe''' or '''wistuwe''' if not immediately after the verb. [by the way ... '''wisto''' = "mind/brain" by the way]


..
..


The reciprocal particle is '''bèn'''
..


'''jonos jenes timpur bèn''' = "John and Jane are hitting each other" = "John and Jane hit one and other"
PUT ANOTHER WAY ...


Note ...  '''''' "and" is not used when two nouns in the ergative case occur adjacent to each other.
There are many actions that are kind of fluid as to the number of participants involved. When languages code an action they take into account whether the action is normally'''*''' involves a single paricipant or two participants [ three participants is also possible but that is another story ]. And then the relevant language will add extra stuff (an extra word … bit of word … something like that) when this action involves more or less participants than suggested by the basic word coding this action.


The particle also comes after adjectives occasionally. For example ...
Two examples from French.


'''jono lè jene r ʔài bèn''' = John and Jane are the same.
The action of boiling is deemed => single paricipant => bouillir
When two participants, we add the word faire => faire bouillir


No real reason why it should be added to the above sentence ... except that it is judged to sound good.
The action of breaking is deemed => double paricipant => casser
When only a single participant, we add the word se => se casser


'''ʔáu bèn''' "to take mutually" is the '''béu''' expression meaning ... do the dirty deed, have relations, roger, root, shag, boink, slam the clam, thump thighs, pass the gravy, wet the willy, make the beast with two backs ...  make love.
Certain languages deem certain actions pretty evenly split between single-participant manifestations and double-participant manifestations. In these cases, it can be impossible to determine what is the basic form of the verb.


..
An example from Swahili.


== ..... Five slots before the verb==
cham-k-a = to boil as the soup over the open fire boils
cham-sh-a = to boil as your mother boils the water for a cup of tea


..
Further examples, Japanese this time.


We have already covered the 5 slots for "agent", "tense/aspect", " '''r''', "evidentiality", "perfect" at the end of the denuded infinitive. As well as the nuances given by these suffixes, there are particles which add further information to the basic verb. These are called (near-standers ?). These particles occur in 5 pre-verbal slots and a maximum of one is allowed from each slot.


The complete verbal block is shown below  ...


[[Image:TW_756.png]]
{| border=1
 
  ||  生きる
Some restrictions on the co-occurence of these termsare given above. There are some additional restrictions not given above. For example '''juku''' is how you negate the perfect (dropping the '''yə'''). As '''yə''' can not co-occur with '''ʔès'''/'''ʔàn''' or '''jù''', '''juku''' also can not co-occur with '''ʔès'''/'''ʔàn''' or '''jù'''.
  || ikiru
  ||  to live
  |:
  ||  活かす
  || ikasu
  || to revive
  |-
  || 逃げる
  || nigeru
  || to escape
  |:
  || 逃がす
  || nigasu
  || to set free
  |-
  || 揺れる
  || yureru
  || to sway
  |:
  || 揺らす
  || yurasu
  || to shake
  |}


..


=== ... Slot 1===
Japanese has a many verbs pairs of this sort.


..
..


These two particles indicate probability.
'''*''' The choice can be culturally determined in some circumstances. Imagine a community in which each grown male visits the barber to get shaved every morning versus a community in which shaving is a private affair. The language of the former will inevitably pattern "shave" as transitive, anf the latter will inevitably pattern "shave" as intransitive.
 
'''màs''' = possibly
 
'''lói''' = probably
 
The probability distribution for '''màs''' centres around 50 %.
 
The probability distribution for '''lói''' centres around 85 %.


..
..


=== ... Slot 2===
== ..... To undergo==


..
..


We have seen the subjectless verb form above where the vowel before the '''r''' becomes a schwa.`However there is another way to drop a subject ... by using the verb '''jwòi''' "to undergo" followed by the base form. Of these two ways of dropping the subject, the former is overwhelmingly preferred. However for forming present participles and infinitives, the second method is necessary.


'''''' is a negative particle which has scope over the entire sentence ... equivalent to "not" in English.
'''timp+ra pà''' = I am being hit : '''jwola timpa''' = being hit : '''jwòi timpa''' = to be hit


'''awa''' gives a "habitual but irregular" (maybe best translated as "now and again" or "occasionally" or even "not usually") meaning to the verbal block. Possibly related to the verb '''awata'''  which means "to wander".
[Note to self .... sort out the below ... and also all the RUBBISH PARTICIPLE stuff I have]


'''bolbo''' gives a "habitual and regular" (best translated as "normally" or "usually" or "regularly") meaning to the verbal block. Possibly related to the verb '''bolboi''' which means "to roll".
'''hecari jono katala lazde''' = I saw John cutting the grass ....................... '''katala lazde''' is a '''saidau kaza''' ..... '''katala''' is a '''saidau baga'''


'''juku''' is used for negating the perfect aspect. For example ...
'''hecari lazde jwola kata''' = I saw the grass being cut ............................. '''jwola kata''' is a '''saidau kaza'''


'''joryə*''' = he has gone : '''juku jora''' = he hasn't gone :...  '''juku jor''' = he never goes
'''hecari lazde jwola kata hí jono''' = I saw the grass being cut by John .... '''jwola kata hí jono''' is a '''saidau kaza'''  


'''joriyə''' = he had gone : '''juku jori''' = he hadn't gone
Note ... although the '''''' suffix is probably connected to the second '''pila?o''' it should be recognized as a separate siffix here. If it was the '''pila?o''' we would have ... '''bwari lazde là jwòi kata'''


'''joruyə''' = he will have gone : '''juku joru''' = he will not have gone
'''hecari lazde kataya''' = I saw the grass that has been cut


It is as if, for -'''i''',  -'''e''',  -'''a''' and  -'''u''', '''juku''' is saying that the action didn't happen from the beginning of time to the time specified by the tense. As -'''iyə''',  -'''eyə''',  -'''ryə''' and  -'''uyə''' are saying that the action happen at least once from the beginning of time to the time specified by the tense ... we can see how one is the negative of the other.
'''hecari lazde katawa''' = I saw grass that must be cut = I saw that the grass must be cut


'''toili dé juku kludara''' = "I haven't read that book" or "I have never read that book"
'''lazde katawa hecari''' = I saw the grass that must be cut


By the way ... this negative construction mirrors what is done in Mandarin ... 没 méi or 没有 méiyǒu is used instead of 不 bù and the aspect marker 了 le is  omitted.
'''hecari lazde nài r katawa'''


..
..


'''*''' ... '''joryə''' is actually semantically '''jorayə''' ... there is no perfect aspect for the habitual tense in '''béu'''
== ..... The copula==
 
Note that '''juku''' with the generic/habitual tense is translater by "never" in English :  '''juku jor''' = he never goes : juku pyár fanfai = horses never fly


..
..


OK ... but if you are only allowed one of these five, how would you translate .. "I don't usually come to these parent-teacher meetings but ...."
The three'''*''' components of a copular clause usually have a strict order'''***''' ... "copular subject" => "copula" => "copula complement". For example ...
 
Well you wont say ... '''awa tár''' to these parent-teacher '''nò twás _ ...."


..
..


=== ... Slot 3===
{|
|-
| "copular subject" ||align=center| "copula" || "copula complement"
|-
! align=center| jono ||align=center| r || koduʒi
|-
|align=center| John ||align=center| is ||align=center| diligent
|-
|align=center| - ||align=center| - ||align=center| -
|-
! align=center| jono ||align=center| r || moltai
|-
|align=center| John ||align=center| is ||align=center| doctor
|}
 
..
 
The copula's base form is '''sàu'''. You will see that it is listed among the 37 short verbs. However it patterns differently from the other 36. And indeed it patterns differently from all other verbs. Below are the '''r'''-forms of  '''sàu''' ...
 
..
 
[[Image:TW_969.png]]


..
..


These are called aspectual operators or aspectual particles.
The copula form rule ...  "When the copular subject noun (or noun phrase) is overtly stated, use the short form. At all other times, use the long form"


..
..


In English the nearest translations<sup>*</sup> are '''ʔàn''' = "still" and '''ʔès''' = "already". 
The short form is used when the copular subject noun (or noun phrase) is overtly stated. In other situations the full form is used. For example when the copular subject is a pronoun'''**''', the long form must be used.
 
You can see in the above chart that the short form of the aortist tense has two forms. '''''' is used in two situations ...
 
1) If the copula subject ends in a consonant.  For example ....
 
'''sòs rò hau?e''' = the snow is beautiful


Many many languages have equivalents to these two particles. For example ...
2) If an evidential is tagged on. For example ...
 
'''tìa ròn hau?e''' = the house is beautiful (I guess)


..
..


{|border=1
'''r''' by itself is used in all other situations.it is a clitic attached the the last vowel of the copula subject. However it is always written as a separate word. For example ....
|align=center| English
 
|align=center| still
'''tomo r tumu''' = Thomas is stupid
|align=center| already
 
|-
It takes the tone of the copula subject.
|align=center| German
 
|align=center| noch
..
|align=center| schon
 
|-
The aortist form is the form corresponding to "am", "are" ans is in English. The present tense is "marked" (i.e. the unusual case that carries extra eaning). For example ...
|align=center| '''béu'''
 
|align=center| '''ʔàn'''
..
|align=center| '''ʔès'''
 
|-
'''sòs rò hau?e''' = snow is beatiful ….. a timeless truth
|align=center| French
 
|align=center| encore
'''sòs rà hau?e''' = the snow is beatiful (for now) ... maybe the speakers are contemplating the snow melting and the consequent slush
|align=center| déjà
 
|-
..
|align=center| Mandarin
 
|align=center| hái
And another example ...
|align=center| yîjing
|-
|align=center| Dutch
|align=center| nog
|align=center| al
|-
|align=center| Russian
|align=center| eščë
|align=center| uže
|-
|align=center| Serbo-Croatian
|align=center| još
|align=center| već
|-
|align=center| Finnish
|align=center| vielä
|align=center| jo
|-
|align=center| Swedish
|align=center| än(nu)
|align=center| redan
|-
|align=center| Indonesian
|align=center| masih
|align=center| sudah
|}


..
..


'''ʔàn''' indicates ...
'''jono r bòi''' = John is good (it is his nature)


1) An activity is ongoing.
'''jono rà bòi''' = John is being good ... maybe to impress somebody who is visiting.


2) The activity must stop some time in the future, possibly quite soon.
Note ... to say '''jono rà bòi''' invalidates '''jono r bòi''' to a certain extent.


3) There is a certain expectation<sup>*</sup> that the activity should have stopped by now.
..


'''ʔès''' indicates ...
Because there is a strict word order, definiteness can not be expressed as it usually is with other verbs (S, O, A, dative ... left of verb if definite, right of verb if not). However the particles '''èn''' and '''ín''' can be drafted for this purpose.


1) An activity is ongoing.
[Note to self : should every '''pila?o''' defined argument act thus ... what about other arguments ? ]


2) The activity was not ongoing some time in the past, possibly quite recently.
It is only the '''r'''-form of the copula which is irregular. All other forms are perfectly normal. For example ...


3) There is a certain expectation<sup>*</sup> that the activity should not have started yet.
'''sauhu bòi''' = be good ................................................................. '''u'''-form


..
'''kodor sə kludado''' = he works as a clark .................................... '''i'''-form


<sup>*</sup> Inevitably a connotation of "contrary to expectation" will develope to a certain degree. This is because if the situation was according to expectation often nothing would need be utterred. Hence '''ʔàn'''  
'''kodi sòr kludado''' = he/she works as a clark …........................…  '''i'''-form .............. Actually, I think this way is better (change the rest of the website ?)
and '''ʔès''' are often found in contrary to expectation situation which in turn colours their meaning.


..
..


[[Image:TW_753.png]]
There is also the change of state copula, '''tezau'''. While '''tezau''' < '''té''' + '''sàu''', I would not call it a calque on English "become", rather the deep semantic process that formed "become" in English, worked also in '''béu'''.


A very interesting thing about the above two situations is how they are negated. Either the verb can be negated or the operator can be negated. (The verb is always under the scope of the operator so if you negate the operator you are also negating the verb). The first case I represent with a bar over the verb. The second I represent with a bar over the operator+verb.
There is strict word order with this copula as well ...


On the diagram ... If the verb is negated ... then the yellow place becomes white and the white space becomes yellow.
..


On the diagram ... If the operator+verb is negated ... the dashed line representing "now" changes places with the line that represents onset/cessation of activity.
{|
|-
| "copular subject" ||align=center| "copula" || "copula complement"
|-
! align=center| jono ||align=center| tezori || koduʒi
|-
|align=center| John ||align=center| became ||align=center| diligent
|-
|align=center| - ||align=center| - ||align=center| -
|-
! align=center| jono ||align=center| tezori || moltai
|-
|align=center| John ||align=center| became ||align=center| doctor
|}


..
..


[[Image:TW_354.png]]
As you can see there is no erosion here.
 
Notice that for the two copulas the copuls subjects are always unmarked ... that is they never take the ergative suffix.


..
..


As you see by above ... by changing whether the negator act on the operator+verb or whether only on the verb give diametrically opposite meanings.
How to negate a copular sentence ? Some examples ...
 
'''jono bù r jutu''' = john isn’t big


Note that there are 4 possible negative cases to choose from and a language only needs 2. A language (to cover all negative cases) should be either "(a) (b) type" or "(c) (d) type" or " (a) (c) type" or "(b) (d) type"
'''bù sòr jutu''' = he/she isn’t big


Cross linguistically there are interesting variations. All Slavic languages prefer verb negation, hence they are (c) (d) types.
'''òn bù sòr jutu''' = HE isn’t big (I am)


In German, only (a) and (c) are allowed in positive declarations.  
In the last example, it is not necessary to have the full copula form to show 3SG ... '''*òn bù r jutu''' ... would not be confusing. However we continue to abide by "the copula form rule"


Nahuatl has negation of the operator so is (a) (b) type.
..


It can be said that English also is basically (a) (b) type. However English has suppletion  ... "yet" for "already" : "anymore" or "longer" for "still" ... hence "not yet" and "no longer" (I guess this lessens the chance of mishearing).
'''*''' Well sometimes the copular subject is dropped so two components. It is dropped if the subject is "the world"/"the environment". Under the section "Valancy" we introduced the impersonal form of the verb ... normally used when the subject is "unknown"/"trivial". The copula also has an impersonal form. However now the reason is not because the subject is trivial : rather the opposite, the subject is all encompassing.


In '''béu''', '''bù''' negates the whole clause so you can say that '''béu''' is basically (a) (b) type.  
Note ... Other languages use "world" or "environment" as the subject in similar situations, English used "it".


..
As with English, this construction is often used for the weather ...


For '''ʔès''' ...
'''fona''' = rain : '''fonia''' = rainy/raining : '''fonua''' = dry (well not raining). So ...


{|
'''s+ra fonia''' = it's raining
|-
! ʔès || kod-a-r-a || dían
|-
| already || work-{{small|1SG-IND-PRES}}  || here
|} ==> I already work here


{|
'''tez+ra fonia''' = it's starting to rain
|-
! bù || ʔès || kod-a-r-a || dían
|-
| not || already || work-{{small|1SG-IND-PRES}}  || here
|} ==> I don't work here yet


And for '''ʔàn'''
..


{|
'''**'''But actually to come across "pronoun" followed by "full copuls" is quite rare. As with all other verbs, ‘’’béu’’’ demands that the subject pronouns be dropped. Or at least you only hear them in exceptional circumstances.
|-
! ʔàn || kod-a-r-a || dían
For example, normally you would say ...
|-
| still || work-{{small|1SG-IND-PRES}}  || here
|} ==> I am still working here


{|
'''tìa bundari''' : "I built the house"
|-
! bù || ʔàn || kod-a-r-a || dían
|-
| not || still || work-{{small|1SG-IND-PRES}}  || here
|} ==> I no longer work here


..
However upon hearing '''jono tia bundari''' (John built the house) you would say ...


Notice that all the above expressions have the present tense ... that means that the time of onset/cessation of activity is referenced to NOW.
aiya _ pás tìa bundari = No, I built the house
However it is trivial to reference the time of onset/cessation of activity to a special reference time.
The example below has a refernce time in the past. This is shown by having verb in the past tense.


..
And another example, normally you would say


[[Image:TW_755.png]]
'''sar jutumo''' : "I am biggest"


..
However upon hearing '''jono r jutumo''' (John is biggest) you would say ...


To have the reference time in the future, simply put the future tense on the verb.
'''aiya _ pà sar jutumo''' : "No, I am biggest"


Now when you have a reference time other than NOW, this reference time must be already understood by all or it must me stated very soon. For example ...
..


{|
'''***'''There are two exceptions to this rule.
|-
! ʔès || kod-o-r-i || dían || kyù ||  yaya  ||    dai-o-r-i 
|-
| already || work-{{small|3SG-IND-PAST}}  || here  ||  when  ||  his son  ||    die-{{small|3SG-IND-PAST}}
|} ==> He had worked here for seven years when he died [have I references "his son" correctly ?]


..
..


We have already said that '''béu''' is basically an (a) (b) type language.
1If the copula subject is a '''manga''' or a '''manga''' phrase you have two possible orders.
 
However if a time period is specified between onset/cessation of activity and the reference time (I call these cases s.o.t. cases ... "specified offset time"'''béu''' becomes a  (c)  (d) type.
 
By the way ... adding an "offset time" produces a clause with a significant different clause ...  '''ʔès''' and '''ʔàn''' clause are focused on the present time ... if an "offset time" is added then we focus on a period of past time extending into the present or a period of time extending from the present into the future.


..
..
Line 1,753: Line 1,865:
{|  
{|  
|-
|-
! ʔès || kod-a-r-a || dían || euca || yìa
! nyáu ||align=center| r || bòi
|-
|-
| already || work-{{small|1SG-IND-PRES}}  || here  || seven || year
| to return ||align=center| is ||  good
|} ==> I have worked here for seven years
|} ==> To return is good


Note ... in English, one of the functions of the perfect is to indicate that an action started sometime in the past and is still going on. For example ... "I have worked here for seven years". In '''béu''' this is indicated by '''ʔès''' ...
..


I have not worked here for seven years => '''ʔès jù kodara dían euca yìa'''
{|
|-
! sòr ||align=center| bòi || nyáu
|-
| "is" ||align=center| good ||  to return
|} ==> It is good to return


..
..


[[Image:TW_759.png]]
The more accoustic weight the '''manga''' phrase has, the bigger the tendency to use the second order ...


..
..
And for '''ʔàn'''


{|  
{|  
|-
|-
! ʔàn || kod-a-r-u || dían || euca || yìa
! sòr ||align=center| bòi || nyáu || tìa || jindi
|-
|-
| yet || work-{{small|1SG-IND-FUT}}   || here || seven  || year
| "is" ||align=center| good ||  to return ||  home-{{small|DAT}} ||  now
|} ==> I will work here for seven more years
|} ==> It is good to return to home now


I will not work here for seven more years => '''ʔàn jù kodaru dían euca yìa'''
..


Note ... If I wanted to give logical symmetry to the two case I would have the present tense ('''kodara''') in the above example as well. However the human mind treats past time and future time very different as future action is uncertain. I thought this difference in outlook should be reflected in the sounds used.
With the copula coming initially the short eroded form can never be used ... that is '''*r bòi nyáu''' or '''*rò bòi nyáu''' are illegal.


The rule is that '''bù''' is not allowed in a clause that has '''ʔès'''/'''ʔàn''' and an "offset time".
..
 
2) If copula subject is a clause'''****''' with the particle '''''' at the front, you have only one possible order ...  "copula" and then  "copula complement"  and then "copular subject".
 
{|
|-
! sòr ||align=center| bòi || gò || t-o-r-e || heute
|-
| "is" ||align=center| good ||  that || come-{{small|3SG-IND-PAST}} ||  today
|} ==> It is good that he/she came today


..
..


..
'''tezau''' follows '''sàu''' when it comes to word order.


Of course we have clauses where the duration of the action is given (as opposed to "offset time". Some examples are given below ...
..
..


'''****''' this construction is covered in the Ch 4 in the section "The particle '''gò''' "


..


One interesting point ... when the present tense is used and a time period is mentioned. For example ...
----


{|
|-
!  kod-a-r-i || dían || euca || yìa
|-
| work-{{small|1SG-IND-PAST}}  || here  || seven  || year
|} ==> I worked here for seven years but I no longer work here.


Notice that the above expression has no '''ʔès'''/'''ʔàn'''.
The above has all you need to know about the copula's ... not much to them ... just a few rules.


For the '''i''', '''e''' and '''u''' tenses these constructions are self explanatory.
However I am appending a bit about the adverb '''wautus''' to this section as nowhere else really seems appropriate.


However for the present tense (the '''a''' tense) some explanation is necessary.  
'''wautus''' can be broken down into '''wáu''' "a pair of eyes" : ''''tú''' "particle giving the intrumental case" : '''s''' "adverbial marker". It means "apparently" or "seemingly".


{|
In English "by eye" usually means "by not measuring as such but roughly estimating (whatever) only using ones eyes". '''wautu''' does not mean this : it means "apparent".  
|-
!  kod-a-r-a|| dían || euca || yìa
|-
| work-{{small|1SG-IND-PRES}}  || here  || seven  || year
|} ==> I will working here for seven years in total.


In the above example ... we are told that the total work period is seven years, but we get no information about how far we are through this seven year period. One doesn't hear this construction (present tense along with a time period) that often, but when you do hear it, its meaning is quite clear.
More often come across in the form '''wautus''' "apparently".


..
'''jono boizor wautu''' = "John is OK apparently


..
'''wautus jono boizor''' = "John appears to be health"


'''jene r wautu maumala''' = "it seems as if Jane is asleep"


'''jene maumora_wautus''' = "Jane is asleep, apparently" ... Note, in the last example '''wautus''' was added as an afterthought so it needs the adverbial '''s''' (not usually necessary when an adjective follows a live verb).


The adverb has connotations of surprise ... "mirative ?"


..
..


The particle '''jù''' negates one element in a sentence (the element immediately following it). Some examples ...
== ..... Existence==


..
..


If you want to know more about aspect operators "The Meaning of Focus Particles" by Ekkehard König is the book for you.
In the above section we saw how the impersonal form of '''sàu''' links an adjective to the universe at large (well at least to the local environment).
 
In a similar way, the impersonal form of '''yáu''' "to have on your person" links an noun to the universe at large.


..
..
 
 
=== ... Slot 4===
But first let us run through some of the usages of '''yáu'''.
 
..
 
The basic usage is to link an object to a person.
 
'''jonos yór kli.o''' = John has a knike
 
..
 
 
The basic usage can be expanded and it can be used to link objects to a location.
 
{|
|-
!  tunheu-s ||  y-o-r-e || yiŋki || hè || yildos
|-
|  townhall-{{small|ERG}} ||  have-{{small|3SG-IND-PST}} || "attractive girls" || a lot || morning
|} ==>(1) the townhall had many attractive girls this morning
 
..
 
The above usage can become impersonalized (i.e. the locative subject is deleted and the person slot gets a schwa) and the meaning then becomes ... the physical object exists somewhere in the Universe. For example ...
 
..
 
'''y+r dèus''' = "there is a God" or "God exists"
 
This construction can be negated in two ways ...
 
'''bù y+r dèus''' = "there isn't a God" or '''y+r jù dèus''' = "there is no God"
 
So '''y+r''' is basically the '''béu''' existential clause. The English existential clause has "there is"/"there are".
 
 
----
 
 
Now the basic existential clause can be modified. For example ...
 
(2) '''y+r yiŋki hè''' = "There are many attractive girls"
 
Can be modified ... below we modify it with an "adjective phrase of location" '''tunheuʔe''' and an  "adjective phrase of time" '''yildos'''
 
(3) '''y+re yiŋki hè tunheuʔe yildos''' = "there were many attractive girls at the townhall this morning"
 
..
 
Which actually means exactly the same as (1) above ... (i.e. '''tunheus yore yiŋki hè yildos''')
 
Which in turn means pretty much the same as the copular sentence ...
 
(4) '''yiŋki hè rè tunheuʔe yildos''' = "many attractive girls were at the townhall this morning" ... so ... actually three ways to say the same thing ... (1), (3) and (4)
 
But note ...
 
'''*tunheuʔe rè yiŋki hè yildos''' = "at the townhall this morning were many attractive girls"
 
The above construction that is allowed in English, feels a bit strange in '''béu''' ... in the same way that "green is the man" feels a bit strange in English.
 
But three ways to say the same thing, should be sufficient ... don't you think ?
 
..
 
== ..... Shapes et al.==
 
..
 
Now '''béu''' has some justification for claiming to be an engelang. The paradigm above is quite engelangish as is the number system. The naming of shapes is also very engelangish. See below ...
 
..
 
[[Image:TW_956.png]]
 
Derived from '''dano dailo dauzo''' we have the adjectives '''danai dailai dauzai''' meaning "straight flat regular".
 
Derived from '''danai dailai dauzai''' we have the adjectives '''unai ulai uzai''' meaning "crooked/bent uneven/bumpy irregular".
 
..
 
Derived from '''dano dailo dauzo''' we have '''dante daite dauste''' meaning "a crooked line" "a rag"(also plate as in plate tectonics) "a lump"
 
The above may have some connection with '''dò''' "to move". The below may have some connection with '''kwè''' "to turn".
 
'''kwane kwaile kwauze''' = "a ring" "disc/plate/dish" "ball/sphere/globe" [Note '''kwante kwailte kwauste''' are imperfect manifestations of <= ('''kwauste'''=blob) ]
 
Also note ... '''si.anka''' = a testicle, '''si.ankau''' = a pair of testicles, '''si.ai''' = the earth (not used for other worlds), '''si.ana''' = a globe (a facsimile of <=)
 
{Note to self : should -'''ana''' derive other words ? '''taime''' = angle ? '''taume''' = solid angle ? ]
 
---
 
'''dalnoban''' = a triangle < '''uban dalno'''
 
'''dalnogan''' = a square < '''egan dalno'''
 
Note ... '''dailo''' is the usual word for square, '''dailo uzai''' would mean rectangle. However you might hear '''dalnogan''' in a mathematical context.
 
'''dalnodan''' = a pentagon < '''odan dalno'''
 
'''dalnolan''' = a hexagon < '''oilan dalno'''
 
etc. etc.
 
..
 
a tetrahedron = '''daizlogan''' < '''egan daizlo''' (i.e. a foursome of facets)
 
a cube = '''daizlolan''' < '''oilan daizlo'''
 
Note ... '''dauzo''' is the usual word for cube, '''dauzo uzai''' would mean block. However you might hear '''daislolan''' in a mathematical context.
 
an octahedron = '''daizlozan''' < '''aizan daizlo'''
 
a dodecahedron = '''daizlojain''' < '''ajain daizlo'''
 
an icosahedron = '''daizlojaizan''' < '''ajaizan daizlo'''
 
--- THE ABOVE NEEDS UPDATING ---
 
Note ... side as in flank is '''kebo'''  ... face as in human/animal face is '''muka'''
 
..
 
----TO MOVE ELSEWHERE----
 
'''yildos''' = storehouse,barn, '''yildos yè''' = barns, '''yildos ú''' = all barns
 
'''seklas''' = a glass, '''seklas yè''' = glasses (<u>not</u> spectacles)
 
'''yè''' belongs to a small set of words that are never spelt out. They have a special "short hand" symbol. The '''yè''' symbol is shown below.
 
'''húa''' = head, '''húa yè''' = heads ..........[[Image:SW_72.png]]
 
..
 
The main derivation pathways
 
..
 
Derivational morphology often involves the addition of a derivational suffix or other affix. Such an affix usually applies to words of one lexical category (part of speech) and changes them into words of another such category. For example, the English derivational suffix -ly changes adjectives into adverbs (slow → slowly).
 
Examples of English derivational patterns and their suffixes:
 
*adjective-to-noun: -ness (slow → slowness)
*adjective-to-verb: -ize (modern → modernize)
*adjective-to-adjective: -ish (red → reddish)
*adjective-to-adverb: -ly (personal → personally)
*noun-to-adjective: -al (recreation → recreational)
*noun-to-verb: -fy (glory → glorify)
*verb-to-adjective: -able (drink → drinkable)
*verb-to-noun (abstract): -ance (deliver → deliverance)
*verb-to-noun (agent): -er (write → writer)
 
Derivation can be contrasted with inflection, in that derivation produces a new word (a distinct lexeme), whereas inflection produces grammatical variants of the same word.
 
Generally speaking, inflection applies in more or less regular patterns to all members of a part of speech (for example, nearly every English verb adds -s for the third person singular present tense), while derivation follows less consistent patterns (for example, the nominalizing suffix -ity can be used with the adjectives modern and dense, but not with open or strong).
 
Derivation can also occur without any change of form, for example telephone (noun) and to telephone. This is known as zero derivation. [ All the above from "wikipedia" under "linguistic derivation" ]
 
..
 
The diagram below shows the ten main derivational processes which are absolutely fundamental to the working of the language.
[Remember the base verb should be considered a noun]
 
 
[[Image:TW_917.png]]
 
 
[1]
 
Most nouns can be used as adjectives just by placing them directly after the noun they are qualifying. Like "school bus" in English. For example ...
 
'''pintu tìa''' = a/the door of the house
 
Also to indicate possession the possessee is usually just placed after the possessed.
 
'''tìa jono''' = John's house
 
(Actually there is a particle '''yó''' joining the possessed to the possessee ... however it is rarely used. '''yó''' is also a noun meaning possessions, '''yái''' an item possessed, '''yáu''' "to have")
 
"John's house" => '''tìa yó jono''' .... but more usually '''tìa jono'''
 
This is zero derivation and is marked as [[Image:TW_816.png]] in the above diagram.
 
[2]
 
'''gèu''' = green
 
'''+ gèu''' = the green one
 
'''?azwodus''' = lactose intolerant
 
'''+ ?azwodus''' = a/the lactose intolerant one
 
[3]
 
'''gèu''' = green
 
'''k+ gèu''' = the green ones
 
'''k+ gèu làu oila''' = six green ones
 
'''sadu''' = elephant
 
'''k+ sadu''' = elephant-kind
 
'''k+ sadu làu oila''' = six elephants ... well, it is legitimate to say this ... but '''oila sadu''' is so easier.
 
[4]
 
'''gèu''' = green
 
'''kuwai gèu''' = greenness
 
[5]
 
'''yubau''' = strong
 
'''yubako''' = to strengthen
 
'''pona''' = hot
 
'''ponako''' = to heat up
 
[6]
 
'''poma''' = kick (also means leg) .... '''pomora''' = He/she is kicking
 
'''pomako''' = to kick ..... NOW '''kaupa''' = leg ... '''kipa''' = kick
 
However if the base noun ends in '''n''' ...
 
'''kwofan''' = bicycle
 
'''gàu kwofan''' = to (do) bicycle
 
[7]
 
'''pazba yubara'''  "I am strengthening the table"
 
..
 
{|
|-
! pazba || yub-a-r-a
|-
| table || strengthen-{{small|1SG-IND-PRES}} 
|}
 
'''ponara moze'''  "I am heating up some water"
 
{|
|-
! pon-a-r-a || moze
|-
|  "heat up"-{{small|1SG-IND-PRES}} || water
|}
 
[8]
 
'''tunheun kwofanaru''' "I will bicycle to the townhall"
 
..
 
{|
|-
! tunheu-n || kwofan-a-r-u
|-
| townhall-{{small|DAT}} || bicycle-{{small|1SG-IND-FUT}} 
|}
 
[9]
 
This will be covered in detail in the next chapter. However here is a quick example ...
 
'''solbara moze'''  "I am drinking water"
 
..
 
{|
|-
! solb-a-r-a || moze
|-
| drink-{{small|1SG-IND-PRES}} || water
|}
 
from the verb base '''solbe''' "to drink"
 
[10]
 
-'''s''', -'''n''', -'''a''', -'''o''' take -'''is''', all other endings take -'''s''' (including -'''ia''' and -'''ua''')
 
'''saco''' = fast, '''sacois''' = quickly
 
'''pudus''' = timid (of an animal), '''puduʒis''' = timidly
 
'''yubau''' = strong, '''yubaus''' = strongly
 
..
 
.
 
For [7] and [8] if the root that is to be transformed is monosyllabic, then we need -'''ko''' as well as -'''r'''-. For example ...
 
..
 
'''bàu''' = man
 
'''bauko''' = to man (exact same meaning as in English)
 
'''baukara téu dí''' = I am manning this position.
 
..
 
'''gèu''' = green
 
'''geuko''' = to make green
 
'''geukara pazba dí''' = I am painting this table green
 
..
 
You can say, that for monosyllabic words [7] = [5] + [9] and  [8] = [6] + [9].
 
 
..
..
 
 
Unadorned adjective can be used as nouns in many situations. Similar happens in many languages. For example ... '''klár gèu''' is ambiguous.
 
To disambiguate => '''klár kuwai gèu''' "I like greenness"  /  '''klár k+ gèu''' "I like the green ones" /  '''klár + gèu''' "I like the green one"
 
 
 
 
 
 
.
----
 
The remaining two transformations shown on the diagram are for verbalization. Actually the affix -'''ko''' is added to all adjectives or nouns in order to make a verb. However in one circumstance this affix is not needed. This is for the '''r'''-form based on a multi-syllable adjective or noun. For example ...
 
..
 
'''pazba yubaku''' = strengthen the table (a command)
 
'''pazba yubakis''' = you should strengthen the table
 
..
 
'''ponaku moze''' = heat up some water  (a command)
 
'''ponakos moze''' = he/she should heat up some water
 
..
 
 
 
 
'''bauku téu dí''' = man this position  (a command)
 
'''baukos téu dí''' = he/she should man this position
 
 
 
 
 
'''naike''' = sharp : '''naikeko''' = to sharpen
 
'''keŋkia''' = salty : '''keŋkiko''' = to add salt  ... when the adjective ends is a diphthong (and is non-monosylabic) the last vowel is dropped.
 
'''keŋkikara''' = "I am adding salt"  .... note <u>not</u> '''*keŋkara''' ... this is because '''keŋkia''' is a derived word.
 
'''sài''' = colour : '''saiya''' = colourful : '''saiwa''' = colourless : '''saiko''' = to paint (maybe via '''*saiyako''')
 
..
 
Note ... -'''ko''' is possibly an eroded version of '''gàu''' ( "to do" or "to make" ).
 
Note ... There seems to be a method of deriving a two place verb from a one place verb by affixing -'''n'''. For example ... '''diadia''' = "to happen" : '''diadian''' = "to cause". While this mechanism is seen all over the language I have not mentioned it in the chart above. This is because I consider it non-productive. I count '''daidia''' and '''diadian''' both as base words. In a similar way that English speakers consider "rise" and "raise" independent words, "lie" and "lay" independent words and "sit" and "set" independent words.
 
..
 
== ... Intensifiers==
 
..
 
THIS MUST BE REWRITTEN .... TUGE = more : JIGE = less
 
Remember earlier in this chapter, we mentioned the numerative slot (for the '''senko'''). To recap, this slot can contain ...
 
'''yè''' "plural" ... '''aʔa''' "one" ... '''ima''' "two" ... '''uya''' "three" ... '''iyo''' "few" ... '''eja''' "four" ... '''ofa''' "five" ..... up to ..... '''afaufaifa''' "215<sub>10</sub> ... '''hài''' "many"and '''ú''' "all"
 
Below is show how '''hài''' and '''iyo''' divide up the semantic space of quantity(intensity).
 
..
 
[[Image:TW_788.png]]
 
..
 
Now all '''saidau'''(adjectives) can be affixed by -'''ge''' to form the comparative'''*''' form. For example ...
 
'''bàu jutu''' = "the big man" : '''bàu jutuge''' = "the bigger man"
 
This affix can also be used with the numbers ...
 
'''juge''' "more than zero", '''a?age''' "more than one" : '''image''' "more than two" .... up to '''afaufaifage''' "more than 215<sub>10</sub>'''**'''
 
Now -'''ge''' can also be affixed to '''iyo''' letting us fill in every box of the chart given above ... [[Image:TW_789.png]]
 
..
 
Now when attached to '''saidau''', -'''ge''' gives a relative value (i.e. you are comparing one thing with another). However when -'''ge''' is attached to a numbers you get an absolute value (i.e. you are not comparing the modified item with anything).
 
When you want to compare two items as to their numerative value, you must use the particle '''yú'''.
 
(The word '''yú''' and the suffix -'''ge''' both can be translated as "more", however '''yú''' only qualifies nouns and -'''ge''' only qualifies adjectives)
 
'''jonos byór yú klogau jenewo''' = "John has more pairs of shoes than Jane"
 
'''?ár yú halmai''' = "I want more apples"
 
'''?ár hài halmai''' = "I want a lot of apples" or "I want many apples"
 
..
 
Now a number can immediately follow '''yú'''. For example ...
 
'''?ár yú léu halma''' = "I want three more apples"
 
'''yár yú halmai jenewo''' = "I have more apples than Jane" ....... [ note ... '''halma''' with '''léu''' but '''halmai''' with '''yú''' ]
 
..
 
To indicate "less" ... use '''wì'''. For example ...
 
'''jenes yór wì halmai pawo''' = "Jane has less apples than me"
 
'''jenes yór wì hói halma pawo''' = "Jane has two less apples than me" .... but it would sound better to rephrase these as ...
 
'''yár yú halmai jenewo''' = "I have more apples than Jane" : '''yár yú hói halmai jenewo''' = "I have two more apples than Jane"
 
..
 
'''*'''The affix -'''mo''' is the superlative for adjectives. When joined to '''hài''' and '''iyo''' ... we get "the majority" '''haimo''' and "the minority" '''iyomo'''
 
'''**'''Note ... the words '''noge''', '''haige''' and '''uge''' do not exist.
 
..
 
----


..
..


'''liga''' makes verbs which in themselves are quite compact more spread out. Possibly related to the verb '''ligai''' which means "to stay" or "to lie".
Above we have talked about numeratives and in detail about how to quantify '''senko'''.


..
Below we will touch on how other categories of words have their own intensifiers ...
 
{|border=1
  |align=left| '''koʕia'''
  |align=center| to cough
  |align=center| '''liga koʕia'''
  |align=center| "to be coughing", "to have a coughing fit"
  |-
  |align=left| '''timpa'''
  |align=center| to hit
  |align=center| '''liga timpa'''
  |align=center| "to be hitting" or "to assault"
  |}


..
..


'''liga''' is never used with verbs that typically have an inherent long time duration. For example ...
[[Image:TW_920.png]]
 
'''*liga glarua beuba kewe''' would be translated as "I intend to be knowing the language of '''béu''' well" ... (not good in English either ... maybe OK in Indian English ?).
 
Simply '''glarua beuba kewe''' = "I intend to know the language of '''béu''' well" ... is more felicitous in both languages.


..
..


If translating from a language with a "perfective"/"imperfective" dichotomy, perhaps using '''liga''' for translation of the "imperfective" would work. However it should be dome sparingly. If every instance of "imperfective" was rendered with '''liga''' you would end up with a very very bad translation (the style would be judged attrocious by any '''béu''' speaker).
'''hài bàu''' = many men
Now in the very best register of '''béu''' this particle is used for a certain ''poetic'' effect, it is used sparingly and is not necessary for understanding what is being said. However people that are L1 speakers of a language having a perfective/imperfective tend to over-use '''liga'''. This is not really a problem, it just shows that they are not L1 '''béu''' speakers. Conversely people that are L1 speakers of language that lacks this distinction tend to not use '''liga''' enough. Again ... no real problem.


In certain situations '''liga''' can be translated as "keep". For example ...
'''moze hè''' = a lot of water


'''liga doiku''' = keep walking


..
'''hè''' also can qualify verbs. As with normal adverbs, if it doesn't immediately follow the verb it must take the form '''hewe'''.


'''teka''' is the opposite of '''liga'''. It means "momentarily". Possibly related to the verb '''telka''' which means "to slip a little bit".
(Note to self : I can't think of a reason you would want to separate '''''' from its verb)


While in theory it can be used with almost any verb, it tends to be used disproportionately with a dozen or so verbs. For example ...
'''glá doikori hè''' = the woman walked a lot


..
'''hewe glá doikori''' = the woman walked a lot


{| border=1
'''báus timpori glá hewe''' = the man hit a woman a lot
  |align=left| '''bwí'''
  |align=center| to see
  |align=center| '''liga bwí'''
  |align=center| to watch over
  |align=center| '''teka bwí'''
  |align=center| to catch a glimpse
  |-
  |align=left| '''wòi'''
  |align=center| to think   
  |align=center| '''liga wòi'''
  |align=center| to ponder
  |align=center| '''teka wòi'''
  |align=center| to think for a moment
  |-
  |align=left| '''ʕái'''
  |align=center| to want
  |align=center| '''liga ʕái'''
  |align=center| to yearn for
  |align=center| '''teka ʕái'''
  |align=center| to have an momentary urge
  |}


..
And also can intensify '''manga''' and '''mangas'''


So there is assymetry between the usages of '''liga''' and '''teka''' ... '''liga''' used with many verbs albiet verbs of short duration ... '''teka''', while in theory can be used with many verbs, in practice the verbs usually used with it are quite restricted.
'''solbe hè moze''' = "to drink a lot of water"


..
'''solbe moze hè''' = "to drink a lot of water"


=== ... Slot 5===
The above two forms are equally likely to be found. There is a difference in meaning but you would be a real nitpicker to worry about that.


..
..


In English the perfect and "already" in many situation impart the same meaning to a clause. For example ...
'''saidau''' and '''saidaun''' are both intensified by '''sowe''' ...  


1) She has gone to town.
'''jutu sowe''' = "very big"


2) She already go to town.
'''jutun sowe''' = "the very big one"
 
In fact in many cases the perfect and "already" are often both used together ...
 
3) She has already gone to town.
 
In '''béu''' '''ʔès''' and the perfect marker (-'''yə''') can not be used together. They are two completely different things. For example ...


..
..


[[Image:TW_758.png]]
Notice that '''mangan''' and '''saidaun''' can take two intensifiers ...


..
'''hài solben hè wiski''' = the many times a lot of whisky was drink ... '''hài solben hè wiski hí pà''' = the many times I have drunk a lot of whisky


1) When you use  '''ʔès''' (or '''ʔàn''') you are concerned about the onset/cessation of an event ... probably in the recent past or near future.
'''hài gèun sowe''' = the many very green ones
 
2) When you use the perfect aspect you are concerned about the state of the subject (A or S) which has resulted from some event that might be quite far in the past ... impinging on this is a stong "experential" connotation. For example ... if John has read a book on geometry, you can assume he has some knowledge of this subject. If he has been to London, you can assume he has many sounds and sights of London stored away in his memory.
 
The meaning imparted by -'''yə''' is ...
 
a) The action is completed (some languages don't have a "perfect" but show "incompleted action" by having the "O" arguments in the partitive case). This is the base meaning of the perfect. In fact many perfect markers are derived from grammaticizing some word that means "finish".
 
b) There are "present consequences" to the action. This comes about because in most situations which are given a perfect marker THERE ARE "present consequences". Hence "present consequences" come to see associated with the perfect marker (a very simple process but behind all instances of grammaticization).
 
c) The subject has done the "action" once hence he has experienced the action and everything it involves.


..
..


-'''yə''' is used if the action is short compared to the total time frame being considered.
We will take about the opposite of intensifiers and other quantifiers in a later chapter. These are a lot rarer. The intensifiers are the ones most commonly used.
 
'''?ès''' is used if the action is long compared to the total time frame being considered. This is the same thing as saying that the action extends into NOW or into some other reference time.


..
..

Latest revision as of 19:57, 7 August 2020

TW 415.png Welcome to béu



..... Person/Tense/Evidence

..

Also called the r-form or the indicative.

..

To make a verb in the indicative mood, you must first deleted the final vowel from the base form. Then add affixes that indicate "agent", "indicative mood", "tense", "evidentiality" and "perfectness". We will refer to these as slots 1, 2, 3, 4 and 5 respectively. All these affixes together are known as the verb tail. The "agent", "indicative mood", "tense" are mandatory ... however one tense, the aortist is a null morpheme.

..

... Seven Persons

..

Slot 1 is for the agent ..

One of the 7 vowels below is must be added. These indicate the doer..

TW 109.png

Notice that there are 2 entries that represent the 1st person plural subject (i.e. we). The top one represents first person inclusive and the bottom one represents first person exclusive.

Some people might have difficulty remembering whether to use ai or au. The diagram below might help some ...

..

SW 08.png ............... SW 09.png

..

Mathematically it is as if ... ai = me + you ... and ... au = me + they ....... (sort of)

The vowels of the first person plural inclusive pronoun magi are reflected in the infix -ai-.

As are the vowels of the first person plural exclusive pronoun manu reflected in the infix -au-.

..

Note that the ai form is used when you are talking about generalities ... the so called "impersonal form" ... English uses "you" or "one" for this function.

The above defines the "person" of the verb. Then follows an "r" which indicates the word is an verb in the indicative mood. For example ...

doika = to walk

doikar = I walk

doikair and doikaur = we walk

doikir = you walk

doiker = you walk

doikor = he/she/it walks

doikur = they walk

..

... The R-form

..

One mood

..

Slot 2 is for the indicative mood marker.

..

At this point we must introduce a new sound and a new letter.


TW 355.png


This letter has not been mentioned so far because it doesn't occur in any words as such. It only occurs in grammatical suffixes and it indicates the indicative mood.

If you hear an "r" you know you are hearing the main verb of a clause.

..

... Five Tenses

..

Slot 3 is for tense markers. There are 5 tense markers in béu

..

1) *doikaro => doikar = I walk (habitually)

This could be called "the open tense" ... timewise there are no limits to an action marked in this way. Also called "the timeless tense". A sort of habitual tense. Often used for generic statements. For example ...

ngur jwadoi = "birds fly"

Actually you can say this tense has an underlying o which appears again if there is an n or s in slot 4.

2) doikaru = I will walk

This is the future tense

3) doikari = I walked

This is the past tense. This means that the action was done before today (by the way ... the béu day starts at 6 in the morning).

4) doikare = I walked

This is the near-past tense. This means that the action was done earlier on today (a good memory aid is to remember that e is the same vowel as in the English word "day")

5) doikara = I am walking

This is the present tense ... it means that the action is ongoing at the time of speaking.

..

It can be seen that béu is more fine-grained, tense-wise than most of the world's languages ... http://wals.info/chapter/66 and http://wals.info/chapter/67

..

... Evidentials

..

Two Evidentials

..

Slot 4 can have one of the evidential markers a, a, n, s or it can be empty. Actually the first a defines the subjects attitute rather than any evidentiality, however all 4 are usually just called evidential markers.

..

There are three markers that cites on what evidence the speaker is saying what he is saying. However it is not mandatory to stipulate on what evidence you are saying what you are saying. In fact most occurrences of the indicative verb do not have an evidence marker.

The markers are as follows ...

1) -n

For example ... doikorin = "I guess that he walked" ... That is the speaker worked it out from circumstances/clues observed.

I will mention waron here. It means "I think so" and is nearly as common an answer as aiwa "yes"

2) -s

For example ... doikoris = "They say he walked" ....... That is the speaker was told by some third party(ies) or overheard some third party(ies) talking.

3) -a

For example ... doikoria = "he walked, I saw him" ...... That is the speaker saw it with his own eyes.

Note that the above evidential only co-occurs with the past tense and near-past tense. Actually when used with the near-past tense, *ea => ia so the distinction between "past" and "near-past" is lost for this evidential.

Now there is a forth possibility for this slot ... and it is not actually an evidintial. Furthermore it has the same form as 3).

4) -a

For example ... doikorua = "he intends to walk" ... the agent in this case must be a sentient being of course.

This evidential marker only co-occurs with the future tense.

If the speaker doesn't know the evidential or deems it unimportant then this slot can be left empty. According to corpus studies in béu, 60% - 70% of r-form have nothing in this slot.

..

So the complete verb prefix system is ...

TW 980.png

..

It can be seen that the béu evidentiality inventory is quite substantial compared to other languages ... http://wals.info/chapter/78

Also it appears that 4 or 5 categories being appended to the verb is typical of languages of the world. See ... http://wals.info/chapter/22 [If I have understood the chapter properly]

..

... For brevity

..

We have seen that in the verb tail, o is not pronounced if it comes final (the aortist tense).

The reason for this is brevity of speech.

For brevity of writng, every occurrence of o is not written (in the verb tail). For example ...

..

TW 795.png

..

... Probability/Aspect/Negation

..

We have already covered the 4 slots for "agent", r, "tense" and "evidentiality" at the end of the verb. As well as the nuances given by these suffixes, there are particles which add further information to the basic verb. These are called (near-standers ?). These particles occur in three pre-verbal slots.

The two particles in the first slot show probability.

The seven particles in the second slot have to do with aspect in some way. Aspect can be tricky.

In the third slot, only one particle : the negating particle .

..

... Two probability particles

..

SW 051.png

..

lói = probably

màs = possibly

If nothing is in this slot, one assumes probability is 100% ... the option to challenge the underlying premise is never really considered.

The probability distribution for lói centres around 85 %.

The probability distribution for màs centres around 50 %.

One can indicate a probability distribution centred around 15 % by using lói + . For example ... lói bù doikor = He/she probably doesn't walk.

..

... Two habituality particles

..

SW 052.png

..

Every verb can be considered to have a default probability distribution over time.

TW 984.png .... By the way, don't worry too much about the time scale in these sketched.

..

timpa and nko have very simple default probability shapes. But the typical (possible) probability distribution for kludau toili is more complicated.

..

SW 001.png

Likewise the typical (possible) probability distribution for bunda tìa.

We can group all verbs into 3 classes occording to their probability distribution over time.

1) Punctual event ... timpa

2) Steady state ....... nko

3) Process ............ kludau toili or bunda tìa

Now every verb (actually "very situation" would be more acurate) have a range of typical probability distributions associated with them. However the béu aspect markers IMPOSE a typical probability distributions on any verb they touch.

For example the particle awa imposes a probability distribution quite similar to kludau toili on ANY verb that it come in contact with.

awa* gives a "habitual but irregular" (maybe best translated as "now and again" or "occasionally" or even "not usually") meaning to the verbal block.

The particle bolbo* is similar to awa in a way. However it implies quite a bit of regularity. Maybe the regularity implied by ...

TW 985.png

bolbo gives a "habitual and regular" (best translated as "normally" or "usually" or "regularly") meaning to the verbal block.

..

We saw earlier that of the five tenses. The first is a sort of habitual tense. For example ...

doikar = I walk (with a sort of habitual meaning) ... OR ... I can walk (with a sort of potential meaning)

beucar = I am sick ... OR ... I am prone to sickness

So we have a sort of habitual meaning without needing to use either awa or bolbo.

However, if we wanted to restrict the habitualness to either the past or the future, awa or bolbo is needed. For example ...

bolbo doikari = I used to walk (to school)

awa beucaru = I will be sick (when I start the chemotherapy)

awa or bolbo most often co-occur with tense (2) and tense (3). It is quite rare to have the right circumstances to use awa or bolbo with the other three tenses.

..

* awa is possibly related to the verb awata which means "to wander". bolbo is possibly related to the verb bolbolo which means "to roll". [by the way boloi means "to turn over" (as in "to turn over a mat"). boloi also means revolution [ boloi peugan means "social revolution" or boloi tun means "political revolution" ... i.e. the French Revolution ]. gwò is possibly related to the verb gwói which means "to pass (by)".

..

... Three aspect particles

..

Three aspect and a negating particle

..

SW 053.png

..

With the three particles pín, gwò and juku, the fifth tense (present tense) never co-occurs.

..

Maybe the best way to approach pín and gwò is to consider process verb like "read the book" or "build a house" *

Well you could say ...

bù bundar tìa = "I don't build houses" ... which would put you out of the running.

But if you said bundar tìa ... and you were expected to build a house, one of the following might be applicable ...

1) hogi bù bundar tìa = I still haven't started to build the house

2) pín bundar tìa = I am in the process of building a house

3) gwò bundar tìa = I have built the house

It is (2) and (3) we are interested in at the moment.

Notice that bù bundara tìa = "I am not building a house" can be true when (2) is true. Remember that tense 5 refers to the EXACT time of speaking.

SW 056.png

..

In English, it is a bit of a mouthful to say "I am in the process of building a house". So you can see that pín is a useful little particle when you want to be specific in this particular situation. However pín is the rarest out of pín, gwò and juku.

[Is pín also a preposition meaning during ... preceding a noun which is a period of time ?]

..

Lets talk about gwò now.


As we can see in (3), gwò is linked to the idea of completion. It is also linked to the idea of having done something at least once (to have "experienced" some action, in other words). For example ...

gwò jàr glasgoh = "I've been to Glasgow" as opposed to jari glasgoh = I went to Glasgow

As I said above, the present tense never co-occurs with pín, gwò and juku. However the other 3 tenses are possible ...

gwò jaru glasgoh = I will have been to Glasgow

gwò jari glasgoh = I had been to Glasgow (with reference time sometime before today)

gwò jare glasgoh = I had been to Glasgow (with reference time earlier today)

gwò could be called an experiential/resultative perfect. béu also has a resultative perfect expressed with the copula sàu and the suffix -in.

The aspect distinctions available in béu are pretty fine-grained in some areas. Maybe if béu were to become a natlang, many of the fine-grain distinctions I have given it would fall by the wayside.

..

And now it's time to introduce juku. When gwò expresses the experiential idea (as it does above) juku expresses the non-experiential idea ...

juku jare glasgoh = I had never been to Glasgow (with reference time, earlier today)

juku jari glasgoh = I had never been to Glasgow (with reference time, before today)

juku jaru glasgoh = I will never go to Glasgow (with reference time, before today)

juku like gwò is most often referenced to NOW. Hence ...

juku jàr glasgoh = I have never been to Glasgow.

..

It is useful to compare the usage of juku against the usage of .This can best be explained by taking a punctual verb such as timpa. For example, suppose we were discussing "John hitting Paul yesterday afternoon". That particular instance of "hitting" can be negated with . However suppose it is wished to widen what is negated. Suppose that you want to say that there has been no instances of "John hitting Paul" (up until the present time of course), then you would use juku to negate the proposition. This is equivalent to "never" in English and I consider it an aspect particle.

jonos polo bù timpori = John did not hit Paul

jonos polo juku timpori = John never hit Paul .... Notice that both timpori or timpore could be used. It depends upon what has been said before.

is purely negation. It has no aspect to it.

[Note 1 ... The way juku negates gwò keeping the same aspect is similar to the way 没 méi (or 没有 méiyǒu) negates 了 le the perfect aspect particle, in Mandarin. 不 [bù] not being involved, just as isn't involved in béu. ]

[Note 2 ... One little thing you should be aware off. I have equated juku with "never". Taking more strictly it should be equated with "have never". Let me expand on this ...

a) "he has never worked" => juku kodor.

b) "he doesn't work" or "he never works" => bù kodor .... in this one "never" in English is equivalent to the timeless tense plus the normal negator ... juku doesn't make an appearance ]

..

So to restate the béu aspect system ...


juku kludar toili dè = I have never read that book ... not one word

pín kludar toili dè = I have not completed that book (but I have read some of it)

gwò kludar toili dè = I have read that book .............. every word


It is not really felicitous to say *bù kludar toili dè. However if you dropped the object, then bù kludar is acceptable.

bù kludar => "I don't read" or "I never read" or even "I can't read" [This can be regarded as an event with a probability distribution over time, similar to nko. That is it is a sort of generic steady state event. For these sort of events is the normal negator]

"I don't intend to read this book" would be bù kludarua toili dè [And I think that exhausts everything I could want to do regarding "a/the book"]

In a similar way constructions like "horses never fly" *kài fanfa juku ngur are frowned upon. "horses don't fly" kài fanfa bù ngur is considered more felicitous.

..

To restate the system yet again** ...

gwò kodor he has worked juku kodor he has never worked
gwò kodori he had worked juku kodori he had never worked
gwò kodore he has worked (earlier today) juku kodore he hasn't worked (so far) today
gwò kodoru he will have worked juku kodoru he will never have worked

..

These three aspect particles also occur quite frequently in fronted adverb clauses. In these, pín, gwò or juku are followed by an base form (plus any other bits and pieces relevant to the clause), then the main clause follows. English has similar. Here are three examples from English, illustrating the possible uses of these fronted adverb clauses ...

1a) pín doika ...  : Walking dejectedly home, Peter noticed a sudden movement in the hedgerow.

1b) tìa pà pín bunda, I HAD TO LOOK AFTER TWO DAUGHTERS

2a) gwò doika ...  : Having walked all the way home in the rain, Peter was ready for a hot bath and a cosy night in, in front of the TV.

2b)gwò TO TAKE CITY, HE BURNT IT : urbem captem incendit

3) juku jò ...  : Never having gone to Casablanca before, Peter soon got lost in a warren of small streets just north of the Bazaar.

These type of fronted adverb clauses are considered good style. One comes across them quite often. Notice that the tense of the whole sentence is determined by the main clause.

..

Note ... pín can also stand before a noun, a noun that represents a period of time. In which case it means "during". Or is can stand before a base verb, in which case it is equivalent to "while" or "during". Or it can appear in an active predicate, where it specifies a certain aspect type.

..

NOTE TO SELF ... does pín cover all occurrences of "while" and "when" in English ?

..

* I do not consider "read" and "build" in themselves to be process verbs, they are sort of open-ended affairs. But for "read the book" and "build a house" there is a definite completion time ... and completion state, implied.

** You can't have too much of a good thing.

..

... Aspectual operators

..

Two overlapping-action particles

..

SW 054.png

..

I call ʔés and hogi "overlap words".

Sometimes referred to as "aspectual operators" or "aspectual particles" in the Western Linguistic Tradition.

Most languages have equivalents to these two particles ...

..

English already still
German schon noch
French déjà encore
Mandarin yîjing hái
Dutch al nog
Russian uže eščë
Serbo-Croatian već još
Finnish jo vielä
Swedish redan än(nu)
Indonesian sudah masih
béu ʔés hogi

..

hogi indicates ...

1) An activity is ongoing.

2) The activity must stop some time in the future, possibly quite soon.

3) There is a certain expectation* that the activity should have stopped by now.

ʔés indicates ...

1) An activity is ongoing.

2) The activity was not ongoing some time in the past, possibly quite recently.

3) There is a certain expectation* that the activity should not have started yet.

..

* Inevitably a connotation of "contrary to expectation" will develope to a certain degree. This is because if the situation was according to expectation often nothing would need be utterred. Hence hogi and ʔés are often found in contrary to expectation situation which in turn colours their meaning.

..

SW 046.png

..

A very interesting thing about the overlap couplet is how they are negated cross-linguisticly. Either the particle can be negated or the verb can be negated. The first case I represent with a bar over the operator+verb. The second case with a bar over the verb only.


Notice ... compared to the positive case, if the operator+verb is negated ... the line that represents onset/cessation of activity is moved to the other side of the dashed line representing "now".

Notice ... compared to the positive case, if the verb is negated ... then the yellow place becomes white and the white space becomes yellow.

..

SW 007.png .... TW 996.png

..

As you see by above ... by changing whether the negator act on the operator+verb or whether only on the verb give diametrically opposite meanings.

Note that there are 4 possible negative cases to choose from and a language only needs 2. A language (to cover all negative cases) should be either "(a) (b) type" or "(c) (d) type" or " (a) (c) type" or "(b) (d) type"

Cross linguistically there are interesting variations. All Slavic languages prefer verb negation, hence they are (c) (d) types.

In German, only (a) and (c) are allowed in positive declarations.

Nahuatl has negation of the operator so is (a) (b) type.

English is a bit tricky ... it has suppletion and uses "not yet" for situation (c) and "no longer" for situation (d). Now in English "yet" means pretty much the same as "still". I believe "yet" was the original particle but "still" over time largely usurped it in the positive case. However the form "not yet" ... if taken at face value would seem to negate the operator. But it doesn't. Logically it would make more sense if we said "yet not" instead of "not yet" [i.e. we have situation (c) rather than (b)]. I am sure there is a perfectly good explanation for this reversal but unfortunately I do not know it ... anyway ... nothing to worry about too much. [ The form "not work yet" seems more logical in its word order ... how can "not" in "not yet work" have "work" under its scope but not "yet" ... but apparently that is the way it works ]

In béu, negates the verb and comes immediately before the verb. It has scope only over the verb, rather than the whole verb phrase.


hogi kod-a-r-a dían
still work-1SG-IND-PRES here

==> I am still working here


ʔés kod-a-r-a dían
already work-1SG-IND-PRES here

==> I already work here


hogi kod-a-r-a dían
still not work-1SG-IND-PRES here

==> I don't work here yet


ʔés kod-a-r-a dían
already not work-1SG-IND-PRES here

==> I no longer work here


However although hogi bù and ?é bù are possible, they are rarely encountered. Usually the terms jù dìa and uhoge are used. The provenance of these two terms is interesting ...

means zero and is also used for negating nouns. dìa is a verb with quite a norrow meaning. It is what the sun does when it is revealing itself first thing in the morning.

I guess jù dìa is an idiomatic expression.

means "long" [not to be confused with the 13th pila?o). hoge means "longer". So uhoge means "no longer".

So the actual system for these two negatives are ...

jù dìa kod-a-r-a dían
"not yet" work-1SG-IND-PRES here

==> I don't work here yet


uhoge kod-a-r-a dían
"no longer" work-1SG-IND-PRES here

==> I no longer work here


These operators are usually used to specify overlap with present time ... (I call the present time, NOW, in the diagrams). I would think this is true of every language (notice that the above examples the tense is always -a). However it is a trivial matter to reference the time of onset/cessation of activity to a different time ... you just change the tense.

..

... Verbal Moods

..

When people speak they have different intentions. That is they are trying to achieve different things by speaking ... maybe they are trying to convey information, or wanting somebody to do something, or not to do something, or they are just expressing their feelings about something. All these are examples of what is called moods. Different languages have different methods of coding their moods. Also the various moods of a languages cover a different semantic range compared to other languages.

There are 6 moods in béu. The prohibitive, indicative, optative, imperative, suggestive and interrogative ... 2 of these are represented by changes to the root and 4 by adding particles.

Two verb forms ... the inflinitive and the conflative ... do not represent moods, but I present them here along with the moods. These both are represented by changes to the root.

..

SW 189.png

..

How the different moods and forms interact are shown above. This will al be explained later.

..

... The base form

..

About 32% of multi syllable maŋga end in "a".

About 16% of multi syllable maŋga end in "e", and the same for "o".

About 9% of multi syllable maŋga end in "au", and the same for "oi", "eu" and "ai".

TW 626.png

Note that no maŋga end in "i", "u", "ia" and "ua"

"i" is reserved for marking verb chains, which will be explained later.

"u" is used for the imperative mood ... i.e. for commanding people.

"ia" is used for a past passive participle. For example ...

yubako = to strengthen

yubakia = strengthened ... as in pazba dí r yubakia => "this table is strengthened"

"ua" could be called the future passive participle I guess. For example ...

ndi r yubakua => these ones must be strengthened

To form a negative base form the word is placed immediately in front of the verb. For example ...

doika = to walk

jù doika = to not walk .... not to walk

..

... The imperative

..

You use the following forms for giving orders ... for giving commands. When you use the following forms you do not expect a discussion about the appropriateness of the action ... although a discussion about the best way to perform the action is possible.

..

For non-monosyllabic verbs ...

The final vowel of the maŋga is deleted and replaced with u.

doika = to walk

doiku = walk !

..

For monosyllabic verbs -hu is appended.

gàu = "to do"

gauhu = "do it" ... often is added fot extra emphasis.

só gauhu = do it !

One verb has an irregular form.

= "to go"

ojo = "go" ... actually a bit abrupt, probably expressing exasperation, veering towards "fuck off" ... itself can be used as a very polite form.

..

The imperative cab be directed at second person singular or second person plural. When addressing a group and issuing a command to the entire group you sort of let your eyes flick over the entire group. When addressing a group and issuing a command to one person you keep your eyes on this person when issuing the command ... maybe saying their name before the command ... probably preseded by which is a vocative marker as well as being an emphatic particle.

[ Note ... I think that in English, the infinitive usually has "to" in front of it, in order to distinguish it from the imperative. In béu too there is a need to distinguish between these two verb forms. However as the imperative occurs less often than the infinitive, I have decided to mark the imperative. ]

..

... The prohibitive

..

This is also called the negative imperative. Semantically it is the opposite of the imperative. It is formed by putting the particle kyà before maŋga.

kyà doika = don't walk

That is pretty much all there is to say about it.

..

... The interrogative

..

The interrogative, also called a polar question. This is a question that can be answered with "yes" or "no".

..

To turn a normal statement ( i.e. with the verb in its r-form) into a polar question the r is simply changed into ?.


And here is an example of it in action ...


SW 195.png ... lea r tiji = Lea's small SW 190.png ... lea sòr tiji = Lea is small SW 191.png ... lea so?o tiji = Is Lea small ?

..

Polar questions also exhibit a certain pitch contour ... the pitch rises towards the end of the utterance. There is a symbol to show this utterance pitch contour ... SW 192.png

However the béu question mark is never used when it is obvious that we have a question. But sometimes a single name, noun or adjective can constitute a question by itself. In these cases the special symbol is used.

SW 193.png ... Lea ?

..

The interrogative is neutral as to the response expected ... well at least in positive questions.

To answer a positive question you answer ʔaiwa "yes" or aiya "no" (of course if "yes" or "no" are not adequate, you can digress ... the same as any language).

Here is a positive question ...

glá so?o hauʔe = Is the woman beautiful ?

To which you answer ʔaiwa "yes" or aiya "no". [Actually these two words have their own unique intonation pattern ... at least when said in isolation (see CH1 : Some interjections) ]

..

To answer a negative question it is not so simple. ʔaiwa and aiya are deemed insufficient to answer a negative question on their own. For example ...

glá bù so?o hauʔe = Isn't the woman beautiful ?

If she is not beautiful, you should answer bù sòr*, if she is you can answer either sòr or soro or sòr hau?e

..

We have mentioned already ... in the above section about seŋko. This is the focus particle. It has a number of uses. When you want to emphasis one word in a clause, you would stick in front of the word**.

Another use for is when hailing somebody .... só jono = Hey Johnny

You can also stick it in front of someone's name when you are talking to them. However it is not a "vocative case" exactly. Well for one thing it is never mandatory. When used the speaker is gently chiding the listener : he is saying, something like ... the view you have is unique/unreasonable or the act you have done is unique/unreasonable. When I say unique I mean "only the listener" hold these views : the listener's views/actions are a bit strange.

can also be used to highlight one element is a statement or polar question. For example ...

Statement ... bàus gláh nori alha = the man gave flowers to the woman

Focused statement ... bàus só gláh nori alha = It is the woman to whom the man gave flowers.

Unfocused question ... bàus gláh no?i alha = Did the man give flowers to the woman ?

Focused statement ... bàus só gláh no?i alha = It is to the woman that the man gave flowers ?

..

Any argument can be focused in this way. [béu also has a means of "fronting" to emphasize an element in a sentence. This is discussed elsewhere]

..

*Mmm ... maybe you could answer ʔaiwa here ... but a bit unusual ... not entirely felicitous.

**In English, when you want to emphasis a word, you make it more accoustically prominent : you don't rush over it but give it a very careful articulation. This is iconic and I guess all languages do the same. It is a pity that there is no easy way to represent this in the English orthography apart from increasing the font size or adding exclamation marks.

..

... The suggestive

..


We have come across kái before. In chapter 2.10 we saw that it was a question word meaning "what kind of". It normally follows a noun being an adjective. For example ...

báu kái = what type of man ?

òn rò báu kái = what type of man is he ?

òn rò deuta kái = what type of soldier is he ?

dí kái = this is what type ?

But just as a normal adjective can be a copula complement, so can kái.

òn rò kái = what type is he ?

dí r kái = this is what type ?

?ò r kái = what type of thing is it ?

However when you see kái utterance initial you know that it has a slightly different function : it is introducing the "soliciting opinion" mood. For example ...

kái àn nyairu tìah jindi => "how about we go home now" => "let's go home now"

Actually kái àn is sometimes rendered simply àn. Maybe you come across the two alternatives an equal amount of times.

Is there any difference between the two forms ? Well ... yes. kái àn is used when the proposed venture is connected to leisure and pleasure. àn is used in more work-a-day situations.

Now ... as with the "optative", the "soliciting opinion" mood is usually orientated towards the future and uses maŋga. However their are circumstances where you solicit opinion about past events [for example a group of detectives on a crime scene discussing the possible steps taken by the perpetrator]. In these circumstances the r-form would be used preceded by the particle ... [see the table in the section above]

The main thing about this mood is that the speaker is asking for feedback/advice/approval or disapproval. But it overlaps with the field "gently suggesting a course of action" somewhat.

..

... The conflative

..

Actually the verb itself is called an i-form verb. But a clause that has one or more i-form verbs is called a conflative clause.

I will only touch on this subject here ... in Ch 10 there is a section that goes into this verb form in exhaustive detail. But one quick example ...

..

jana jonos holdori nti flə sainyi uya => "yesterday John caught, cooked and ate three fish"

..

yesterday = jana

to catch = holda

to cook = ntu

to eat = flò

three = uya

fish = sainyi

..

totai timpə+ri jw+ daun = the child was hit and died (instantly) [Note to self : how to say "the child was hit and died later"]

totai = a/the child

timpa = to hit

jwòi = to undergo

dàu = to die

dàun = to kill

jwòi dàun = to be killed

..

In a conflative clause, the first verb is conjugated as normal. However the remaining verbs are in their i-form. That is ... the final vowel of the manga is deleted and replaced with "i". If the verb is monosyllabic, the final vowel is replaced with a schwa. Semantically thei-form verbs follow the first verb. That is nti means ntu.ori and flə means flori.

In conflative clauses, there can only be one subject but there can be more than one object. A conflative clause can consist of a mixture of H verbs and ɸ verbs. If the first verb is H then the subject is in its ergative form, otherwise it is in its base form. In the example given here, the three verbs have a definite time order, so the verb order is pretty much set. But we shall see in Ch 10 many examples where this is not the case.

..

Note ... in this example, all three verbs are intransitive and have the same object. So léu sainyi uya can not come between any of the verbs, but must come either before them all or after them all ... jana jonos sainyi uya holdori nti flə => "yesterday John caught, cooked and ate the three fish"

..

My motivation for having the conflative is to express meanings such as "through" or "into" by pure verbs ... i.e. "to go through", "to enter".

Also the béu verb tail can get pretty long so I didn't want it to be necessary to repeat it three or four times in quick succession.

Conflative clauses are very often used to describe situations involving motion. But no actual restrictions on what verbs can enter into a conflative clause (of course the verbs plus other arguments must represent a coherent subset of reality. That is the overall clause must make sense semantically).


..

To say that one activity happens totally within the time of an other activity, we use the conflative plus the particle pín which we met earlier in this chapter. For example ...

jonos lailore pín doiki = "John sang while walking earlier today"

jonos lailore pín doiki tunheun = "John sang while walking to the civic centre earlier today"

The whole constuctions (i.e. pín doiki and pín doiki tunheuh) are equivalent adverbs.

An adverb meaning "the r-form (matrix verb) happened during the time of the pín + -i verb".

..

... The optative

..

See Ch 4 : The particles àn and

..

..... Negativity

..

béu has three particles/prefixes for expressing negativity.

Different particles for different parts of speech. Usually the particle is immediately to the left of the concept it modifies.

..

SW 145.png

..

negates the live verb (i.e. the verb in its r-form). We have encountered already in the section "probability/aspect/negation".

The verb in its u-form is negated by the particle kyà to the left of the maŋga. For example ...

..

sauhu bòi= be good

However kyà sàu bòi = "don’t be good" instead of *bù sauhu bòi

..

The verb in its u-form can not be negated.

..

u- can connect to any adjective.

?ár wèu u.ai = I want a nonwhite car (I want a car, any colour but white)

u- can on occasion be prefixed to nouns, the same as "non"- is used in English. However this construction is quite rare.

u- can connect to some verbs. The number of verbs it can connect to is limited ... about 20 or 30. Here are some examples ...

..

kunja to fold ukunja to unfold
laiba to cover ulaiba to uncover
tata to tangle utata to untangle
fuŋga to fasten, to lock ufuŋga to unfasten, to unlock
benda to assemble, to put together ubenda to take apart, to disassemble
pauca to stop up, to block upauca to unstop
senza to weave uzenza to unravel
fiŋka to put on clothes, to dress ufiŋka to undress

..

negates nouns. In the next chapter we will encounter it in the section on numbers. It means "zero".

It also negates maŋga or dead verbs.

It also negates clauses. For example ...

jù àn ?ár jò = "not that I want to go"

..

Sometimes béu uses two of these three methods in the same sentence. I guess you could call this double negation. Double negation does NOT cancel, and it does NOT produce emphatic negation.

..

Here is an example of / double negation ... jenes bù mbor jù flò cokolata ... meaning "Jane lacks the willpower to resist chocolates".

..

And here is an example of .-u double negation ...

..

SW 149.png ..................... SW 148.png

..

mutu/umutu "important/unimportant" patterns with such antonym pairs as big/small ( jutu/tiji ) in that the two pole values together do not fill up the entire semantic space.

..

Sometimes you have a choice, as to which negative to use. As in English, where "I don't have a house" can also be exressed as "I have no house". in béu you can say bù byár tìa or byár jù tìa. For both languages the latter form comes across as being more vivid, carries greater emotion [I am not 100% sure why this should be so].

..

..... Six useful verbs

..

Six verbs of a kind

..

bala to open kala to shut/close
bana to let go, to release, to free ... kana to connect, to make fast, to join
baza to empty kaza to fill

..

And we have six common adjectives derived from the above ...

..

balya open kalya shut/closed
banya free, seperate kanya connected, joined
baʒya empty kaʒya full

..

balo an key kalo a (window)shutter/valve
bano padding kano link/connector
bazo a void/vucuum bano fill


The o suffix implies something solid. "connection", "association" or "relationship" would be covered by the manga ... kana.

bazda = desert ?? : kazda = ocean " kanda = an intersection ?? : balda = a gap/opening

bano originally padding to separate a warriors leather armour from his tunic.

..

..... Valency

..

In every language a particular verb can be associated with a number of nouns (we usually called these nouns arguments of the verb). For example ....

jono-s jene-h slaigau haun-o-r-a eŋglaba-tu
John-ERG Jane-DAT calculus teach-3SG-IND-PRES English-INST

==> John is teaching calculus to Jane in English

In the above example "teach" is associated with 4 nouns.

Now things can get a bit confusing here. Some people hold that it is easy to distinguish between "core arguments" which are essential and "peripheral arguments" which simply add more information. But this is questionable. The consensus w.r.t. English seems to be that if an argument requires a preposition, then it is a "peripheral arguments", if no preposition required then it is a "core argument". A simple to implement system at the least.

In the above example "English" can be dismissed as a peripheral argument because of "using". But what about "Jane". In the above example Jane's roll in the clause is defined by the prefix "to". But what if "John is teaching calculus to Jane in English" is re-arranged as "John is teaching Jane calculus in English"? Here you have three nouns not qualified by a prefix. In English "teach" is sometimes called a ditransitive verb (a verb that can take three essential (unmarked) arguments).

In beu no verbs are considered ditransitive ... Jane will always be marked by the dative suffix. Now you might argue that every instance of teaching involves "somebody getting taught" ... well this is true, but it is also true that every instance of teaching involves some language being used. At the end of the day ... the English verb "teach" means exactly the same as its béu equivalent ( haun ). It is just that there are two different conventions for expounding an action (verb) in two different linguistic traditions. The béu linguistic tradition is the simplest :-)

The béu linguistic tradition divides all verbs in into two types .... H (transitive) and Ø (intransitive). In dictionaries all verbs are marked by the simbol H or Ø. H means a transitive verb ( called a "dash verb" ) and Ø means an intransitive verb ( called a "stroke verb" ). The rule is ...

..

A verb is H if it is ever associated with a noun that has the ergative marker "-s".

A verb is Ø if it is never associated with a noun that has the ergative marker "-s".

..

Now I will introduce the S A O convention which was devised by RMW Dixon. This convention is a useful way to refer to the arguments of transitive and intransitive verbs. The one argument of the intransitive verb is called the S argument. The argument of the transitive verb in which the success of the action most depends is referred to as the A argument. The argument of of the transitive verb is most affected by the action is called the O argument.

O was probably chosen from "object", A from "agent" and S from "subject" ( I find this useful to keep in mind as a memory aid). However O does not "mean" object and A does not mean agent and S does not mean subject. I (and many other linguists) use the word subject to refer to either A or S. Easier to talk about "subject" that to talk about "A or S" all the time.

[ In the béu linguistic tradition, the A argument is "the sadu noun", the O argument is the "the dash noun" and the S argument is the "the stroke noun".]

..

Now in English certain verbs appear to be Ø in some situations and H in others. These are called ambitransitive verbs.

..

1) The old woman knitted a sweater

2) The old woman knitted

"knit" is regarded as a "A=S ambitransitive". In (1) "old woman" is A ... in (2) "old woman" is S ... [ (2) is partially the reality described by (1) ]

..

3) The old woman opened the door

4) The door opened

"open" is regarded as a "O=S ambitransitive". In (3) "the door" is O ... in (2) "the door" is S ... [ (4) is not inconsistant* to being partially the reality described by (3) ]

..

In béu, there are no "ambitransitives. "knit" is considered H but with the O argument being dropped when it is unimportant or unknown. Similarly "open" is also considered H but with the A argument dropped** when it is unimportant or unknown.

bala "to open" is always H in béu. In English, "open" is sometimes transitive and sometimes intransitive.

Take pintu baləri*** "the door opened". In English the proper analysis is "door" = "S argument". Well it is subject because it comes before the verb, and as it is the only argument it must be S.

In béu the proper analysis is "door" = "O argument". We know bala "to open" is H becuse on occasion it can occur with A arguments. However in this case the only noun (pintu) is not marked for the ergative hence it must be the "O argument".

pintu baləri could also be translated as "the door was opened".

..

*(4) leave open the question whether human action brought about the action or it was due to some other cause. This question could be answered by rewriting (4) as either "The door was opened" or "The door opened by itself".

**Actually it would be possble to drop A arguments in English if the imperative was not the base verb. For example in English "knit a jersey" is a command ... but if English ... say ... suffixed "ugu" for the imperative, then the command would be "knitugu a jersey". That would allow "knit a jersey" to be interpreted as "jersey being knitted".

***We haven't come across the schwa before the "r" before. This will be explained very soon.

..

So in béu …. each verb is either H or Ø … no ambitransitives or ditransitives. Also “the passive” is not talked about … rather it is just considered a particular case of “dropping”. And actually “dropping” is not considered a bit deal … just an very obvious thing to do.

..

Now one problem with dropping arguments is that the subject (S or A) must be represented in slot "1" of the indicative verb. How should we know what to put in here ( see Ch3.1.2.1 ). One solution could be to use the 3 person plural suffix -u- ... chances are that it is a 3rd person agent and the plural is more generic than the singular. This is what Russian does to make a sort of a passive. Another solution would be to use a vowel not already appropriated for pronoun agreement. This is what béu does. The schwa is inserted in the slot just before the "r".

Everything collapses in ... to the schwa ... an impersonal schwa.

..

TW 664.png

"the door opened" = "the door was opened" = pintu baləri (Actually I do not think the schwa symbol is visually distinct enough ... from now on I will use a cross) => pintu bal+ri

..

Here are some examples of this construction [ I will call it the impersonal construction from now on ]

beuba bl+r dían = "The language of béu is spoken here"

pí gaudoheu dè_blanyo g+r = "In this factory telephones are made"

toilia bù ost+r pí duka dí = "Books are not sold in this shop"

pintu by+r bala = pintu r balwa = the door has to be opened

pintu mb+r bala = the door can be opened ........... [ to understand this example and the one above it ... see Ch 4.7 ]

hala dè nyal+ryə = that rock is eroded .......... nyale = to erode, to wear

..

Note ... the schwa can not support any tone. And as it is only used in the grammer and not in any base words as such it was not introduced in Chapter 1 (as r was not). The schwa is represented in fact by a cross in the béu writing system ...

..

TW 909.png

Note ... Some béu speakers pronounce "schwa" + "syllable final rhotic" as "ø" or "ør". These people also tend to give "ø" the proper tone. However the majority pronoun a schwa followed by a rhotic appoximant with neutral tone.

..

Now "door" is a man-made object and probably it exists in a place with many people around. So it is reasonable to expect there to be human volition involved when it opens. But what about when we get out into nature. When we see a river freezing. There is no agent to be seen behind this "freezing" ... it just happens. For this reason the verb "to freeze" doska is Ø.

But now we have become clever ... we hold dominion over nature. Hence we need to derive a word for freeze that is H. And that deriration is arrived at by appending -n.

Hence ...

doska = to freeze

moze doskori = the water froze

moze doskanaru = I will freeze the water

..

Actually any Ø can take this suffix and become H. Here are a few more examples ...

..

ngeu to fly ngeun to throw
to go jón to send
to come tèn to summon
bái to rise báin to raise
kàu to descend kàun to lower
dàu to die dàun to kill
slài to change slàin to change
diadia to happen diadian to cause

..

And here are a few more examples ....


ʔoime to be happy, happyness ʔoimor he is happy ʔoimen to make happy ʔoimin pleasant
heuno to be sad/sadness heunor she's sad heunon to make sad heunin depressing
taude to be annoyed taudor he is annoyed tauden to annoy taudin annoying
swú to be scared, fear swor she is afraid swún to scare swu.in frightening, scary
centa to be angry, anger centor he is angry centan to make angry centin really annoying
yode to be horny, lust yodor she is horny yoden to make horny yodin sexy, hot
gái to ache, pain gayor he hurts gáin to hurt (something) gai.iin painful
gwibe to be ashamed/shame/shyness gwibor she is ashamed/shy gwiben to embarrass gwibin embarrassing
doimoi to be anxious, anxiety doimor he is anxious doimoin to cause anxiety, to make anxious doimin worrying
ʔica to be jealous, jealousy ʔicor she is jealous ʔican to make jealous ʔicin causing jealousy

..

jài ?oime is an adjective meaning happy by nature.


Six H can also take -n as well. They are ...

..

flò to eat flòn to feed, feeding
heca to see hecan to show, showing
háu to learn háun to teach, tuition
nko to know nkon to inform, informing
pòi to enter, to join pòin to put in, insertion
féu to exit, to leave féun to take out, extraction

..

In English, all the above except the last would be considered ditransitive verbs. "to take out" would not be considered ditransitive because one argument would be marked by the preposition "from". In béu they are all still H although they have undoubtedly one extra noun compared to their non-derived counter parts. Remember H and Ø were defined as ...

A verb is H if it is ever associated with a noun that has the ergative marker "-s".

A verb is Ø if it is never associated with a noun that has the ergative marker "-s".

(Note : fyá "to tell" means basically the same as nkon but is less formal. Also gàu means basically the same as diadian but is less formal. )

..

We have discussed bala and doska so far. The first is considered basically H and the second one basically Ø. There is a third type of verb ... for this type it is hard to say if it is more basic as Ø or more basic as H. So these verbs have two basic forms. For example ...

..

cwamo hulkori = the bridge broke

deutais cwamo helkuri = the soldiers broke the bridge

..

Actually for the first example .. the chances are that the breakage was due to wear and tear caused by human activity. But the important thing is that it is non-volitional. Also there might have been no humans around when the bridge actually did break. So we can talk about the bridge breaking by itself ... as if by an act of nature. And another example ...

..

jono wiltore = John woke up (earlier today)

jenes jone woltore = Jane woke up John (earlier today)

..

There are about 40 of these pairs. If the Ø has u the H will have e ... if the Ø has i the H will have o.

So lets summarize these three typre of verb ...

..

TW 825.png

..


So to wrap it all up about verbs and arguments ...

No verbs are ambitrasitive. They are either Ø or H. However it is easy to drop the A or the O argument from a H clause if either of them is considered trivial or is unknown.

Now in béu any H can be given a Ø meaning ( grammatically the structure is still H ) by making the the O argument tái ... meaning himself, herself, yourself etc. etc. However only animate A arguments do this. Hence ...

bàus tái timpori = the man hit himself ................. acceptable

*pintus tái balori = the door opened itself ...... unacceptable

In English there are two ways to report on a door opening without mentioning any agent ... "the door opened" and "the door was opened"

In béu only one ... pintu bal+ri ... which is just a H clause with the A argument dropped. Comparable to how "the old woman knitted"(as this would appear in béu of course) is a H clause with the O argument dropped.

..

In béu you can make a "passive participle" by suffixing -ia.

If you come across something broken and you know it was broken by human volition ... you would call it helkia.

If you come across something broken and you did not know how it was broken ... you would call it hulkia.

If you come across something frozen you would call it doskia. There is no such word as *doskania.

..

In béu you can make the "general obligation participle" by suffixing -ua.

If you come across something that has to be broken ... you could refer to it as helkua.

If you come across something that had to be frozen ... you could refer to it as doskanua.

There is no such words as *doskua or *hulkua

..

The above method of presenting a verb like bala hints at human volition. To get rid of this connotation (to suggest that the event happened naturely) we must use tezau "to become" plus an adjective. This is demonstrated below ...

Consider geuko = "to turn something green" ... H ... derived from gèu "green"


1) báu tezori gèu = The man became green .. ........................ natural

2) báu geuk+ri = The man was made green .................... human volition

3) báus tái geukori = The man made himself green ......... human volition

..

Now consider bala = "to open" ... H


1) pintu tezori balya = the door became opened = the door opened .......... natural ................ [ here the agent could be anything ... the wind ... or even some fairy cái ... use your imagination ]

2) pintu bal+ri = the door was opened ............................................... human volition .... [ this one implies that the agent was human but is either unknown or unimportant and the action deliberate ]

Note ... there is no (3) here as a door is non-human.

..

In either of the (1)'s wistia "deliberately/carefully" or wistua "accidently/carelessly" can be added after* tezori. This automatically makes Agent => Human

The same for the (2)'s, but the incidence of wistua should greatly excede the incidence of wistia as "intention" is the default for this construction.

With (3) the connotation of intent is so strong that wistia/ wistua could be considered a bit infelicitous ... not impossible but indicative of an unusual situation.

* or wistiwe or wistuwe if not immediately after the verb. [by the way ... wisto = "mind/brain" by the way]

..

..

PUT ANOTHER WAY ...

There are many actions that are kind of fluid as to the number of participants involved. When languages code an action they take into account whether the action is normally* involves a single paricipant or two participants [ three participants is also possible but that is another story ]. And then the relevant language will add extra stuff (an extra word … bit of word … something like that) when this action involves more or less participants than suggested by the basic word coding this action.

Two examples from French.

The action of boiling is deemed => single paricipant => bouillir When two participants, we add the word faire => faire bouillir

The action of breaking is deemed => double paricipant => casser When only a single participant, we add the word se => se casser

Certain languages deem certain actions pretty evenly split between single-participant manifestations and double-participant manifestations. In these cases, it can be impossible to determine what is the basic form of the verb.

An example from Swahili.

cham-k-a = to boil as the soup over the open fire boils cham-sh-a = to boil as your mother boils the water for a cup of tea

Further examples, Japanese this time.


生きる ikiru to live : 活かす ikasu to revive
逃げる nigeru to escape : 逃がす nigasu to set free
揺れる yureru to sway : 揺らす yurasu to shake


Japanese has a many verbs pairs of this sort.

..

* The choice can be culturally determined in some circumstances. Imagine a community in which each grown male visits the barber to get shaved every morning versus a community in which shaving is a private affair. The language of the former will inevitably pattern "shave" as transitive, anf the latter will inevitably pattern "shave" as intransitive.

..

..... To undergo

..

We have seen the subjectless verb form above where the vowel before the r becomes a schwa.`However there is another way to drop a subject ... by using the verb jwòi "to undergo" followed by the base form. Of these two ways of dropping the subject, the former is overwhelmingly preferred. However for forming present participles and infinitives, the second method is necessary.

timp+ra pà = I am being hit : jwola timpa = being hit : jwòi timpa = to be hit

[Note to self .... sort out the below ... and also all the RUBBISH PARTICIPLE stuff I have]

hecari jono katala lazde = I saw John cutting the grass ....................... katala lazde is a saidau kaza ..... katala is a saidau baga

hecari lazde jwola kata = I saw the grass being cut ............................. jwola kata is a saidau kaza

hecari lazde jwola kata hí jono = I saw the grass being cut by John .... jwola kata hí jono is a saidau kaza

Note ... although the suffix is probably connected to the second pila?o it should be recognized as a separate siffix here. If it was the pila?o we would have ... bwari lazde là jwòi kata

hecari lazde kataya = I saw the grass that has been cut

hecari lazde katawa = I saw grass that must be cut = I saw that the grass must be cut

lazde katawa hecari = I saw the grass that must be cut

hecari lazde nài r katawa

..

..... The copula

..

The three* components of a copular clause usually have a strict order*** ... "copular subject" => "copula" => "copula complement". For example ...

..

"copular subject" "copula" "copula complement"
jono r koduʒi
John is diligent
- - -
jono r moltai
John is doctor

..

The copula's base form is sàu. You will see that it is listed among the 37 short verbs. However it patterns differently from the other 36. And indeed it patterns differently from all other verbs. Below are the r-forms of sàu ...

..

TW 969.png

..

The copula form rule ... "When the copular subject noun (or noun phrase) is overtly stated, use the short form. At all other times, use the long form"

..

The short form is used when the copular subject noun (or noun phrase) is overtly stated. In other situations the full form is used. For example when the copular subject is a pronoun**, the long form must be used.

You can see in the above chart that the short form of the aortist tense has two forms. is used in two situations ...

1) If the copula subject ends in a consonant. For example ....

sòs rò hau?e = the snow is beautiful

2) If an evidential is tagged on. For example ...

tìa ròn hau?e = the house is beautiful (I guess)

..

r by itself is used in all other situations.it is a clitic attached the the last vowel of the copula subject. However it is always written as a separate word. For example ....

tomo r tumu = Thomas is stupid

It takes the tone of the copula subject.

..

The aortist form is the form corresponding to "am", "are" ans is in English. The present tense is "marked" (i.e. the unusual case that carries extra eaning). For example ...

..

sòs rò hau?e = snow is beatiful ….. a timeless truth

sòs rà hau?e = the snow is beatiful (for now) ... maybe the speakers are contemplating the snow melting and the consequent slush

..

And another example ...

..

jono r bòi = John is good (it is his nature)

jono rà bòi = John is being good ... maybe to impress somebody who is visiting.

Note ... to say jono rà bòi invalidates jono r bòi to a certain extent.

..

Because there is a strict word order, definiteness can not be expressed as it usually is with other verbs (S, O, A, dative ... left of verb if definite, right of verb if not). However the particles èn and ín can be drafted for this purpose.

[Note to self : should every pila?o defined argument act thus ... what about other arguments ? ]

It is only the r-form of the copula which is irregular. All other forms are perfectly normal. For example ...

sauhu bòi = be good ................................................................. u-form

kodor sə kludado = he works as a clark .................................... i-form

kodi sòr kludado = he/she works as a clark …........................… i-form .............. Actually, I think this way is better (change the rest of the website ?)

..

There is also the change of state copula, tezau. While tezau < + sàu, I would not call it a calque on English "become", rather the deep semantic process that formed "become" in English, worked also in béu.

There is strict word order with this copula as well ...

..

"copular subject" "copula" "copula complement"
jono tezori koduʒi
John became diligent
- - -
jono tezori moltai
John became doctor

..

As you can see there is no erosion here.

Notice that for the two copulas the copuls subjects are always unmarked ... that is they never take the ergative suffix.

..

How to negate a copular sentence ? Some examples ...

jono bù r jutu = john isn’t big

bù sòr jutu = he/she isn’t big

òn bù sòr jutu = HE isn’t big (I am)

In the last example, it is not necessary to have the full copula form to show 3SG ... *òn bù r jutu ... would not be confusing. However we continue to abide by "the copula form rule"

..

* Well sometimes the copular subject is dropped so two components. It is dropped if the subject is "the world"/"the environment". Under the section "Valancy" we introduced the impersonal form of the verb ... normally used when the subject is "unknown"/"trivial". The copula also has an impersonal form. However now the reason is not because the subject is trivial : rather the opposite, the subject is all encompassing.

Note ... Other languages use "world" or "environment" as the subject in similar situations, English used "it".

As with English, this construction is often used for the weather ...

fona = rain : fonia = rainy/raining : fonua = dry (well not raining). So ...

s+ra fonia = it's raining

tez+ra fonia = it's starting to rain

..

**But actually to come across "pronoun" followed by "full copuls" is quite rare. As with all other verbs, ‘’’béu’’’ demands that the subject pronouns be dropped. Or at least you only hear them in exceptional circumstances.

For example, normally you would say ...

tìa bundari : "I built the house"

However upon hearing jono tia bundari (John built the house) you would say ...

aiya _ pás tìa bundari = No, I built the house

And another example, normally you would say

sar jutumo : "I am biggest"

However upon hearing jono r jutumo (John is biggest) you would say ...

aiya _ pà sar jutumo : "No, I am biggest"

..

***There are two exceptions to this rule.

..

1) If the copula subject is a manga or a manga phrase you have two possible orders.

..

nyáu r bòi
to return is good

==> To return is good

..

sòr bòi nyáu
"is" good to return

==> It is good to return

..

The more accoustic weight the manga phrase has, the bigger the tendency to use the second order ...

..

sòr bòi nyáu tìa jindi
"is" good to return home-DAT now

==> It is good to return to home now

..

With the copula coming initially the short eroded form can never be used ... that is *r bòi nyáu or *rò bòi nyáu are illegal.

..

2) If copula subject is a clause**** with the particle at the front, you have only one possible order ... "copula" and then "copula complement" and then "copular subject".

sòr bòi t-o-r-e heute
"is" good that come-3SG-IND-PAST today

==> It is good that he/she came today

..

tezau follows sàu when it comes to word order.

..

**** this construction is covered in the Ch 4 in the section "The particle "




The above has all you need to know about the copula's ... not much to them ... just a few rules.

However I am appending a bit about the adverb wautus to this section as nowhere else really seems appropriate.

wautus can be broken down into wáu "a pair of eyes" : ' "particle giving the intrumental case" : s "adverbial marker". It means "apparently" or "seemingly".

In English "by eye" usually means "by not measuring as such but roughly estimating (whatever) only using ones eyes". wautu does not mean this : it means "apparent".

More often come across in the form wautus "apparently".

jono boizor wautu = "John is OK apparently

wautus jono boizor = "John appears to be health"

jene r wautu maumala = "it seems as if Jane is asleep"

jene maumora_wautus = "Jane is asleep, apparently" ... Note, in the last example wautus was added as an afterthought so it needs the adverbial s (not usually necessary when an adjective follows a live verb).

The adverb has connotations of surprise ... "mirative ?"

..

..... Existence

..

In the above section we saw how the impersonal form of sàu links an adjective to the universe at large (well at least to the local environment).

In a similar way, the impersonal form of yáu "to have on your person" links an noun to the universe at large.

..

But first let us run through some of the usages of yáu.

..

The basic usage is to link an object to a person.

jonos yór kli.o = John has a knike

..


The basic usage can be expanded and it can be used to link objects to a location.

tunheu-s y-o-r-e yiŋki yildos
townhall-ERG have-3SG-IND-PST "attractive girls" a lot morning

==>(1) the townhall had many attractive girls this morning

..

The above usage can become impersonalized (i.e. the locative subject is deleted and the person slot gets a schwa) and the meaning then becomes ... the physical object exists somewhere in the Universe. For example ...

..

y+r dèus = "there is a God" or "God exists"

This construction can be negated in two ways ...

bù y+r dèus = "there isn't a God" or y+r jù dèus = "there is no God"

So y+r is basically the béu existential clause. The English existential clause has "there is"/"there are".




Now the basic existential clause can be modified. For example ...

(2) y+r yiŋki hè = "There are many attractive girls"

Can be modified ... below we modify it with an "adjective phrase of location" tunheuʔe and an "adjective phrase of time" yildos

(3) y+re yiŋki hè tunheuʔe yildos = "there were many attractive girls at the townhall this morning"

..

Which actually means exactly the same as (1) above ... (i.e. tunheus yore yiŋki hè yildos)

Which in turn means pretty much the same as the copular sentence ...

(4) yiŋki hè rè tunheuʔe yildos = "many attractive girls were at the townhall this morning" ... so ... actually three ways to say the same thing ... (1), (3) and (4)

But note ...

*tunheuʔe rè yiŋki hè yildos = "at the townhall this morning were many attractive girls"

The above construction that is allowed in English, feels a bit strange in béu ... in the same way that "green is the man" feels a bit strange in English.

But three ways to say the same thing, should be sufficient ... don't you think ?

..

..... Shapes et al.

..

Now béu has some justification for claiming to be an engelang. The paradigm above is quite engelangish as is the number system. The naming of shapes is also very engelangish. See below ...

..

TW 956.png

Derived from dano dailo dauzo we have the adjectives danai dailai dauzai meaning "straight flat regular".

Derived from danai dailai dauzai we have the adjectives unai ulai uzai meaning "crooked/bent uneven/bumpy irregular".

..

Derived from dano dailo dauzo we have dante daite dauste meaning "a crooked line" "a rag"(also plate as in plate tectonics) "a lump"

The above may have some connection with "to move". The below may have some connection with kwè "to turn".

kwane kwaile kwauze = "a ring" "disc/plate/dish" "ball/sphere/globe" [Note kwante kwailte kwauste are imperfect manifestations of <= (kwauste=blob) ]

Also note ... si.anka = a testicle, si.ankau = a pair of testicles, si.ai = the earth (not used for other worlds), si.ana = a globe (a facsimile of <=)

{Note to self : should -ana derive other words ? taime = angle ? taume = solid angle ? ]

---

dalnoban = a triangle < uban dalno

dalnogan = a square < egan dalno

Note ... dailo is the usual word for square, dailo uzai would mean rectangle. However you might hear dalnogan in a mathematical context.

dalnodan = a pentagon < odan dalno

dalnolan = a hexagon < oilan dalno

etc. etc.

..

a tetrahedron = daizlogan < egan daizlo (i.e. a foursome of facets)

a cube = daizlolan < oilan daizlo

Note ... dauzo is the usual word for cube, dauzo uzai would mean block. However you might hear daislolan in a mathematical context.

an octahedron = daizlozan < aizan daizlo

a dodecahedron = daizlojain < ajain daizlo

an icosahedron = daizlojaizan < ajaizan daizlo

--- THE ABOVE NEEDS UPDATING ---

Note ... side as in flank is kebo ... face as in human/animal face is muka

..


TO MOVE ELSEWHERE----

yildos = storehouse,barn, yildos yè = barns, yildos ú = all barns

seklas = a glass, seklas yè = glasses (not spectacles)

belongs to a small set of words that are never spelt out. They have a special "short hand" symbol. The symbol is shown below.

húa = head, húa yè = heads ..........SW 72.png

..

The main derivation pathways

..

Derivational morphology often involves the addition of a derivational suffix or other affix. Such an affix usually applies to words of one lexical category (part of speech) and changes them into words of another such category. For example, the English derivational suffix -ly changes adjectives into adverbs (slow → slowly).

Examples of English derivational patterns and their suffixes:

  • adjective-to-noun: -ness (slow → slowness)
  • adjective-to-verb: -ize (modern → modernize)
  • adjective-to-adjective: -ish (red → reddish)
  • adjective-to-adverb: -ly (personal → personally)
  • noun-to-adjective: -al (recreation → recreational)
  • noun-to-verb: -fy (glory → glorify)
  • verb-to-adjective: -able (drink → drinkable)
  • verb-to-noun (abstract): -ance (deliver → deliverance)
  • verb-to-noun (agent): -er (write → writer)

Derivation can be contrasted with inflection, in that derivation produces a new word (a distinct lexeme), whereas inflection produces grammatical variants of the same word.

Generally speaking, inflection applies in more or less regular patterns to all members of a part of speech (for example, nearly every English verb adds -s for the third person singular present tense), while derivation follows less consistent patterns (for example, the nominalizing suffix -ity can be used with the adjectives modern and dense, but not with open or strong).

Derivation can also occur without any change of form, for example telephone (noun) and to telephone. This is known as zero derivation. [ All the above from "wikipedia" under "linguistic derivation" ]

..

The diagram below shows the ten main derivational processes which are absolutely fundamental to the working of the language. [Remember the base verb should be considered a noun]


TW 917.png


[1]

Most nouns can be used as adjectives just by placing them directly after the noun they are qualifying. Like "school bus" in English. For example ...

pintu tìa = a/the door of the house

Also to indicate possession the possessee is usually just placed after the possessed.

tìa jono = John's house

(Actually there is a particle joining the possessed to the possessee ... however it is rarely used. is also a noun meaning possessions, yái an item possessed, yáu "to have")

"John's house" => tìa yó jono .... but more usually tìa jono

This is zero derivation and is marked as TW 816.png in the above diagram.

[2]

gèu = green

+ gèu = the green one

?azwodus = lactose intolerant

+ ?azwodus = a/the lactose intolerant one

[3]

gèu = green

k+ gèu = the green ones

k+ gèu làu oila = six green ones

sadu = elephant

k+ sadu = elephant-kind

k+ sadu làu oila = six elephants ... well, it is legitimate to say this ... but oila sadu is so easier.

[4]

gèu = green

kuwai gèu = greenness

[5]

yubau = strong

yubako = to strengthen

pona = hot

ponako = to heat up

[6]

poma = kick (also means leg) .... pomora = He/she is kicking

pomako = to kick ..... NOW kaupa = leg ... kipa = kick

However if the base noun ends in n ...

kwofan = bicycle

gàu kwofan = to (do) bicycle

[7]

pazba yubara "I am strengthening the table"

..

pazba yub-a-r-a
table strengthen-1SG-IND-PRES

ponara moze "I am heating up some water"

pon-a-r-a moze
"heat up"-1SG-IND-PRES water

[8]

tunheun kwofanaru "I will bicycle to the townhall"

..

tunheu-n kwofan-a-r-u
townhall-DAT bicycle-1SG-IND-FUT

[9]

This will be covered in detail in the next chapter. However here is a quick example ...

solbara moze "I am drinking water"

..

solb-a-r-a moze
drink-1SG-IND-PRES water

from the verb base solbe "to drink"

[10]

-s, -n, -a, -o take -is, all other endings take -s (including -ia and -ua)

saco = fast, sacois = quickly

pudus = timid (of an animal), puduʒis = timidly

yubau = strong, yubaus = strongly

..

.

For [7] and [8] if the root that is to be transformed is monosyllabic, then we need -ko as well as -r-. For example ...

..

bàu = man

bauko = to man (exact same meaning as in English)

baukara téu dí = I am manning this position.

..

gèu = green

geuko = to make green

geukara pazba dí = I am painting this table green

..

You can say, that for monosyllabic words [7] = [5] + [9] and [8] = [6] + [9].


.. ..


Unadorned adjective can be used as nouns in many situations. Similar happens in many languages. For example ... klár gèu is ambiguous.

To disambiguate => klár kuwai gèu "I like greenness" / klár k+ gèu "I like the green ones" / klár + gèu "I like the green one"




.


The remaining two transformations shown on the diagram are for verbalization. Actually the affix -ko is added to all adjectives or nouns in order to make a verb. However in one circumstance this affix is not needed. This is for the r-form based on a multi-syllable adjective or noun. For example ...

..

pazba yubaku = strengthen the table (a command)

pazba yubakis = you should strengthen the table

..

ponaku moze = heat up some water (a command)

ponakos moze = he/she should heat up some water

..



bauku téu dí = man this position (a command)

baukos téu dí = he/she should man this position



naike = sharp : naikeko = to sharpen

keŋkia = salty : keŋkiko = to add salt ... when the adjective ends is a diphthong (and is non-monosylabic) the last vowel is dropped.

keŋkikara = "I am adding salt" .... note not *keŋkara ... this is because keŋkia is a derived word.

sài = colour : saiya = colourful : saiwa = colourless : saiko = to paint (maybe via *saiyako)

..

Note ... -ko is possibly an eroded version of gàu ( "to do" or "to make" ).

Note ... There seems to be a method of deriving a two place verb from a one place verb by affixing -n. For example ... diadia = "to happen" : diadian = "to cause". While this mechanism is seen all over the language I have not mentioned it in the chart above. This is because I consider it non-productive. I count daidia and diadian both as base words. In a similar way that English speakers consider "rise" and "raise" independent words, "lie" and "lay" independent words and "sit" and "set" independent words.

..

... Intensifiers

..

THIS MUST BE REWRITTEN .... TUGE = more : JIGE = less

Remember earlier in this chapter, we mentioned the numerative slot (for the senko). To recap, this slot can contain ...

"plural" ... aʔa "one" ... ima "two" ... uya "three" ... iyo "few" ... eja "four" ... ofa "five" ..... up to ..... afaufaifa "21510 ... hài "many"and ú "all"

Below is show how hài and iyo divide up the semantic space of quantity(intensity).

..

TW 788.png

..

Now all saidau(adjectives) can be affixed by -ge to form the comparative* form. For example ...

bàu jutu = "the big man" : bàu jutuge = "the bigger man"

This affix can also be used with the numbers ...

juge "more than zero", a?age "more than one" : image "more than two" .... up to afaufaifage "more than 21510**

Now -ge can also be affixed to iyo letting us fill in every box of the chart given above ... TW 789.png

..

Now when attached to saidau, -ge gives a relative value (i.e. you are comparing one thing with another). However when -ge is attached to a numbers you get an absolute value (i.e. you are not comparing the modified item with anything).

When you want to compare two items as to their numerative value, you must use the particle .

(The word and the suffix -ge both can be translated as "more", however only qualifies nouns and -ge only qualifies adjectives)

jonos byór yú klogau jenewo = "John has more pairs of shoes than Jane"

?ár yú halmai = "I want more apples"

?ár hài halmai = "I want a lot of apples" or "I want many apples"

..

Now a number can immediately follow . For example ...

?ár yú léu halma = "I want three more apples"

yár yú halmai jenewo = "I have more apples than Jane" ....... [ note ... halma with léu but halmai with ]

..

To indicate "less" ... use . For example ...

jenes yór wì halmai pawo = "Jane has less apples than me"

jenes yór wì hói halma pawo = "Jane has two less apples than me" .... but it would sound better to rephrase these as ...

yár yú halmai jenewo = "I have more apples than Jane" : yár yú hói halmai jenewo = "I have two more apples than Jane"

..

*The affix -mo is the superlative for adjectives. When joined to hài and iyo ... we get "the majority" haimo and "the minority" iyomo

**Note ... the words noge, haige and uge do not exist.

..


..

Above we have talked about numeratives and in detail about how to quantify senko.

Below we will touch on how other categories of words have their own intensifiers ...

..

TW 920.png

..

hài bàu = many men

moze hè = a lot of water


also can qualify verbs. As with normal adverbs, if it doesn't immediately follow the verb it must take the form hewe.

(Note to self : I can't think of a reason you would want to separate from its verb)

glá doikori hè = the woman walked a lot

hewe glá doikori = the woman walked a lot

báus timpori glá hewe = the man hit a woman a lot

And also can intensify manga and mangas

solbe hè moze = "to drink a lot of water"

solbe moze hè = "to drink a lot of water"

The above two forms are equally likely to be found. There is a difference in meaning but you would be a real nitpicker to worry about that.

..

saidau and saidaun are both intensified by sowe ...

jutu sowe = "very big"

jutun sowe = "the very big one"

..

Notice that mangan and saidaun can take two intensifiers ...

hài solben hè wiski = the many times a lot of whisky was drink ... hài solben hè wiski hí pà = the many times I have drunk a lot of whisky

hài gèun sowe = the many very green ones

..

We will take about the opposite of intensifiers and other quantifiers in a later chapter. These are a lot rarer. The intensifiers are the ones most commonly used.

..

... Index

  1. Introduction to Béu
  2. Béu : Chapter 1 : The Sounds
  3. Béu : Chapter 2 : The Noun
  4. Béu : Chapter 3 : The Verb
  5. Béu : Chapter 4 : Adjective
  6. Béu : Chapter 5 : Questions
  7. Béu : Chapter 6 : Derivations
  8. Béu : Chapter 7 : Way of Life 1
  9. Béu : Chapter 8 : Way of life 2
  10. Béu : Chapter 9 : Word Building
  11. Béu : Chapter 10 : Gerund Phrase
  12. Béu : Discarded Stuff
  13. A statistical explanation for the counter-factual/past-tense conflation in conditional sentences