User:Masako/naho: Difference between revisions

From FrathWiki
Jump to navigationJump to search
mNo edit summary
mNo edit summary
Line 1: Line 1:
grammar outline and resources
=Introduction=


* en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Lingua_Franca_Nova_grammar
'''Kala''' is a personal [[wp:Constructed_language|conlang]] (actually more of an [[wp:Artistic_language|artlang]]), based on my aesthetic preferences, not attached to any [[wp:Worldbuilding|conworld or conculture]]. This language draws on [[wp:Natural_language|natlangs (natural language)]], other conlangs, and of course imagination. '''Kala''' was started in late 2009. The phonemic inventory is based on [[wp:Classical_Nahuatl|Classical Nahuatl]] while the syllable structure and vowels are based on the strict (C)V structure of [[wp:Japanese_phonology#Phonotactics|Japanese]], and the presence of [[wp:Prenasalized_consonant|prenasalized]] stops is influenced by [[wp:Bantu_languages|Bantu languages]]. '''Kala’s''' grammar was initially based on Japanese but has changed based on influence from several natural and constructed languages. Many – if not most – of '''Kala''' lexemes are derived from or inspired by natural languages. A few have been taken from previous projects or constructed languages such as '''Ajara''' (a [[wp:Substitution_cipher|cipherlang]] from my youth) and [[Qatama]] (a conlang that I abandoned several years ago).
* zompist.com/virtuver.htm
* languagesgulper.com/eng/languages.html


= text =
== Characteristics ==


languagesgulper.com/eng/Quechua.html
'''Kala''' has two parts of speech. Nouns and verbs are [[wp:Content_word|content words]], while particles (and others) tend to be only [[wp:Function_word|functional]]. Many content words can be used as both nouns and verbs. The best and most common example would be '''ina''' /iːˈna/ "''food; to eat''". '''Kala''' is a context-oriented language. In most cases, the more important elements of a phrase are clustered toward the end of the sentence (e.g. verbs and their modifiers). The less important an element is to the understanding of a sentence, the more likely it is to be dropped. Consequently, many '''Kala''' sentences end-up consisting solely of a verb (or adjectival verb); more so in conversation than in written Kala, these short phrases are grammatically correct and natural. Here are some examples:


§ 2.3) Phonotactics (otomato)
* '''muya ka''' - /muːja gaː/ - <small>do Q</small> - ''(What are you) doing?''
* '''ina''' - /iːna/ - <small>eat</small> - ''(I am) eating.''
* '''tamatse''' - /tamaːˌt͡ʃɛ/ - <small>good-seem</small> - ''(That looks) good.''
* '''ueha ka''' - /weːɦa kaː/ - <small>want Q</small> - ''(Do you) want (some)?''
* '''nyasak''' - /ɲaːʃak/ - <small>thank-NEG</small> - ''No, thank (you).''


Kala words are typically made up of open syllables of the type CV (consonant-vowel) with most words having syllables exclusively of this type. There is a limited set of syllables allowed by Kala phonotactics, similar to Japanese or Chinese. Kala phonotactics does not typically allow the onsets of adjacent syllables to be identical, nor both to be labialized or palatalized. (There are a few exceptions to this, such as tata for the informal/familiar form of “father”, etc.) Syllables beginning with /l/ do not occur as the first syllable of a headword. Gemination does occur and is distinctive, however it is rare and usually only occurs in compound words.
Notice that none of the above contains any pronouns, or nouns. Any contextually understood elements may be omitted unless indispensable. There can be considerable divergence from what is grammatical, and what is acceptably idiomatic. The spectrum of formality and grammatical to idiomatic can be seen in the example below:


yasa wind & yesa peace > yassa peaceful-wind
* '''na’etla muyaye''' <small>1s-P.4s do-PST</small> ''I did it.'' > [grammatical, formal]
naka woman & kana leader > nakkan chieftess; queen
* '''etla muyaye''' <small>P.4s do-PST</small> – ''(I) did it. >> It was done.'' > [grammatical, formal, passive]
* '''na muyaye''' <small>1s do-PST</small> ''I did (it).'' > [grammatical, informal]
* '''muyaye''' <small>do-PST</small> – ''(I) did (it).'' > [semi-grammatical, idiomatic]
* '''muyye''' – /muːɟɛ/ – <small>do-PST</small> – ''(I) did (it).'' > [ungrammatical, idiomatic]


§ 4) Syntax (teyeto)
== Borrowing ==


The basic structure of a Kala sentence is: AGENT--PATIENT--VERB (or SOV) The agent is the person or thing doing the action described by the verb; the patient is the recipient of that action. The importance of word order can be seen by comparing the following sentences:  
:: ''See also'': [[Kala/etymology|''Kala etymology'']]


• mita tlaka anya tlaka mita anya
'''Kala''' borrows extensively from various natural languages. This is a very small sample of borrowings:
dog man see man dog see
The dog sees the man. The man sees the dog.


In both sentences, the words are identical; the only way to know whom is seeing who is by the order of the words in the sentence.
* '''pato''' – ''duck (Anatidae)''; from Spanish [[wiktionary:pato#Noun_9|''pato'']]
* '''kala''' – ''to speak, talk, converse''; from Arabic [[wiktionary:تكلم|''takallama'']]
* '''myonta''' – ''to allow, permit''; from Finnish [[wiktionary:myöntää|''myöntää'']]
* '''na''' – ''I, me''; from Arabic  [[wiktionary:أنا|''ʾanā'']]
* '''tsenka''' – ''orange''; from Chinese [[wiktionary:橙#Chinese|''chéng'']]
* '''uasi''' – ''to take, get, acquire''; from Lakota [[wiktionary:wasicu|''wasichu'']]
* '''a''' – ''to be, exist, yes''; from Japanese [[wiktionary:ある|''aru'']]


§ 4.1) Basic sentences (enketeye)
So, some phrases can contain words from multiple natlangs:


Intransitive (including those of the existential type) clauses in Kala minimally consist of a subject followed by an intransitive verb, giving SV word order.
* '''ta ke inu uasiye ka'''
: <small>2s O drink take-PST Q</small>
: ''Did you take the drink?''


nta’i moku sama nala
* '''ta''' - Arabic [[wiktionary:أنت|''ʾanta'']]
baby sleep sun shine
* '''ke''' - Chinese [[wiktionary:個#Definitions|''gè'']]
The baby sleeps. The sun shines.
* '''inu''' - Hawaiian [[wiktionary:inu#Hawaiian|''inu'']]
* '''uasi''' - Lakota [[wiktionary:wasicu|''wasichu'']]
* '''ka''' - Japanese [[wiktionary:か#Particle|''か'']]


Transitive clauses follow a SOV pattern and grammatically require the object particle ke.
=Phonology=


ona ke matla kuha tasako ke masala yake
== Consonants ==
mother O stew cook hunt-AG O deer-INDEF pursue
The mother is cooking the stew. The hunters are chasing some deer.


* Where '''~''' appears, it indicates [[wp:Free_variation|free variation]] between phonemes.


{|class=wikitable style="text-align: center; width:40%;"
|+Consonants
!
!Labial
!colspan=2|Alveolar
!Palatal
!Velar
!Glottal
|-
! Nasal
| m ('''m''')
|colspan=2|n ('''n''')
| ɲ ('''ny''')
|
|
|-
! Plosive
| p~b ('''p''')
|colspan=2|t~d ('''t''')
|
| k~g ('''g''')
| ʔ (''' ' ''')
|-
! Affricate
|
| ts~t͡ʃ ('''ts''') || t͡ɬ~tl ('''tl''')
|
|
|
|-
! Continuant
|
| s~ʃ ('''s''') || l~ɾ ('''l''')
|
|
| h~ɦ ('''h''')
|-
! Semivowel
|
|colspan=2|
| j ('''y''')
| w ('''u''')
|
|}


The [[wp:Glottal_stop|glottal stop]] is not phonemic but is included in the chart above for completeness. It is only ever intervocalic, meaning it is pronounced between two vowels and/or diphthongs.  Where ~ appears, it indicates free variation between phonemes. However, certain sounds change in a predictable way. For example, /h/ becomes [ɦ] when preceded or followed by a front vowel, including when labialized or palatalized. The alveolar affricates are most often /t͡ʃ/ and /t͡ɬ/.  The “s” is almost always /ʃ/ unless preceded or followed by a syllable with the onset /t͡ʃ/, in which case “s” becomes /s/. So, '''sama''' (''sun; star; solar'') is /ˈʃaːma/ where '''sitsa''' (''heat; hot'') is /ˈsiːt͡ʃa/ and '''tsisi''' (''embroider; embroidery'') is /t͡ʃiːsi/.


* '''Prenasalized''': /ᵐp ⁿt ᵑk/
* '''Labialized''':/pʷ kʷ mʷ nʷ ʃʷ hʷ t͡ʃʷ/
* '''Palatalized''': /pʲ kʲ mʲ hʲ/


<small>Note: Because of its small phoneme inventory, '''Kala''' allows for quite a lot of [[wp:Free_variation|allophonic variation]]. For example, /p t k/ may be pronounced [b d ɡ] as well as [p t k], /s l h/ as [ʃ ɾ ɦ], and /t͡s t͡ɬ/ as [t͡ʃ t͡l]; also, vowels may be either long or short.</small>


§ 4.1.2) Oblique Participants
== Vowels ==


$$$
{| class="wikitable"
|+Vowels
!
!'''Front'''
!'''Back'''
|-align=center
!'''Close'''
|{{IPA|i~ɪ}} '''(i)''' || {{IPA|u~u:}} '''(u)'''
|-align=center
!'''Mid'''
|{{IPA|e~ɛ}} '''(e)''' || {{IPA|o~o:}} '''(o)'''
|-align=center
!'''Open'''
|colspan="2" align="center"|{{IPA|a~a:}} '''(a)'''
|}


Kala verb phrases have only a single object slot. As a result, the recipient of a ditransitive clause needs to be introduced with the help of an adverbial preposition. The same strategy is also used to introduce other participants in oblique roles.  
'''Kala''' has five vowels /i/, /e/, /a/, /o/ and /u/. Each occurs in both stressed and unstressed syllables. Phonetic nasalization occurs for vowels occurring between nasal consonants or when preceding a syllable-final nasal, e.g. '''tsunka''' [ˈt͡ʃũᵑka] ('bug').


ka’e – to; toward [Dative]
=== Diphthongs ===
ma’a – with; using [Instrumental] / with; together [Comitative]
mue – without; lacking [Abessive]
nya – for (the benefit of) [Benefactive] / by [Passive]
-hue – at; in; on [Locative]


§ 4.1.2.1) Dative (ka’e)
Phonetically, '''Kala''' has only two diphthongs, both falling; [aɪ̯] and [aʊ̯], but there are five syllables that can be analyzed as rising diphthongs; [wa], [we], [ja], [je], and [jo]. The two triphthongs [waɪ̯] and [jaʊ̯] are very rare but should be noted as possible.


Dative participants can be marked with ka’e (“toward; to”), nya (“for; by”), or be syntactically indicated.
== Phonotactics ==


ntahi ke ina ka’e mita yeta ikema nya ena enke
'''Kala''' words are typically made up of [[wp:Syllable#Open_and_closed|open syllables]] of the type CV (consonant-vowel) with most words having syllables exclusively of this type. There is a limited set of syllables allowed by '''Kala''' phonotactics, similar to Japanese or Chinese.  
child O food toward dog give PROX-task for P.1s easy
The child gives food to a dog. This task is easy for me.


katiko nya ntakum tsani teki ke kama na’amyo tanyaye
* /l/ cannot occur word initially (except in loanwords and [[wp:Toponymy|toponyms]]):
old-AG for sibling-PL tell.story enemy O village 1pl.EXCL.GEN destroy-PST
:: '''lupunan''' = Lebanon
The old man recites a story for the siblings. The enemies destroyed our village.
:: '''lupusu''' = [[wp:Lupus_(constellation)|Lupus]]
:: '''


§ 4.1.2.2) Instrumental (ma’a)
=== Syllables ===


Instrumental participants can be marked with ma’a (“with; using”), nya (“for; by”), or be syntactically indicated.
Syllable structure in '''Kala''' is typically made up of open syllables of the type CV (consonant-vowel) with most lexemes having syllables exclusively of this type. The exception to this rule are the endings –'''m''' (indicating general plural), -'''n''' (indicating adverbial or adjectival use), and –'''k''' (indicating negative mood). These endings all are word final. In detail a Kala syllable can be analyzed thusly: '''(N)(C)(u, y)V(a, i)''' where ('''N''') indicates [[wp:Nasalization|nasalization]], and '''u''' and '''y''' indicate [[wp:Labialization|labialization]] and [[wp:Palatalization_(phonetics)|palatalization]] respectively.


ona ke ntahi ma’a tlimu nohya tsani nya ntaha moyapua
=== Syllable Chart ===
mother O child with blanket wrap story by elder write-PFV
The mother wraps the child in a blanket. The story has been written by the elder.


§ 4.1.2.3) Comitative (ma’a)
{| class="wikitable" style="width: 600px;"
|+the 136 basic Kala syllables
|-
!
! a
! e
! i
! o
! u
! ua
! ue
! ya
! ye
! yo
! ai
! ao
! uai
! yao
|-
! p
| align="center"|''(m)pa''
| align="center"|''(m)pe''
| align="center"|''(m)pi''
| align="center"|''(m)po''
| align="center"|''(m)pu''
| align="center"|''pua''
| align="center"|''pue''
| align="center"|''pya''
| align="center"|''pye''
| align="center"|''pyo''
| align="center"|<font color="red">''pai''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''pao''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''puai''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''pyao''</font>
|-
! t
| align="center"|''(n)ta''
| align="center"|''(n)te''
| align="center"|''(n)ti''
| align="center"|''(n)to''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|<font color="red">''tai''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''tao''</font>
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
|-
! k
| align="center"|''(n)ka''
| align="center"|''(n)ke''
| align="center"|''(n)ki''
| align="center"|''(n)ko''
| align="center"|''(n)ku''
| align="center"|''kua''
| align="center"|''kue''
| align="center"|''kya''
| align="center"|''kye''
| align="center"|''kyo''
| align="center"|<font color="red">''kai''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''kao''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''kuai''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''kyao''</font>
|-
! m
| align="center"|''ma''
| align="center"|''me''
| align="center"|''mi''
| align="center"|''mo''
| align="center"|''mu''
| align="center"|''mua''
| align="center"|''mue''
| align="center"|''mya''
| align="center"|''mye''
| align="center"|''myo''
| align="center"|<font color="red">''mai''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''mao''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''muai''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''myao''</font>
|-
! n
| align="center"|''na''
| align="center"|''ne''
| align="center"|''ni''
| align="center"|''no''
| align="center"|''nu''
| align="center"|''nua''
| align="center"|''nue''
| align="center"|''nya''
| align="center"|''nye''
| align="center"|''nyo''
| align="center"|<font color="red">''nai''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''nao''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''nuai''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''nyao''</font>
|-
! s
| align="center"|''sa''
| align="center"|''se''
| align="center"|''si''
| align="center"|''so''
| align="center"|''su''
| align="center"|''sua''
| align="center"|''sue''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|<font color="red">''sai''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''sao''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''suai''</font>
| align="center"|'' ''
|-
! h
| align="center"|''ha''
| align="center"|''he''
| align="center"|''hi''
| align="center"|''ho''
| align="center"|''hu''
| align="center"|''hua''
| align="center"|''hue''
| align="center"|''hya''
| align="center"|''hye''
| align="center"|''hyo''
| align="center"|<font color="red">''hai''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''hao''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''huai''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''hyao''</font>
|-
! ts
| align="center"|''tsa''
| align="center"|''tse''
| align="center"|''tsi''
| align="center"|''tso''
| align="center"|''tsu''
| align="center"|''tsua''
| align="center"|''tsue''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|<font color="red">''tsai''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''tsao''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''tsuai''</font>
| align="center"|'' ''
|-
! tl
| align="center"|''tla''
| align="center"|''tle''
| align="center"|''tli''
| align="center"|''tlo''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|<font color="red">''tlai''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''tlao''</font>
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
|-
! l
| align="center"|''la''
| align="center"|''le''
| align="center"|''li''
| align="center"|''lo''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|<font color="red">''lai''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''lao''</font>
| align="center"|'' ''
| align="center"|'' ''
|-
! -
| align="center"|''a''
| align="center"|''e''
| align="center"|''i''
| align="center"|''o''
| align="center"|''u''
| align="center"|''ua''
| align="center"|''ue''
| align="center"|''ya''
| align="center"|''ye''
| align="center"|''yo''
| align="center"|<font color="red">''ai''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''ao''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''uai''</font>
| align="center"|<font color="red">''yao''</font>
|}


Comitative participants are marked with the preposition ma’a (“with; together”), and anticomitative (or abessive) participants are marked with the preposition mue (“without”).
<small>Syllables such as '''nsa''', '''ntla''', or '''ntsa''' can occur but usually only in place names or loanwords. The red syllables above occur infrequently and most often as the final syllable of a word.</small>


na ma’a amyako nayo ke masa tasa ha ke naha mue ta ka’elaye ka
=== Collating Order ===
1s with friend 1s.GEN O deer hunt 3s O river without 2s toward-MOT Q
I'm hunting deer with my friend. Did she go to the river without you?


§ 4.1.2.4) Locative (hue)
The collating sequence (alphabetical order) is based on the order established in the '''Naua''' script.


Locative participants can be marked with a variety of adverbial prepositions, most typically -hue (“at; in; on”). See also 3.6.2) Locative verbs
{| class="wikitable" style="text-align: center; width: 400px;"
! Consonants
| '''p'''
| '''t'''
| '''k'''
| '''m'''
| '''n'''
| '''s'''
| '''h'''
| '''ts'''
| '''tl'''
| '''l'''
|-align=center
! Vowels
| '''a'''
| '''e'''
| '''i'''
| '''o'''
| '''u'''
| '''ua'''
| '''ue'''
| '''ya'''
| '''ye'''
| '''yo'''
|-align=center
|}


taku nayo ke poti patlahue patsi nam tlatsahue masetli
Based on this order, '''ma''' would come before '''ha''', etc. Prenasalized syllables are ordered after their non-prenasalized counterparts, i.e. '''mpa''' comes after '''pyo''' but before '''ta'''. To see the collating sequence in action, look through the [[Kala/lexicon|lexicon]].
brother 1s.GEN O goat field-LOC herd 1pl fire-LOC dance-FUT
My brother is herding goats in the field. We will dance near (at) the fire.


§ 4.1.3) Negation (nke; ak)
== Stress ==


The negative mood [NEG] (always marked finally on the primary verb) is indicated by the suffix –k or –nke (when the last syllable contains /k/). See also: 3.1.4) Mood
[[wp:Stress_(linguistics)|Stress]] generally falls on the penultimate syllable, which means that stress is ''de facto'' initial in most lemma given that stems are most often (CVCV). Monosyllabic words are not stressed. So;


tahi inyak yohuaye ha ke samalo anyak
* '''<u>ma</u>sa''' - /ˈmaːsa/ >> '''ma<u>sa</u>ko''' - /maːˈsako/
boy hunger-NEG night-PST 3s O star-PL watch-NEG
* '''tli<u>ya</u>ma''' - /tɬiːˈjama/ >> '''tliya<u>ma</u>lo''' - /tɬiːjaˈmalo/
The boy is not hungry. She did not watch the stars last night.
* '''kam''' - /kaːm/ >> '''<u>ka</u>myo''' - /ˈkaːmʲo/


In general, contiguous serial verb constructions can only be negated as a whole. Negating one or more of the verbs in the construction separately is ungrammatical.
=Morphology=


na ke tsakahue nya ina ka’elatlik
'''Kala''' is a mostly [[wp:Agglutinative language|agglutinative]] language that makes extensive use of compounding, incorporation and derivation. That is, it can add many different [[wp:Prefix (linguistics)|prefixes]] and [[wp:Suffix|suffix]]es to a [[wp:Root (linguistics)|root]] until very long words are formed, and a single word can sometimes constitute an entire sentence.
1s O home-LOC for eat toward-MOT-FUT-NEG
I'm not coming home for dinner.


However, if there is a modal auxiliary, negation may either take scope over the modal (and thus over the whole clause), or alternatively only over the non-modal part of the serial verb construction:
== Nouns ==


eta tlahapok eta tlahamyok
Nouns in '''Kala''' are inflected only for number. Other relevant distinctions are animacy and possession, but these are not marked on the noun itself. Animacy plays a role both for pronoun choice and for the validity of some syntactic constructions.
P.2s leave-compel-NEG P.2s leave-PERM-NEG
You don't have to leave. You're not allowed to leave.


In some serial verb constructions, where the middle noun phrase acts both as the object of the first verb and as the subject of the second verb, each verb phrase can be negated separately.
=== Number ===
§ 4.1.5) Passives (kemi)


Passive voice emphasizes the process rather than who is performing the action. In Kala this form is called kemi. There are few patterns to help distinguish between active and passive voices in Kala verbs. Using the passive voice is not common in Kala. It can sometimes be used to emphasize what would normally have been the object of the sentence. In order to shift emphasis away from the agent and towards the patient or theme, a transitive sentence can be passivized simply by word order, or using the particle ni (“by”) [PASS].
In general the plural suffix is not used when the plurality of the noun is clear from context. For example, while the English sentence "''there are three dogs''" would use the plural "dogs" instead of the singular "dog", the '''Kala''' sentence '''mita ha'o a''' "''dog three exist''" keeps the word '''mita''' "''dog''" in its unmarked form, as the numeral makes the plural marker redundant.


ACTIVE PASSIVE
Nouns are marked for number; plural and collective:
na ke tanum yempahue moheye ke tanum yempahue moheye
1s O plate-PL table-LOC place-PST O plate-PL table-LOC place-PST
I put dishes on the table. Dishes were put on the table.


ACTIVE PASSIVE
* <b>mita</b> - <small>dog</small> - <i>a dog</i>
na ke topa muntaye ke topa ni naku nayo muntaye
** <b>mita[[Kala/affixes#ma|m]]</b> - <small>dog-PL</small> - <i>dogs</i>
1s O bed rearrange-PST O bed PASS sister 1s.GEN rearrange-PST
** <b>[[Kala/affixes#tla|tli]]mita</b> - <small>COL-dog</small> - <i>a dog pack / a pack of dogs</i>
I changed the bed. The bed was changed by my sister.
** <b>tlimitam</b> - <small>COL-dog-PL</small> - <i>dog packs / packs of dogs</i>


ACTIVE PASSIVE
When the final syllable of a word contains an <b>m</b>, <b>mp</b>, and sometimes a <b>p</b> the plural marking changes to <b>-lo</b>.
ha ke mosa yomutli ke mosa ni kola yomutli
3s O book read-FUT O book PASS AG.INDEF read-FUT
She will read the book. The book will be read by someone.


§ 4.1.6) Reflexives & Reciprocals (ki ma anku)
* <b>yama</b> - <small>mountain</small> - <i>a mountain</i>
** <b>yama[[Kala/affixes#la|lo]]</b> - <small>mountain-PL</small> - <i>mountains</i>
** <b>tliyama</b> - <small>COL-mountain</small> - <i>a mountain range / range of mountains</i>
** <b>tliyamalo</b> - <small>COL-mountain-PL</small> - <i>mountain ranges / ranges of mountains</i>


Kala has reflexive and reciprocal pronoun forms as well as the verbal affix –ki (“self; reflexive”) [REFL], and as such reflexive and reciprocal declarative phrases are regular and predictable.
The collective plural is marked by '''tli'''-, derived from '''tatli''', meaning "''group; collection; gathering''". It is mainly used to indicate collectives of animals, but can also indicate groups of flora, geographic features, and various other groupings. This is called the [[wp:Collective_noun|collective]] plural ('''COL''').


na'i sepaye kanku ke onta nayo itsa
=== Gender ===
1s.REFL injure-PST 3pl.RECP O parent 1s.GEN love
I hurt myself. My parents love each other.


ha'i tlela na'anku amyapak
Gender is not normally marked but can be with the endings <b>-na</b> and <b>-ta</b> to mark the feminine and masculine, respectively or nouns such as '''naka''', '''tlaka''', '''nahi''', or '''tahi''' (''the woman, the man, the girl, the boy''), etc. A gender neutral suffix, '''-nta''' may be used when the gender is unknown or ambiguous.
3sg.REFL bathe 1pl.EXCL.RECP like-ABIL-NEG
She bathes herself. We (but not you) are not able to like each other.


In order to differentiate non-singular reflexives from reciprocals, -li (“each; every”) can be added – to the subject for reflexives, and to the object for reciprocals. Note however that this construction usually implies that all members of the subject group were actually affected by the action.
* <b>uma</b> - <small>horse</small> - <i>a horse</i>
** <b>uma[[Kala/affixes#na|na]]</b> - <small>horse-FEM</small> - <i>mare</i>
** <b>uma[[Kala/affixes#ta|ta]]</b> - <small>horse-MASC</small> - <i>stallion</i>


tanakoli matakiye kanku ke tanakoli matakiye
== Pronouns ==
fight-AG-each kill-REFL-PST 3pl.RECP O fight-AG-each kill-REFL-PST
Each one of the warriors killed himself. The warriors killed each other [and nobody survived].


§ 4.2) Complex Sentences (mpukuteye)
'''Kala''' agent pronouns are often omitted when the person is obvious from context. There are four persons in Kala. The 4th being inanimate, or indefinite. The pronoun '''na'am''' is used as the 1st person plural exclusive, meaning "We, but not you." The 3rd person plural is irregular, all other pronoun decline regularly. Pronouns do not inflect for gender; if gender is significant, one can use words like '''naka, tlaka, nahi, tahi''' (''the woman, the man, the girl, the boy''), etc.
{{col-begin}}
{{col-break}}
Personal pronouns:


§ 4.2.1) Clause Coordination (penkuteye)
* '''na''' - 1st person
* '''ta''' - 2nd person
* '''ha''' - 3rd person
* '''tla''' - 4th person ("it", "one") (used for [[wp:Animacy|inanimate]] nouns)
{{col-break}}
Modifiers:


Clause-level conjunctions such as ku (“and; also; too”), ua (“or; either; otherwise”), or ehe (“but; however”) are placed clause-initially. Note that these conjunctions (except for ku) can be used to connect noun phrases.
* [[Kala/affixes#ma|'''-m''']] - plural
* [[Kala/affixes#ka|'''-nku''']] - [[wp:Reciprocal_pronoun|reciprocal]] (only attaches to plural pronouns)
* [[Kala/affixes#a|'''e-''']] - patient
* [[Kala/affixes#a|'''-i''']] - reflexive
* [[Kala/affixes#ya|'''-yo''']] - possessive
{{col-break}}
Other pronouns include:


tahi tohyo ku nahi pina ima kihu saman ehe pakyotlai
* '''tlokua''' - everyone, everybody
boy brave CL.CONJ girl intelligent now weather sun-ADJ however storm-IMM
* '''kola''' - someone, somebody; whomever, anyone, anybody
The boy is brave and the girl is intelligent. Now the weather is sunny, but a storm will come soon.
* '''tlok''' - no one, nobody
* '''nokua''' - everything
* '''nola''' - something; whatever, anything
* '''nok''' - nothing
{{col-end}}


§ 4.2.2) Coordination of noun phrases (no-teye)
{| class="wikitable" style="text-align: center; width: 600px;"
|+ nkalo
|-
!
! Agent
! Patient
! Reflexive
! Possessive
! Reciprocal
|-
! 1sg
| '''na''' || '''ena''' || '''na'i''' || '''nayo''' || '''-'''
|-
! 2sg
| '''ta''' || '''eta''' || '''ta'i''' || '''tayo''' || '''-'''
|-
! 3sg
| '''ha''' || '''eha''' || '''ha'i''' || '''hayo''' || '''-'''
|-
! 4sg
| '''tla''' || '''etla''' || '''tla'i''' || '''tlayo''' || '''-'''
|-
! 1pl<br>1pl exclusive
| '''nam'''<br>'''na'am''' || '''enam'''<br>'''ena'am''' || '''nami'''<br>'''na'ami''' || '''namyo'''<br>'''na'amyo''' || '''nanku'''<br>'''na'anku'''
|-
! 2pl
| '''tam''' || '''etam''' || '''tami''' || '''tamyo''' || '''tanku'''
|-
! 3pl
| '''kam''' || '''ekam''' || '''kami''' || '''kamyo''' || '''kanku'''
|-
! 4pl
| '''tlam''' || '''etlam''' || '''tlami''' || '''tlamyo''' || '''tlanku'''
|}


Non-subject noun phrases are coordinated using the conjunction ma "and" (sometimes "with").
==== Reflexives and Reciprocals ====
'''Kala''' has reflexive and reciprocal pronoun forms.
{{col-begin}}
{{col-break}}
* '''na'i sepaye'''
: <small>1s.REFL injure-PST</small>
: ''I hurt myself.''


yomaye na ke tanka ma pato anya ona ma ota kyosanku
* '''ha'i tlela'''
day-PST 1s O eagle CONJ duck see mother and father fornicate-RECP
: <small>3s.REFL bathe</small>
I saw an eagle and a duck yesterday. Mother and father have sex [with each other].
: ''She bathes herself.''
{{col-break}}
* '''kanku ke onta nayo itsa'''
: <small>3pl.RECP O parent 1s.GEN love</small>
: ''My parents love each other.''


kinti ke tsaka kamyo ma'a yosu sapotle ma siuem muya
* '''na'anku amyapak'''
squirrel O house 3pl.GEN with moss soft-REL and leaf.PL make
: <small>1pl.EXCL.RECP like-ABIL-NEG</small>
The squirrels make their nest comfortable with soft moss and leaves.
: ''We (but not you) are not able to like each other.''
{{col-end}}


ta ma'a na ke molihuelatli
=== Determiners & Demostratives ===
2s with 1s O forest-LOC-MOT-FUT
You and I will go to the forest together.


Noun phrases can be presented as alternatives to each other with the conjunction ua ("or; other"). This conjunction can be used with both subjects and non-subjects. The conjunction ue ("(exclusive) either X or Y") is used to delimit other nouns from the conjunction phrase.
The demonstratives can be prefixed to any noun to show [[Wikipedia:Deixis|deixis]]. Kala makes a three-way distinction. Typically there is a distinction between [[Wikipedia:Demonstrative#Distal_and_proximal_demonstratives|proximal]] or first person (objects near to the speaker), [[Wikipedia:Demonstrative#Distal_and_proximal_demonstratives|medial]] or second person (objects near to the addressee), and [[Wikipedia:Demonstrative#Distal_and_proximal_demonstratives|distal]] or third person (objects far from both).


ta ke nasi ua poma inamyo tsola ue otso itsikua mataye
{{col-begin}}
2s O pear or apple eat-PERM fox either.X.or.Y wolf PROX-bird kill-PST
{{col-break}}
You may eat an apple or a pear. It must have been a fox or a wolf that killed this bird.
* '''itla''' ('''i-''') - this (near me)
* '''uatla''' ('''ua-''') - that (near you)
* '''yetla''' ('''ye-''') - that (over there)


Contrastive coordination of noun phrases is achieved with ehe ("but; however") (or me more informally) if the noun phrases appear in subject position.
Examples:


yomaye mita'u ehek mitana ke kutsu kapya na itlaka mek inaka unya
* '''imitami''' - <small>PROX-dog-few</small> - ''These few dogs''
day-PST dog-MASC but-NEG O meat receive 1s PROX-man but-NEG PROX-woman know
* '''yemitampa''' - <small>DIST-dog-many</small> - ''Those many dogs (over there)''
The male dog but not the female dog I know this man, but not this woman.
* '''uamitali''' - <small>MED-dog-each</small> - ''Each dog (each of those dogs) (near you)''
received meat yesterday.
{{col-break}}
Quantifiers follow the noun that modify.


§ 4.2.3) Complement clauses
* '''kua''' ('''-kua''') - all; every; whole
* '''oli''' ('''-li''') - each; every
* '''ula''' ('''-la''') - whatever; any; some
* '''mi''' ('''-mi''') - few; little
* '''nke''' ('''-k''') - none
* '''mpa''' ('''-mpa''') - many; much; a lot
* '''maha''' - more; plus
* '''ohi''' - less; fewer
{{col-end}}
==== Correlatives ====


Complement clauses, i.e. subordinated clauses acting as a noun (most importantly as the subject or object of a sentence), and are formed exactly like ordinary main clauses, preceded by the particle ke (“topic particle”).
{| class="wikitable" style="text-align: center; width: 800px;"
|+ uatse
|-
!
! Proximal<br>'''i-'''
! Medial<br>'''ua-'''
! Distal<br>'''ye-'''
! Inclusive<br>'''-kua'''
! Negative<br>'''-k'''
! Indefinite<br>'''-la'''
|-
! mo<br>''(place)''
| '''hina'''<br>here || '''uana'''<br>there || '''yemua'''<br>over there || '''mokua'''<br>everywhere || '''mok'''<br>nowhere || '''mola'''<br>somewhere; anywhere
|-
! ko<br>''(person)''
| '''iko'''<br>this person || '''uako'''<br>that person || '''yeko'''<br>that person<br>(over there) || '''tlokua'''<br>everyone || '''tlok'''<br>no one || '''kola'''<br>someone; anyone
|-
! uku<br>''(amount)''
| '''iku'''<br>this much || '''uaku'''<br>that much || '''-''' || '''kua'''<br>all; every|| '''ok'''<br>none || '''ula'''<br>some; any
|-
! ama<br>''(time)''
| '''ima'''<br>now, at present || '''uama'''<br>then; at that time || '''-''' || '''kuama'''<br>always || '''amak'''<br>never || '''tlama'''<br>sometime; anytime
|-
! so<br>''(kind, type)''
| '''iso'''<br>this kind || '''so'o'''<br>that kind || '''yeso'''<br>that kind<br>(over there) || '''sokua'''<br>all kinds || '''sok'''<br>no kind (at all) || '''sola'''<br>some/any kind
|-
! no<br>''(thing)''
| '''itla'''<br>this || '''uatla'''<br>that || '''yetla'''<br>that<br>(over there) || '''nokua'''<br>everything || '''nok'''<br>nothing; none || '''nola'''<br>something; anything
|-
! to<br>''(manner, way)''
| '''yoto'''<br>thus; like this; this way|| '''uato'''<br>that way || '''ato'''<br>that way<br>(over there) || '''tokua'''<br>every way || '''tok'''<br>no way || '''tola'''<br>somehow; anyway
|}


nakkan ke eya tekim kamahi hyoka munaye
== Verbs ==
woman-chief O maybe enemy-PL city-DIM attack worry-PST
The queen was worried that the enemies might attack the village.


itomatle ke maliya noyamya
'''Kala''' relies on analytic serial verb constructions, and can therefore get by with very little verbal morphology. Each verb has at most two possible forms: the [[wp:Dynamic_verb|''active'']] and the [[wp:Stative_verb|''stative'']]. Passivity is marked on the subject thus verbs are unmarked and must be analyzed based on surrounding morphology. Active verbs solely denote actions and occurrences and never states in Kala. Stative verbs are the words that modify nouns in an attributive and often adjectival way. They often express a state like a quality or result. Verbs can be marked with several suffixes to add or change meaning. The modals and tense affixes can be added in different order to a verb to create a new meaning; their placement is not always fixed. The negative, adverbial, and plural endings are always final, while other affixes can be varied, but in general they should be ordered:
wood-carve O Mary happy-CAUS
Carving wood makes Mary happy.


<tt>STEM-(SIZE/IMPORTANCE)-(MOOD)-(ASPECT)-(TENSE)-(NEGATIVE)</tt>


Complement clauses can also act as the object of a motional/locational verb:
Example:


ta ke naha ka’elak yatli ke tlohi kuyepak
{| class="wikitable" style="text-align: center; width: 600px;"
2s O river toward-MOT-NEG if.X.then.Y O salmon grab-ABIL-NEG
|+
You can't catch salmon if you don't go to the river.
|-
! Verb Stem
! Size/Importance
! Mood
! Aspect
! Tense
! Negative
|-
| '''empa''' || '''-hi''' || '''-pa'''  || '''-nko''' || '''-ye''' || '''-k'''
|-
| run || DIM || ABIL || PROG  || PST || NEG
|}


kam ka’e tsiua uahe moku ma ina yalayenko
* '''na empahipankoyek'''
3pl toward lake instead.of rest and eat walk-PST-CONT
: <small>1SG run-DIM-able-PROG-PST-NEG</small>
Instead of taking a rest and eating, they continued to walk towards the lake.
: ''I was not able to keep jogging.''


Since complement clauses behave like nouns syntactically, they may participate in existential constructions as well. Semantically, this indicates that the occurrence of the action described in the complement clause is emphatically affirmed.
=== Tense ===


uala ke yemua tlana masenko
Kala has three simple tenses; past, present, and future. Present tense is unmarked. However, past ('''-ye''') and future ('''-tli''') tenses can be modified to include immediate future ("''is about to''..."), distant future ("''will...in a long while''"), recent past ("''just'' ..."), and remote past ("...''a long while ago''"). These distinctions are made with the augmentative and diminutive endings '''-ha''' and '''-hi'''.
verily O DIST-place person dance-CONT
There is dancing over there. (lit. it's true that people are dancing there)


In addition to the particle ke, Kala possesses a few other words which may fill the same syntactic position under special circumstances. The most common of these appears in the context of reported speech. A couple of other specialized particles have a more limited distribution, appearing mainly in evidential constructions.
{| class="wikitable" style="text-align: center; width: 700px;"
|+ eme
|-
!
! ''Kala''
! ''gloss''
! ''English''
|-align=center
| remote past
| '''kamahi hina<span style="color:red">yeha</span>'''<br>or <span style="color:red">-'''hai'''</span>
| <small>town-DIM be.here-[[wp:Past_tense|REM]]</small>
| ''There was a village here (long ago).<br>''(before the lifetime of the speaker)
|-align=center
| past
| '''naka mita anya<span style="color:red">ye</span>'''
| <small>woman dog see-[[wp:Past_tense|PST]]</small>
| ''The woman saw the dog.''
|-align=center
| recent past
| '''ota namyo akya<span style="color:red">yehi</span>'''<br>or <span style="color:red">-'''hye'''</span>
| <small>father 1pl.GEN wake-[[wp:Past_tense|REC]]</small>
| ''Our father just woke.<br>''(action just finished) 
|-align=center
| present
| '''mita tahi yatsi'''
| <small>dog boy bite</small>
| ''The dog bites the boy.''
|-align=center
| future
| '''naka tahi tlepa<span style="color:red">tli</span>'''
| <small>woman boy teach-[[wp:Future_tense|FUT]]</small>
| ''The woman will teach the boy.''
|-align=center
| immediate future
| '''na tlela<span style="color:red">tlihi</span>'''<br>or <span style="color:red">-'''tlai'''</span>
| <small>1s bathe-[[wp:Future_tense|FUT]]</small>
| ''I'll bathe soon.''<br>(within the day)
|-align=center
| distant future
| '''panam opua<span style="color:red">tliha</span>'''
| <small>rain-PL end-[[wp:Future_tense|FUT]]</small>
| ''The rains will end.''<br>(months from now)
|}


§ 4.2.4) Relative clauses
* The present tense can show immediacy by using the adverb '''ima''', "now; at this time":
:'''ima mita ina''' - <small>now dog eat</small> - ''The dog is eating right now.''


Relative clauses, i.e. subordinated clauses acting as an attribute to a noun phrase, are marked with the relativizer -tle (or -le if the last syllable has tl). A pronoun referring to the relativized noun is retained within the relative clause:
* If a temporal adverb is used, the tense suffix may be omitted:
:'''yomaye nam ina''' - <small>yesterday 1pl eat</small> - We ate yesterday.


na ke naka amyatle pesoue kam tananitle ke teki tlalitli
=== Aspect ===
1s O woman liked-REL meet-VOL 3pl fight-nice-REL O enemy defeat-FUT
I want to meet a girl who is friendly. They who fight well will defeat the enemy.


naku nayo ke yakokua na tikuyetle inapua
There are four aspects in Kala. The progressive, also called the [[Wikipedia:Continuous_and_progressive_aspects|continuous]] ['''CONT'''], this is used to express an incomplete action or state in progress at a specific time. It is marked with '''-nko''', from '''nkoso''' - "''to continue; proceed; progress''". The [[Wikipedia:Perfective_aspect|perfective]] aspect indicates that an action is completed ['''PFV''']. It is often translated by the English present perfect (''have done some-thing''). It is marked with '''-pua''', from '''opua''' - "''to end; finish; complete''". The [[Wikipedia:Inchoative_aspect|inchoative]] aspect refers to the beginning of a state ['''INCH''']. It is marked with '''-mu''', from '''mula''' - "''to begin; start; initiate''". The [[Wikipedia:Frequentative|frequentative]]  aspect refers to a repeated action ['''FREQ''']. It is marked with '''-nua''', from '''nua''' - "''frequent; often; regular''".
sister 1s.GEN O strawberry-all 1s pick-PST-REL eat-PFV
My sister has eaten all the strawberries that I picked.


If both subject and object of a transitive relative clause are represented by the same pronoun, the relativized noun will be assumed to be the subject of the subclause. In order to relativize the object in such a situation, the subject must be represented by one of the reflexive, or reciprocal pronouns instead, which explicitly refer back to the subject of the matrix clause:
{| class="wikitable" style="text-align: center; width: 700px;"
!
! ''Kala''
! ''gloss''
! ''English''
|-align=center
| Continuous
| '''na ke niye puku<span style="color:red">nko</span>'''
| <small>1s O undergarment wear-[[wp:Continuous_and_progressive_aspects|CONT]]</small>
| ''I am wearing underclothes.''
|-align=center
| Frequentative
| '''tlaka ke apua tlato<span style="color:red">nua</span>'''
| <small>man O song recite-[[wp:Frequentative|FREQ]]</small>
| ''The man recites the song repetitively.''
|-align=center
| Inchoative
| '''nahi yoti<span style="color:red">mu</span>'''
| <small>girl play-[[wp:Inchoative_aspect|INCH]]</small>
| ''The girl begins to play.''
|-align=center
| Perfective
| '''kam ina<span style="color:red">pua</span>'''
| <small>3pl eat-[[wp:Perfective_aspect|PFV]]</small>
| ''They have eaten.''
|}


aye tanako ke tlaka eha hyokatle mata
=== Mood ===
past fight-AG O man P.3s attack-REL kill
The warrior killed the man who attacked him.


aye tanako ke ha tlaka hyokatle mata
Besides various aspects, '''Kala''' also marks moods other than [[wp:Realis_mood|realis]]: irrealis, imperative, hortative, and negative. These are also expressed by suffixes on the verb and typically follow aspectual marking where it is expressed by a suffix. As is common throughout natural and constructed languages, the indicative mood is unmarked. Subjunctive, conditional, and imperative moods are marked lexically, by various particles, and as such are covered in detail in the “particles” of this grammar.
past fight-AG O 3s man attack-REL kill
The warrior killed the man whom he attacked.


Relativization of oblique participants works very much the same way as relativization of subjects and objects, but the repeated pronoun needs to appear inside a prepositional phrase or coverb phrase which shows the role of the relativized noun within the subclause.
{| class="wikitable" style="text-align: center; width: 900px;"
!
! ''Kala''
! ''gloss''
! ''English''
|-align=center
| Abilitative
| '''na mokuye<span style="color:red">pa</span>k'''
| <small>1s sleep-PST-[[wp:Natchez_language#Preverbs|ABIL]]-NEG</small>
| ''I was unable to sleep.''
|-align=center
| Assumptive
| '''naka hina<span style="color:red">ho</span>'''
| <small>woman be.here-[[wp:Assumptive_mood|ASS]]</small>
| ''The woman must be here. (I assume)'' (also used as "assertive")
|-align=center
| Attemptative
| '''neko ke panya mata<span style="color:red">pya</span>'''
| <small>cat O mouse kill-ATT</small>
| ''The cat is trying to kill the mouse.''
|-align=center
| Dubitative
| '''ha tsakahue<span style="color:red">ke</span>'''
| <small>3s home-LOC-[[wp:Irrealis_mood#Dubitative|DUB]]</small>
| ''I guess he is at home.'' lit: ''He is at home, supposedly.''
|-align=center
| Hortative
| '''yala<span style="color:red">kya</span>'''
| <small>walk-[[wp:Hortative|HORT]]</small>
| ''Let's go!''
|-align=center
| Necessitative
| '''mita ina<span style="color:red">he</span>'''
| <small>dog eat-[[wp:Irrealis_mood#Necessitative|NEC]]</small>
| ''The dog needs to eat.''
|-align=center
| Negative
| '''naku nayo hina<span style="color:red">k</span>'''
| <small>sister 1s-GEN be.here-[[wp:Affirmative_and_negative|NEG]]</small>
| ''My sister is not here.''
|-align=center
| Permissive
| '''ta ke hina sima<span style="color:red">myo</span>k'''
| <small>2s O here sit-[[wp:Irrealis_mood#Permissive|PERM]]-NEG</small>
| ''You are not allowed to sit here.''
|-align=center
| Precative
| '''ke asi yeta<span style="color:red">te</span>'''
| <small>O salt give-[[wp:Irrealis_mood#Precative|PREC]]</small>
| ''Will you please pass me the salt?''
|-align=center
| Preparative
| '''tahi moku<span style="color:red">sue</span>'''
| <small>boy sleep-PREP</small>
| ''The boy is ready to sleep.''
|-align=center
| Propositive
| '''ta moku<span style="color:red">ne</span>'''
| <small>2s sleep-[[wp:Propositive_mood|PROP]]</small>
| ''You should sleep.''
|-align=center
| Volitive
| '''otsokai ka'e moli yala<span style="color:red">ue</span>'''
| <small>wolf-red toward forest go-[[wp:Irrealis_mood#Volitive|VOL]]</small>
| ''Redwolf wants/intends to go to the forest.''
|}


ikamahi ena nasayetle
PROX-city-DIM P.1s be.born-PST-REL
This is the village in which I was born.


ke taku tlakayo na tayayetle nya teki matapua
The negative mood (always marked finally) is indicated by the suffix –'''k''' or '''–nke''' (when the last syllable contains /k/).
O brother man-GEN 1s wed-PST-REL by enemy kill-PFV
The man whose brother I married has been killed by the enemy.


iyoma ke yomatle ta’ena nya uapa talaue
* '''mita inayek''' - <small>dog eat-PST-NEG</small> - ''The dog did not eat.''
today O day-REL 2s-P.1s for visit come-VOL
* '''mita mokunke''' - <small>dog sleep-NEG</small> - ''The dog does not sleep.''
Today is the day on which you want to come and visit me.


In case a relative clause would contain only the subject and an intransitive verb phrase, speakers of Kala are likely to use an attributive construction instead. If the attributed verb phrase contains more than one verb, all of them need to appear in the attributive form.
== Adjectives ==


na ke nahi amyan pesoue
Kala does not have morphologically distinct adjectives. Stative verbs are the words that modify nouns in an predicative and often adjectival way. They often express a state like a quality or result.  In the simplest form, the adjective simply appears after the noun, in verbal position. Many statements that would be phrased as adjectival predicates in English are preferably expressed with stative intransitive verbs in '''Kala''', requiring no copula. (For simplicity, such verbs are glossed without “be”.) This leaves open to interpretation many phrases.
1s O girl like-ADV meet-VOL
I want to meet a girl who is friendly. (lit. a friendly girl)


kola sima ke ina kapyatli
{{Col-begin}}
AG-INDEF sit O food receive-FUT
{{Col-break}}
Anyone who is sitting quietly gets food.
* '''mita inya''' - <small>dog hungry</small>
** ''The dog hungers.''
** ''The dog is hungry.''
** ''The hungry dog.''
** ''A hungry dog.''
{{Col-break}}
* '''tsaka ketlahi''' - <small>house red-DIM</small>
** ''The house is a little red.''
** ''The light-red house.''
** ''A pale red house.''
{{Col-break}}
* '''taki saua''' - <small>coat wet</small>
** ''The coat is wet.''
** ''The wet coat.''
** ''A wet coat.''
{{Col-break}}
* '''umalo tahaku''' - <small>horse-PL big-extreme</small>
** ''The horses are extremely large.''
** ''The very big horses.''
{{Col-end}}


Note that both relative clauses and attributive constructions tend to be avoided when they refer to the subject of a sentence. Instead, the semantically ‘attributive’ verb describing the subject is treated syntactically as forming a sequential or simultaneous event together with the main verb of the sentence:
=== Comparison ===


tahi pina ke kema unya
In [[Kala]] the concepts of comparative and superlative degree of an predicative adjective (verb) are merged into a single form, the [[Wikipedia:Elative_(gradation)|elative]]. How this form is understood or translated depends upon context and definiteness. In the absence of comparison, the elative conveys the notion of “greatest”, “supreme.” The comparative is made by using the augmentative or diminutive ending on the verb.
boy smart O task understand
{{col-begin}}
The smart boy understands the task. (lit. the boy is smart and understands the task)
{{col-break}}
§ 4.2.5) Adverbial clauses
* '''tsaka hayo ke nayo tahaka'''
: <small>house 3sg.POSS O 1sg.POSS big-AUG</small>
: ''His house is bigger than mine.''
{{col-break}}
* '''ke mauam tayo yanaha'''
: <small>O flower.PL 2sg.POSS yellow-AUG</small>
: ''Your flowers are the most yellow.''
{{col-break}}
* '''iyapo ke tsaka tayo pakoha'''
: <small>PROX-building O home 2sg new-AUG</small>
: ''This building is newer than your home.''
{{col-end}}


Kala has several different ways to express adverbial elements – adverbial suffix, adpositional phrases, serial verb constructions, and full-scale adverbialized subclauses. For most types of adverbials, more than one of these methods can be used. Because an adequate description of this gets rather lengthy, and because it presupposes an understanding of how serial verb constructions work in Kala, it is described in a later section of this document. Adverbial constructions which are valid constituents typically appear near the beginning of a sentence, with adpositional phrases preceding subclauses, but they may be topic-fronted for emphasis. If several adverbial constituents of the same syntactic type are present, they are generally ordered place → manner → reason → purpose → result → time.
=== Equivalence ===


§ 3) Morphology (umpu)
Equivalence is indicated with either '''kue''' (as, like), or '''mya''' (as...as).
{{col-begin}}
{{col-break}}
* '''tsaka hayo kue nayo ketla'''
: <small>house 3s.GEN as 1s.GEN be.red</small>
: ''His house is red like mine.''
{{col-break}}
* '''tsaka hayo mya nayo ketla'''
: <small>house 3s.GEN as.X.as 1s.GEN be.red</small>
: ''His house is as red as mine.''
{{col-end}}


Kala is a mostly agglutinative language that makes extensive use of compounding, incorporation and derivation. That is, it can add many different prefixes and suffixes to a root until very long words are formed, and a single word can sometimes constitute an entire sentence. The words of Kala can be divided into two basic functional classes: verbs and nouns as content words, and particles and others as functional. Adjectives exist, but they generally behave like verbs and there are very few adjectives that are not derived from either verbal or nominal roots. The few adverbs that exist fall into the class of particles or are derived from verbs. The most important element of Kala lexemes to keep in mind is that they may function as a verb, noun, adjective, or an adverb based on where they fall in the phrase, and any various endings that may be affixed.
Like verbs, adjectives can be used as nouns. For example, '''aya''' means "''beautiful''", but '''ayako''' means "''a beautiful one''" or "''a beauty''." An adjective can be made into an abstract noun by adding -'''n''' (''-ity, -ness, -ship, -hood''). In this way '''aya''' becomes '''ayan''', meaning "''beauty''". This can also be used with nouns: '''ona''' (mother) becomes '''onan''' (''motherhood'').


§ 3.1) Verbal morphology (uati)
==== Relative ====


§ 3.1.1) Copular Verbs (uati te a)
In a relative clause, the verb has the suffix '''-tle''' (or '''-le''' if the final syllable contains /tl/) added to it. The order of the words in relative clauses remains the same as in regular clauses. The use of participles in Kala is rather different than in English and at first sight is difficult to understand. This is mainly due to the fact that the relative pronouns ''who, what, which, where'' are not used in Kala as in English.


The copula a (to be; exist; yes) is not used as it is in English. It is primarily used to affirm Yes/No questions. ta inaye ka (Did you eat?) a (Yes.) However, it can be used to add emphasis or nuance to a descriptive phrase. In an adjectival predicate the verb [to be] is not normally used.
* '''yalapa''' - ''to be able to walk'' produces: '''yalapatle''' - ''who/which/that can walk''
* '''yalapak''' - ''to not be able to walk'' produces: '''yalapanketle''' - ''who/which/that can't walk''
{{col-begin}}
{{col-break}}
This nominalizes the verb in some cases, and makes it possible for it to be either the subject or the object.


ha kiha tomua nya itsaka yasue
* '''na ke tlaka nya inama talatle unya'''
3s tall rent for PROX-house cheap
: <small>1sg O man for eat-time come-REL know</small>
She is tall. The rent for this house is cheap.
: ''I know the man who is coming to lunch.''


§ 3.1.2) Tense (eme)
* '''ke naka patlole pako'''
: <small>O woman sweep-REL young</small>
: ''The woman who is sweeping is young.''
{{col-break}}
The relative suffix is most often in the final position. In some cases, it may be followed by the negative '''-k'''.


Kala does not always mark tense, especially when it can be inferred from context. However, Kala verbs can be marked for past and future tenses. Both past and future can be marked with augmentative and diminutive suffixes to add further layers of nuance, i.e. recent and remote past, as well as immediate and distant future tenses. Kala’s distinguishing three levels of both past and future time is a unique typological trait. The use of the variations of past and future are not subject to strict grammatical rules and are a question of pragmatics. The recent and immediate markers are most commonly used for near-scope, that is, things which have just happened or will happen very soon. Of the triad tense–aspect–mood this section will only cover basic uses of the marked tense categories, followed by a discussion of complex tense combinations such as past-in-future. The subsequent section will provide more insight into the morphological marking of aspectual categories; and the following section deals with the morphology of mood marking in Kala.
* '''itsaka na sutahuetle'''
: <small>PROX-house 1sg reside-LOC-REL</small>
: ''This is the house in which I live.''


Verbs in Kala are unmarked for present tense, as it is the normal mode of speaking. Besides being used to comment or report on current events, the present tense is also used to make statements of general truth. Also, Kala does not strictly mark its verbs for past tense in narrative discourses (instead the adverbial aye (“it was”) will start the first phrase); verbs may therefore appear as a present-time reference in spite of recounting past events, whether historical or fictional.
* '''itsaka na sutahueyetlek'''
: <small>PROX-house 1sg reside-LOC-PST-REL-NEG</small>
: ''This is the house in which I did not live.''
{{col-end}}


The present tense can show immediacy by using the adverb ima, "now; at this time":
== Adverbs ==


ima mita ina
Adverbs tell us when, how, why or where the action happens. They modify a verb, a noun, an adjective, another adverb or a complete sentence. They also can provide us information about manner, quantity, frequency, time, or place. Kala does not have morphologically distinct adverbs. Adverbs can be formed from all adjectives (or stative verbs) by adding '''-n''' to the root. Since this rule is regular, it is not generally indicated in grammatical examples or in the lexicon.
now dog eat
The dog is eating right now.


If a temporal adverb is used, the tense suffix may be omitted:
* '''aya''' - beautiful >> '''ayan''' - beautifully
* '''tama''' - good >> '''taman''' - well
* '''poyo''' - rich >> '''poyon''' - richly
* '''tsipue''' - slow >> '''tsipuen''' - slowly (<small>this can also be marked on the main verb with '''-tsue'''</small>)
* '''tlaki''' - fluent >> '''tlakin''' - fluently


yomaye nam ina
Many adverbs (mostly temporal) do not derive from verbs:
yesterday 1pl eat
We ate yesterday.


The past tense indicates actions in the past if not further modified. Past tense [PST] is marked by –ye from aye (“it was”) and indicates a general sense that the action occurred before the present.
* '''yomaye''' - yesterday
* '''iyoma''' - today
* '''yomali''' - every day
* '''kuama''' - always
* '''ima''' - now


na’eha anyaye
Temporal adverbs always precede the phrase they modify.
1s-P.3s see-PST
I saw her.


The recent past [REC] is marked by –yehi or -hye (“it just was”) and indicates that the action just finished.
* '''yomuali na ka'e hakyo yala'''
: <small>morning-each 1s to school go</small>
: ''I go to school every morning.''


ha ke tsunku tanyayehi / tanyahye
Other adverbials can be marked on the verb.
3s O pot break-PST-DIM / break-REC
He just broke the pot.


The remote past [REM] is marked by –yeha  (“a long while ago”) and indicates the action took place before the lifetime of the speaker or at least several years prior.
* '''ona kamyo ma'a siku kupayetsua'''
: <small>mother 3pl.GEN with accident die-PST-almost</small>
: ''Their mother almost died in the accident.''


kam hinahue asayeha
== Prepositions ==
3pl here-LOC live-REM (PST-AUG)
They lived here (a long while ago).


Note that the recent and the remote past tense are generally not marked if the past context is clear, for instance, when a past context has already been established in discourse. This may also happen explicitly by using a temporal adverbial such as yomaye (“yesterday”) or anyoye nye’o (“a hundred years ago”). In the presence of an explicit time adverbial, redundant tense marking is also dropped subsequently:
Kala does not have prepositions (or postpositions) as a distinct part of speech. Instead, many locative verbs can be used as adpositionals, in which case they precede the noun they modify. There is one general locative (-'''hue''') which is affixed to nouns (and occasionally verbs) to indicate the sense of “at; in; on”. Here are some common verbs used as [[wp:Preposition_and_postposition|adpositions]]:
{{col-begin}}
{{col-break}}
* '''pahe''' - against; touching
* '''pa'e''' - apart from; other than; except for
* '''paye''' - beyond; exceeding; farther than
* '''pue''' - after; back; behind; rear
* '''tahe''' - below; beneath; under
* '''ka'e''' - to; towards; at [moving toward]
* '''kaye''' - around; encircling; surrounding
* '''mahe''' - around; approximate; close to
* '''ma'a''' - with [accompanied by / furnished with]
* '''ma'e''' - before; in front
* '''maye''' - between; among
{{col-break}}
* '''nahe ''' - in [located inside of]; internal
* '''nyaue''' - outside of; exterior to
* '''sahe''' - across; opposite; other side
* '''saye''' - along; following [a line]
* '''hue / -hue''' - at [in the same location as] [LOC]
* '''tsa'e''' - across; through
* '''ua'e''' - above; over / on
* '''uaye''' - from [moving out of or away from]
* '''ya'e''' - near; close to
* '''yomo''' - to the right of
* '''yoso''' - to the left of
{{col-end}}


semaye na’am ma’a anku ina
* '''na ke ito yamahue anyapa'''
week-PST 1pl.EXCL with each.other eat
: <small>1sg O tree hill-LOC see-ABIL</small>
We had lunch together last week.
: ''I can see a tree on the hill.''


The reference to the past is explicitly stated in the above example by the adverbial semaye (“last week”), so the verb here appears simply as ina (“eat”) rather than including the redundant past tense marking of –ye.
* '''ntahim nyaue tsaka yoti'''
: <small>child-PL outside.of house play</small>
: ''The children are playing outside of the house.''


Future tense [FUT] is marked by –tli from atli (“it will be”) and indicates a general sense that the action will occur sometime after the present. It explicitly references to future time in Kala, that is, “someone’s plans, intentions or obligations”, as well as predictions. The future suffixes behave analogously to the ones indicating past tense. The following examples show the future tense markers in context:
Many of these take the motive suffix '''-la'''.


nam nyahihue yelotli
* '''mita ke tsaka nahelaye'''
1pl snow-LOC freeze-FUT
: <small>dog O house go.into-PST</small>
We will freeze in the snow.
: ''The dog went into the house.''


The immediate future tense [IMM] is marked by –tlihi or -tlai (“is about to”) and indicates that the action will occur within the day, or sooner.
* '''taku nayo ke ito ua'ela'''
: <small>brother 1s.GEN O tree go.up</small>
: ''My brother is climbing the tree.''


na talatlihi / talatlai
== Particles ==
1s come-FUT-DIM / come-IMM
I’m coming. (in a moment)


The distant future [DIS] is marked by –tliha (“will...in a long while”) and indicates the action will take place months or years from the present.
=== Conjunctions ===


kam ke naku kamyo talatliha haua
Words and phrases may be coordinated in Kala with the following words:
3pl O sister 3pl.GEN return-DIS (FUT-AUG) believe
{{col-begin}}
They believe that their sister will return. (one day)
{{col-break}}
* '''pa''' - although; even though; even if
* '''ku''' - and; also [clause level]
* '''ma''' - and; also; too; as well
* '''ehe''' / ('''me''') - but; yet; however
* '''impo''' - therefore; as a result; so; consequently; thus
* '''ua''' - or; other; else
* '''ue''' - either X or Y
* '''uenke''' / ('''uek''') - neither X nor Y
* '''yatli''' - if X then Y
* '''yema''' - both X and Y
* '''yetli''' - if it were not; if not X then Y >> X '''yatli''' Y
{{col-break}}
* '''ha'ena itsa ehe hinak'''
: <small>3s-P.1s love but be.here-NEG</small>
: ''She loves me but is not here.''


Like the past tense, the future is often not explicitly marked if the time frame is clear from context or has been clarified with such adverbials as “tomorrow”;
* '''aye na tala ku matsu'''
: <small>PST 1s come CONJ conquer</small>
: ''I came, I conquered.''
{{col-end}}


yomatli na’eta anya
=== Interjections ===
day-FUT.(tomorrow) 1s-P.2s see
There are a few particles, usually appearing at the beginning of the sentence, with a pragmatic meaning. These typically precede phrases they modify.
I will see you tomorrow.


It is possible here to explicitly mark the verb for future tense as well, for example, to make a promise, or to otherwise emphasize that the future condition will come to pass:
{| class="wikitable" style="text-align: center; width: 900px;"
|-
! morpheme !! indicates !! gloss !! example
|-
| '''a''' || acknowledgement, agreement, or that one is listening || ''yes; hm mm; yeah'' || '''a ta inaue'''<br>''Mm hmm...You want to eat.''
|-
| '''e''' || filler or pause during conversation || ''uh, er, well'' || '''e na uamek'''<br>''Well, I'm not sure.''
|-
| '''yali''' || excuses jostling or interruptions || ''excuse me'' || '''yali itla tayo ka'''<br>''Excuse me, is this yours?''
|}


ya ueta, sematli na’etla tamitli
=Derivational morphology=
VOC uncle week-FUT 1s-P.4s repair-FUT
I will fix it next week, uncle.


“Already”, past in past & past in future
Because '''Kala''' has only two main parts of speech ([[wp:Content_word|content]] and [[wp:function_word|functional words]]), new words formed by derivation should be analyzed based on context. [[wp:Function_word|Functional]] words can rarely be used to form new words, but this is typically to form extensions of functions, or new functions.


So far, we have only dealt with tense marking from the point of view of the present. However, it is also possible to refer to an event which precedes another event in the past. Kala uses the particle tso (already; since) to indicate actions that took place prior to the primary tense of the verb. It is most often placed at the beginning of a verb phrase.
== Compounding ==


tso mikelo yetla inaye tso maliya yetla inatli ama nam talatli
New nouns are usually created through head-initial compounding, using both nominal and verbal stems as the second, dependent element of the compound. The resulting lexical entries usually behave as single phonological words, which, however, have four full syllables: '''kuatlatloha''' "grass snake". Compounding of more than two elements is not common.
already Michael DIST-4s eat-PST already Mary DIST-4s eat-FUT time 1pl arrive-FUT
Michael already ate that (before). Mary will have already eaten that (before) when we (will) arrive.


§ 3.1.3) Aspect (ti’a)
* '''kayapusu''' - "earthquake" > '''kaya''' - earth + '''pusu''' - vibrate
* '''asuaseka''' - "leather" > '''asua''' - skin + '''seka''' - dry


There are four aspects in Kala. The progressive [PROG], also called the continuous [CONT], this is used to express an incomplete action or state in progress at a specific time. It is marked with -nko, from nkoso - "to continue; proceed; progress". This aspect could also be analyzed as the imperfective in that it can easily be combined with tense markers to add nuance.
There are also numerous [[Kala/affixes|'''affixes''']] used to form new meanings. A few examples are;


kyolon ha antankoye
* '''tiyasu''' - "bakery" > '''tiya''' - bread + '''-su''' - market; shop
be.quick-ADV 3s breathe-CONT-PST
* '''onyomo''' - "school" > '''onyo''' - learn + '''-mo''' - place; location
She was breathing quickly.
* '''kuhasa''' - "kitchen" > '''kuha''' - cook + '''-sa''' - room; chamber
* '''pyetampu''' - "egg-shaped" > '''pyeta''' - egg + '''-mpu''' - shape; form


The perfective aspect indicates that an action is completed [PFV]. It is often translated by the English present perfect (have done some-thing). It is marked with -pua, from opua - "to end; finish; complete".  Perfective forms are used for completed actions, no matter what time they occur.
== Derivation ==


na mita kamyo anyapua
=== Verbalization ===
1s dog 3pl.GEN see-PFV
I’ve seen their dog.


Causative verbs (as well as achievement verbs) can be formed from other verbs by adding [[Kala/affixes#mya|-'''mya''']] (from [[Kala/lexicon#ma|'''muya''']] - ''"do, make, cause"'') or [[Kala/affixes#la|-'''la''']] (from [[Kala/lexicon#a|'''ela''']] - ''"become; change into; turn into"''). This type of derivation is fairly common; however, verbs created in this way are syntactically defective and tend to appear only in serial verb constructions.


* '''tinamya''' - "bend" < '''tina''' - be bent
* '''pitamya''' - "hollow out" < '''pita''' - be hollow; void
* '''enomya''' - "annoy, bother" < '''eno''' - be angry
* '''tsipuela''' - "slow down" < '''tsipue''' - be slow
* '''kyolola''' - "speed up" < '''kyolo''' - be quick


The inchoative aspect refers to the beginning of a state [INCH]. It is marked with -mu, from mula - "to begin; start; initiate".  
Intensive verbs can be formed from other verbs by adding [[Kala/affixes#mpa|-'''mpa''']] (from [[Kala/lexicon#mpa|'''mpa''']] - ''"many; much; very"''), or more commonly [[Kala/affixes#hu|-'''hu''']] (from [[Kala/lexicon#ka|'''kyohu''']] - ''"be drastic; extreme; aggressive"'').


tso ha apuamu
* '''ketsahu''' - "dismiss, reject, repudiate" < '''ketsa''' - doubt
already 3s sing-INCH
* '''amyampa''' - "fall in love with" < '''amya''' - be fond of; like; prefer (of people)
She already began to sing.


The frequentative aspect refers to a repeated action [FREQ]. It is marked with -nua, from nua - "frequent; often; regular".
Adjective-like stative verbs which name an associated quality may be formed from nouns by [[Kala/affixes#n|-'''n''']] (from [[Kala/lexicon#na|'''no''']] - ''"thing" (-ish, -ly, -ous)'').


tahi topunuaye
* '''timan''' - "be cruel, be bloody" < '''tima''' - blood
boy jump-FREQ-PST
* '''amyan''' - "be welcoming, be hospitable" < '''amya''' - be fond of
The boy was jumping up and down.


§ 3.1.4) Mood (toka)
=== Nominalization ===


==== Agentive ====


Abilitative mood
Nouns referring to a human subject of a verb (''usually in a habitual sense'') can be formed with the agentive suffix [[Kala/affixes#ko|-'''ko''']] (from [[Kala/lexicon#ka|'''ko''']] - ''"individual; person"''). This suffix changes to '''-tlo''' when a [[wp:Velar_stop|velar stop]] is present in the preceding syllable.


The abilitative mood [ABIL] expresses ability to accomplish, and positive potential occurrences.  It is marked with -pa, from pala - "to be able; can ~; ability".
* '''kitlako''' - "craftsman" < '''kitla''' - create; invent; make-up
* '''sutako''' - "inhabitant (of)" < '''suta''' - live; reside; dwell; inhabit; settle
* '''yekatlo''' - "unmarried young adult" < '''yeka''' - be separate, be on one's own
* '''makatlo''' - "musician" < '''maka''' - music; play ~; tune
* '''tsaniko''' - "storyteller" < '''tsani''' - recite, tell (a story)


kam yalapa ha ke mita anyapayek
==== Instrumental ====
3pl walk-ABIL 3s O dog see-ABIL-PST-NEG
They are able to walk. She couldn’t see the dog.


Assumptive mood
Instrument nouns and names for tools and other inanimates can be derived from verbs or from other nouns by adding the suffix [[Kala/affixes#nyo|-'''nyo''']] (from [[Kala/lexicon#ma|'''mayo''']] - ''"device; equipment; tool"'').


The assumptive mood [ASS] indicates that the statement is assumed to be true, because it usually is under similar circumstances, although there may not be any specific evidence that it is true in this particular case.  It is marked with -ho, from toho – ("claim; assert that ~; assume").
* '''hitanyo''' - "atlatl (spear-thrower)" < '''hita''' - throw; cast; expel
* '''amonyo''' - "handle (for carrying)" < '''amo''' - transport; carry
* '''kusunyo''' - "clasp, brooch, fibula" < '''kusu''' - squeeze
* '''toponyo''' - "lock" < '''topo''' - door; gate


ueta namyo hinaho
==== Locative ====
uncle 1pl.GEN be.here-ASS
Our uncle must be here.
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
Attemptive mood
 
The attemptive mood [ATT] expresses an attempt to accomplish.  It is marked with -pya, from upya - "to try; attempt".
 
na’am nya uatsi inahahue tasapyaye
1pl.EXCL for fish PROX-river-LOC hunt-ATT-PST
We tried to fish in this river.
 
Dubitative mood
 
The dubitative mood [DUB] indicates that the statement is dubious, doubtful, or uncertain.  It is marked with -ke, from ketsa - "doubt; suspect; suspicion".
 
ha yetloke mita nya pyolato unyake
3s be.malnourished-DUB dog for roll-way know-DUB
She must be sick. It is doubtful the dog knows how to roll-over.
(I guess she's sick / maybe she's sick / she might be sick, but it is doubtful)
 
Hortative mood
 
The hortative mood [HORT] expresses encouragement, or urges.  It is marked with -kya, from kya - "the imperative particle".
 
inakya hakyohue mokukyanke
eat-HORT school-LOC sleep-HORT-NEG
Let’s eat! Let’s not sleep at school.
 
Necessitative mood
 
The necessitative mood [NEC] expresses requirement, necessity, insistence, imploring, self-encouragement, intent, command, purpose or consequence.  It is marked with -he, from heya - "to need; require".
 
nam mokuhe tlenoko ke tseuem mahahe
1pl sleep-NEC wood-AG O nail more-NEC
We need (to) sleep. The carpenter needs more nails.
 
Negative mood
 
The negative mood [NEG] (always marked finally) is indicated by the suffix –k or –nke (when the last syllable contains /k/).  This mood also indicates an abessive case [ABE].
 
mita inayek ueta kamyo takanke
dog eat-PST-NEG uncle 3pl.GEN honor-ABE
The dog did not eat. Their uncle is dishonor(ed/able).
 
 
 
Permissive mood
 
The permissive mood [PERM] indicates that the action is permitted by or for the speaker. It is marked by  –myo from myonta “permit; allow”.
 
ena inamyoyek nameha nahelamyo
P.1s eat-PERM-PST-NEG 1pl-P.3s inside-MOT-PERM
I was not allowed to eat. We permit her to enter.
 
Precative mood
 
The precative mood [PREC] expresses a request or proposition. It can sometimes be translated as “please”.  It is marked with -te, from teya - "propose; request; suggest". The interrogative particle ka can be dropped when the precative is used. This makes the phrase a more polite request.
 
ta ke asi yetate akate
2s O salt give-PREC move.slightly-PREC
Will you please pass the salt? Please, move.
(lit: You pass the salt, please) (just a bit less polite than “excuse me”)
 
Preparative mood
 
The preparative mood [PREP] expresses readiness or anticipation.  It is marked with -sue, from yaso - "prepare; be ready".
 
na inasuek yalasue ka
1s eat-PREP-NEG go-PREP Q
I am not ready to eat. (You) ready to go?
 
Propositive mood
 
The propositive mood [PROP] expresses proposals or suggestions.  It is marked with -ne, from neya - "ought to; suggest".
 
ta inane ke itanum apyapuane
2s eat-PROP O PROX-plate-PL clean-PFV-PROP
You should eat. These dishes should have been cleaned.
 
Volitive mood
 
The volitive mood [VOL] expresses wants, desires, hopes, and intentions.  It is marked with -ue, from ueha - "want; desire; wish" and/or ueyo - "intend; intention".
 
taku nayo hinauek nahim ka’e kamahi yalaueye
brother 1s.GEN be.here-VOL-NEG girl-PL toward city-DIM go-VOL-PST
My brother doesn’t want to be here. The girls intended to go toward the village.
 
 
§ 3.2) Nouns (noma)
 
 
§ 3.2.3) Size & Importance (manyo ma muna)
 
Diminutive
 
Diminutive nouns and endearment terms can be formed from verbs and other nouns by adding the suffix -hi (from ahi - "few; small") [DIM]. This becomes -ki after a syllable that contains a glottal fricative.
 
mukuhi - "blade" < muku - knife
umahi - "foal" < uma - horse; equine
mitahi - "puppy" < mita - dog; canine
ohuaki - "indulgence" < ohua - luxurious; extravagant
 
This is also used to denote an offspring, younger sibling, or descendent in familial words.
 
hyata nayo ke nekohi nomok takuhi kamyo hinayeha
grandfather 1s.GEN O cat-DIM like-NEG brother-DIM3pl.GEN be.here-REM
My grandfather doesn’t like kittens. Their little brother was here (a long while ago).
 
Augmentative
 
Augmentative nouns can be formed from verbs and other nouns by adding the suffix -ha (from taha - "big; large; grand") [AUG]. This becomes -ka after a syllable that contains a glottal fricative.
 
kamaha - "city" < kama - village; town
ohaka - "dislocate one's jaw" < oha - yawn; open one's mouth
mosaha - "epic; novel" < mosa - book; letter; scroll
tiniha - "hurricane" < tini - spiral; whorl
 
There is also -hu (from kyohu - “be drastic; extreme; aggressive”) [EXT], occasionally used to describe the superlative in comparison. This becomes -ku after a syllable that contains a glottal fricative.
 
imita ke na anyapuatle kapihu
PROX-dog O 1s see-PFV-REL ugly-EXT
This dog is the ugliest I’ve seen.
 
The diminutive and augmentative are also used in making comparisons (covered in detail later in this grammar).
 
ha kiha > 3s tall > She is tall.
ha ke takuhi nayo kihaka > 3s O brother-DIM 1s.GEN tall-AUG > She is taller than my little brother.
ha ke tanko hayo kihaku > 3s O team 3s.GEN tall-EXT > She is the tallest on her team.
ha kihaku > 3s tall-EXT > She is the tallest.
 
§ 3.3) Pronouns (nkalo)
 
Kala generally distinguishes four persons, the fourth person indicating abstract and inanimate nouns – both in the singular and plural numbers. There is also a distinction between inclusive (I/we and you) and exclusive (EXCL) (we but not you) forms of the first person plural.
 
The chart above demonstrates that the nominative/agentive (NOM/A) and absolute forms of the pronouns are identical.
 
-m marks the plural (PL)
-nku marks the reciprocal (RECP)
e- marks the patient (accusative) (P)
-i marks the reflexive (REFL)
-yo marks the possessive or genitive (GEN)
 
Pronominal constructions
 
The agent and patient pronouns are linked in most constructions. That means that the agent and the patient form one word. This is done with the pronominal patient marking affix -e-.
 
na’eha anya kameta motoyek
1s-P.3s see 3pl-P.2s remember-PST-NEG
I see her. They didn’t remember you.
 
tametla yohauek nye ta’ena tapya ka
2pl-P.4s have-VOL-NEG reason 2s-P.1s follow Q
You (all) don’t want to have it. Why are you following me?
 
§ 3.5) Derivation (yota)
 
Verbalization
 
Causative verbs (as well as achievement verbs) can be formed from other verbs by adding -mya (from muya - "do, make, cause") or -la (from ela - "become; change into; turn into"). This type of derivation is fairly common; however, verbs created in this way are syntactically defective and tend to appear only in serial verb constructions.
 
tinamya - "bend" < tina - be bent
pitamya - "hollow out" < pita - be hollow; void
enomya - "annoy, bother" < eno - be angry
tsipuela - "slow down" < tsipue - be slow
kyolola - "speed up" < kyolo - be quick
 
 
Intensive verbs can be formed from other verbs by adding -mpa (from mpa - "many; much; very"), or more commonly -hu (from kyohu - "be drastic; extreme; aggressive").
ketsahu - "dismiss, reject, repudiate" < ketsa - doubt
amyampa - "fall in love with" < amya - be fond of; like; prefer (of people)
 
Adjective-like stative verbs which name an associated quality may be formed from nouns by -n (from no - "thing" (-ish, -ly, -ous)).
 
timan - "be cruel, be bloody" < tima - blood
amyan - "be welcoming, be hospitable" < amya - be fond of
 
Nominalization
 
Agentive
 
Nouns referring to a human subject of a verb (usually in a habitual sense) can be formed with the agentive suffix -ko (from ko - "individual; person"). This suffix changes to -tlo when a velar stop is present in the preceding syllable.
 
kitlako - "craftsman" < kitla - create; invent; make-up
sutako - "inhabitant (of)" < suta - live; reside; dwell; inhabit; settle
yekatlo - "unmarried young adult" < yeka - be separate, be on one's own
makatlo - "musician" < maka - music; play ~; tune
tsaniko - "storyteller" < tsani - recite, tell (a story)
 
Instrumental
 
Instrument nouns and names for tools and other inanimates can be derived from verbs or from other nouns by adding the suffix -nyo (from mayo - "device; equipment; tool").
 
hitanyo - "atlatl (spear-thrower)" < hita - throw; cast; expel
amonyo - "handle (for carrying)" < amo - transport; carry
kusunyo - "clasp, brooch, fibula" < kusu - squeeze
toponyo - "lock" < topo - door; gate
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
Locative


Location nouns can be formed from both nouns and verbs by several suffixes. These indicate specific places where either something happens, or something resides there are a few affixes which modify both verbs and nouns.
Location nouns can be formed from both nouns and verbs by several suffixes. These indicate specific places where either something happens, or something resides there are a few affixes which modify both verbs and nouns.


-mo (from mo - "location; place; site"). This suffix is used to form the general idea of where something happens or resides.
[[Kala/affixes#mo|-'''mo''']] (from [[Kala/lexicon#ma|'''mo''']] - ''"location; place; site"''). This suffix is used to form the general idea of where something happens or resides.
 
tanamo - "battlefield; boxing ring; wrestling mat, etc." < tana - fight; combat
uelomo - "bicycle-place; bike path; bike rack, etc." < uelo - bicycle; bike
inamo - "eat-place; dining room; restaurant" [This can also mean food-place; pantry, etc.] < ina - food; eat
onyomo - "learn-place; school" < onyo - learn; study
 
-su (from suku - "market; shop; store"). This suffix is used to specify a business where items are produced and/or sold.
 
tiyasu - "bread-shop; bakery" < tiya - bread
inasu - "food-market; grocery store; restaurant" < ina - food; eat
uelosu - "bicycle-shop" < uelo - bicycle; bike
 
-kyo (from hakyo - "college; school; university"). This suffix is used to specify a location where students learn. This can also be used to indicate a school of thought, or ideology.
 
tanakyo - "dojo; martial arts training academy; etc." < tana - fight; combat
kuhakyo - "culinary-school; chef’s academy" < kuha - cook; prepare food
tsiyakyo - "liberalism" < tsiya - freedom; liberty
ya'akyo - "medical-school" < ya'a - medicine; drug; cure
 
-sa (from sala - "chamber; room; section"). This is more specific than -mo and used primarily for spaces inside buildings.
 
kuhasa - "cook-room; kitchen" < kuha - cook; prepare food
mokusa - "sleep-room; bedroom" < moku - sleep; rest
inasa - "eat-room; dining room" < ina - food; eat
onyosa - "learn-room; classroom" < onyo - learn; study
 
Honorific
 
Honorific nouns can be formed from other nouns by prefixing o-.
 
omasa - "stag" < masa - deer; cervine
okama - "capital" < kama - town; village
 
 
 
§ 3.5.1) Word Formation (muntamila)
 
In Kala, new words can be formed by adding prefixes or suffixes to existing words, or by combining two existing words as a compound noun. It's also possible to reuse adjectives as nouns, and verbs as nouns, without adding an affix.
 
The most common ending (other than tense, aspect, and modals) is the adverbial ending –n. It is used to mean "similar to ...", "-like", "-ish", "full of ..." or "made of ...", and "pertaining to ..." or "to do with ...".
 
Here are some common examples:
kyo’a - "quiet" > kyo’an – quietly
enke - "simple" > enken – simply
ntahi - "child" > ntahin – childish; childlike
putsu - "monster" > putsun – monstrous
yoti - "game" > yotin – playful
hanya - "nation" > hanyan – national
kuaha - "science" > kuahan – scientific
olo - "gold" > olon – made of gold
§ 3.5.2) Compounding (tsunamila)
 
New nouns are usually created through head-initial compounding, using both nominal and verbal stems as the second, dependent element of the compound. The resulting lexical entries usually behave as single phonological words; which, however, have four full syllables: kuatlatloha "grass snake". Compounding of more than two elements is not common.
kayapusu - "earthquake" > kaya - earth + pusu - vibrate
asuaseka - "leather" > asua - skin + seka - dry
There are also numerous affixes used to form new meanings. A few examples are;
tiyasu - "bakery" > tiya - bread + -su - market; shop
onyomo - "school" > onyo - learn + -mo - place; location
kuhasa - "kitchen" > kuha - cook + -sa - room; chamber
pyetampu - "egg-shaped" > pyeta - egg + -mpu - shape; form


* '''tanamo''' - "battlefield; boxing ring; wrestling mat, etc." < '''tana''' - fight; combat
* '''uelomo''' - "bicycle-place; bike path; bike rack, etc." < '''uelo''' - bicycle; bike
* '''inamo''' - "eat-place; dining room; restaurant" [This can also mean food-place; pantry, etc.] < '''ina''' - food; eat
* '''onyomo''' - "learn-place; school" < '''onyo''' - learn; study


[[Kala/affixes#su|-'''su''']] (from [[Kala/lexicon#sa|'''suku''']] - ''"market; shop; store"''). This suffix is used to specify a business where items are produced and/or sold.


* '''tiyasu''' - "bread-shop; bakery" < '''tiya''' - bread
* '''inasu''' - "food-market; grocery store; restaurant" < '''ina''' - food; eat
* '''uelosu''' - "bicycle-shop" < '''uelo''' - bicycle; bike


[[Kala/affixes#kyo|-'''kyo''']] (from [[Kala/lexicon#ha|'''hakyo''']] - ''"college; school; university"''). This suffix is used to specify a location where students learn. This can also be used to indicate a school of thought, or ideology.


* '''tanakyo''' - "dojo; martial arts training academy; etc." < '''tana''' - fight; combat
* '''kuhakyo''' - "culinary-school; chef’s academy" < '''kuha''' - cook; prepare food
* '''tsiyakyo''' - "liberalism" < '''tsiya''' - freedom; liberty
* '''ya'akyo''' - "medical-school" < '''ya'a''' - medicine; drug; cure


[[Kala/affixes#sa|-'''sa''']] (from [[Kala/lexicon#sa|'''sala''']] - ''"chamber; room; section"''). This is more specific than
[[Kala/affixes#mo|-'''mo''']] and used primarily for spaces inside buildings.


* '''kuhasa''' - "cook-room; kitchen" < '''kuha''' - cook; prepare food
* '''mokusa''' - "sleep-room; bedroom" < '''moku''' - sleep; rest
* '''inasa''' - "eat-room; dining room" < '''ina''' - food; eat
* '''onyosa''' - "learn-room; classroom" < '''onyo''' - learn; study


==== Diminutive ====


§ 3.6) Particles (peya)
Diminutive nouns and endearment terms can be formed from verbs and other nouns by adding the suffix [[Kala/affixes#hi|-'''hi''']] (from [[Kala/lexicon#a|'''ahi''']] - ''"few; small"''). This becomes '''-ki''' after a syllable that contains a [[wp:Glottal_fricative|glottal fricative]].


Particles in Kala cover a broad spectrum of what are more accurately called function words. These include adverbs, prepositions (more accurately locative or relative verbs), conjunctions, interjections, onomatopes, and structural particles.
* '''mukuhi''' - "blade" < '''muku''' - knife
* '''umahi''' - "foal" < '''uma''' - horse; equine
* '''mitahi''' - "puppy" < '''mita''' - dog; canine
* '''ohuaki''' - "indulgence" < '''ohua''' - luxurious; extravagant


§ 3.6.1) Adverbs (pusa)
==== Augmentative ====


Adverbs in Kala strictly modify verb phrases and tend to be placed before the construction. They are marked with -n, from no - "thing; item".
Augmentative nouns can be formed from verbs and other nouns by adding the suffix [[Kala/affixes#ha|-'''ha''']] (from [[Kala/lexicon#ta|'''taha''']] - ''"big; large; grand"''). This becomes '''-ka''' after a syllable that contains a [[wp:Glottal_fricative|glottal fricative]].


kyolon ha ina tsumun mita yalapayek
* '''kamaha''' - "city" < '''kama''' - village; town
be.quick-ADV 3s eat be.careful-ADV dog walk-ABIL-PST-NEG
* '''ohaka''' - "dislocate one's jaw" < '''oha''' - yawn; open one's mouth
She is eating quickly. The dog was unable to walk carefully.
* '''mosaha''' - "epic; novel" < '''mosa''' - book; letter; scroll
* '''tiniha''' - "hurricane" < '''tini''' - spiral; whorl


kyo’an nta’im mokunko sapon ha ke tapo hayo iuanko
==== Honorific ====
be.quiet-ADV infant-PL sleep-PROG be.soft-ADV 3s O shoulder 3s.GEN massage-CONT
The babies are sleeping quietly. He is massaging her shoulder softly.


§ 3.6.2) Locative verbs (tatse)
Honorific nouns can be formed from other nouns by prefixing [[Kala/affixes#o|'''o'''-]].


Kala does not have prepositions (or postpositions) as a distinct part of speech. Instead, many locative verbs can be used as adpositionals, in which case they precede the noun they modify. There is one general locative (-hue) which is affixed to nouns (and occasionally verbs) to indicate the sense of “at; in; on”. Here are some common verbs used as adpositions:
* '''omasa''' - "stag" < '''masa''' - deer; cervine
* '''okama''' - "capital" < '''kama''' - town; village


-hue  – in; at; on (general locative)
=Syntax=
nahe – within; inside
nyaue – out; outside of; exterior
ma’e – before; in front of
pue – behind; after; in back of
ua’e – above; over; on
tahe – below; under; beneath; bottom
ya’e – near; close to
uaye – away (from)
maye – between; among


The above are used as prepositions, but can also function strictly as verbs.
Kala has an extremely regular grammar, with very few exceptions to its rules. Sentences are made up of one or more phrases. Each phrase consists of a verb (optionally followed by modifying particles) and a subject (optionally followed by modifying particles). The subject, if understood, can be omitted at the end of an utterance: '''pana''' ("''It is raining.''") '''pana!''' ("''Rain!''") An utterance can be anything from an interjection to a story.


mita tahe yempa ina mita ke yempa tahe
The basic structure of a '''Kala''' sentence is: <tt>AGENT--PATIENT--VERB</tt> (or [[wp:Subject–object–verb|'''SOV''']])
dog under table eat dog table be.under
The dog is eating under the table. The dog is under the table.
(The dog is under the table eating.)


The agent is the person or thing doing the action described by the verb; The patient is the recipient of that action. The importance of word order can be seen by comparing the following sentences:
{{col-begin}}
{{col-break}}
* '''mita tlaka anya'''
: <small>dog man see</small>
: ''The dog sees the man.''
{{col-break}}
* '''tlaka mita anya'''
: <small>man dog see</small>
: ''The man sees the dog.''
{{col-end}}
In both sentences, the words are identical, the only way to know who is seeing whom is by the order of the words in the sentence.


The use of the object marker '''ke''' indicates the recipient of the action.


The suffix -la (from yala “go; walk; travel”) forms an allative preposition, expressing movement in the indicated direction, stopping at the position indicated by the locative:
* <b>naka ke mita itsa</b>
: <small>woman O dog love</small>
: <i>The woman loves the dog.</i>


nahela  topu – into bed
Kala lacks morphological adjectives and instead uses [[wp:Predicative_verb|predicative]] verbs.
pahela ke ana tayo – onto your head
tsayela tsaka – up to the house


The locative/allative pair works like English on/onto, in/into, but in Kala this distinction is made for all locatives: you must distinguish between
* <b>ke tsaka taha</b>
: <small>O house be.big</small>
: <i>The big house / The house is big</i>


pue’ela  kuanu – go behind a bush  - (motion implied → allative)
Kala lacks morphological adverbs, verbs modified with the adverbial ending '''-n''' tend to precede the verb phrase they modify.
pue kuanu koma – hide behind a bush  - (no motion → locative)


§ 3.6.3) Conjunctions (penku)
* <b>tsumun nam yokone</b>
: <small>cautious-ADV 1pl swim-SUG</small>
: <i>We should swim cautiously.</i>


$$$
Kala lacks morphological prepositions and instead uses locational and relational verbs.


Clause-level conjunctions such as ku "and", ua "or", or ehe "but, however" are placed clause-initially. Note that these conjunctions cannot be used to connect noun phrases.  
* <b>mita ke yempa tahe</b>
: <small>dog O table be.under</small>
: <i>The dog is under the table.</i>


tahi tohyo ku nahi pina
== Clauses ==
boy brave CL.CONJ girl intelligent
The boy is brave and the girl is intelligent.


ima kihu saman ehe pakyotlai
Relative clauses (or adjective clauses) function like adjectives. Relative clauses follow the noun or noun phrase that they modify:
now weather sun-ADJ however storm-IMM
Now the weather is sunny, but a storm will come soon.


Non-subject noun phrases are coordinated using the conjunction ma "and" (sometimes "with").  
* '''naka ke na itsatle te ameyo'''
: <small>woman O 1s love-REL from America</small>
: ''The woman (that) I love comes from America.''


yomaye na ke tanka ma pato anya
* '''mayo ke na kitlayetle muyak'''
day-PST 1s O eagle CONJ duck see
: <small>tool O 1s create-PST-REL do-NEG</small>
I saw an eagle and a duck yesterday.
: ''The tool (that) I built doesn't function.''


kinti ke tsaka kamyo ma'a yosu sapotle ma siuem muya
* '''na ke ta yani unyak''' / '''na ke yani tayo unyak'''
squirrel O house 3pl.GEN with moss soft-REL and leaf.PL make
: <small>1s O 2s mean know-NEG / 1s O meaning 2s.GEN know-NEG</small>
The squirrels make their nest comfortable with soft moss and leaves.
: ''I don't understand what you mean.''


ona ma ota kyosanku
Subordinate clauses rely on [[Kala#Conjunctions_.28penku.29|conjunctions]] and other particles.
mother and father fornicate-RECP
Mother and father have sex [with each other].


ta ma'a na ke molihuelatli
* '''eya ta ke mpeka inaye yatli ta pasala'''
2s with 1s O forest-LOC-MOT-FUT
: <small>maybe 2s O toad eat-PST therefore 2s nauseous-become</small>
You and I will go to the forest together.
: ''If you ate the toad (which you might have), you might get sick.''


Noun phrases can be presented as alternatives to each other with the conjunction ua "or; other". This conjunction can be used with both subjects and non-subjects. The conjunction ue "(exclusive) either X or Y" is used to delimit other nouns from the conjunction phrase.
* '''naye na tasa ke masa okyohue anyaye'''
: <small>while 1s hunt O deer clearing-LOC see-PST</small>
: ''While hunting, I saw a deer in a clearing.''


ta ke nasi ua poma inamyo tsola ue otso itsikua mataye
== Questions ==
2s O pear or apple eat-PERM fox either.X.or.Y wolf PROX-bird kill-PST
You may eat an apple or a pear. It must have been a fox or a wolf that killed this bird.


Contrastive coordination of noun phrases ("but") is achieved with ehe "but; however" (or me more informally) if the noun phrases appear in subject position.
There are two types of questions: Polar, those which may be answered "yes" or "no," and those which require explanations as answers.


yomaye mita'u ehek mitana ke kutsu kapya
==== Polar Questions ====
day-PST dog-MASC but-NEG O meat receive
The male dog but not the female dog received meat yesterday.


na itlaka mek inaka unya
Any statement can become a polar question by adding the interrogative particle '''ka''' at the end of the sentence.
1s PROX-man but-NEG PROX-woman know
I know this man, but not this woman.


{{col-begin}}
{{col-break}}
* '''mita ina'''
: <small>dog eat</small>
: ''The dog eats.''


* '''nta'i moku'''
: <small>baby sleep</small>
: ''The baby is sleeping. / The baby sleeps.''


* '''ta ke tlo'o anyaye'''
: <small>2SG O elephant see-PST</small>
: ''You saw the elephant.''


* '''tekatlo eta ke ya'a yetaye'''
: <small>heal-AG P.2SG O medicine give-PST</small>
: ''The doctor gave you the medicine.''
{{col-break}}
* '''mita ina ka'''
: <small>dog eat Q</small>
: ''Does the dog eat?''


* '''nta'i moku ka'''
: <small>baby sleep Q</small>
: ''Is the baby sleeping?''


* '''ta ke tlo'o anyaye ka'''
: <small>2SG O elephant see-PST Q</small>
: ''Did you see the elephant?''


* '''tekatlo eta ke ya'a yetaye ka'''
: <small>heal-AG P.2SG O medicine give-PST Q</small>
: ''Did the doctor give you the medicine?''
{{col-end}}
==== Content questions ====


Questions that give a list of possible answers are formed like polar questions, with the conjunction '''ue''' ‘or’ introducing each alternative (which must appear in the form of a noun phrase).
{{col-begin}}
{{col-break}}
* '''ta ke nkapa ue maya inuue ka'''
: <small>2SG O beer or.EXCL water drink-VOL Q</small>
: ''Do you want to drink beer or water?''
{{col-break}}
* '''uala ta ke sinka mataye ue empa ma koma ka'''
: <small>truly 2sg O lion kill-PST or.EXCL flee CONJ hide Q</small>
: ''Did you really kill the lion, or did you run away and hide?''
{{col-end}}
Open content questions are most easily formed with the correlatives, such as '''ko''' ‘person’, '''mo''' ‘place’, '''to''' ‘manner’, etc. These correlatives always appear clause-initially:


{{col-begin}}
{{col-break}}
* '''ko ta ka'''
: <small>person 2sg Q</small>
: ''Who are you?''
{{col-break}}
* '''itla ka'''
: <small>this Q</small>
: ''What is this?''
{{col-break}}
* '''to kihu ka'''
: <small>manner weather Q</small>
: ''What's the weather like?''
{{col-end}}
The other type contains a question word and is followed by '''ka''':


§ 3.6.4) Interjections (nita)
{| class="wikitable" style="text-align: center; width: 700px;"
|+ kanyo
|-
!
! Kala
! gloss
! English
|-
! object
| '''ke mita ina ka''' || <small>O dog eat Q</small> || ''What does the dog eat?''
|-
! person
| '''ko ina ka''' || <small>person eat Q</small> || ''Who eats?''
|-
! possession
| '''koyo mita ina ka''' || <small>person-POSS dog eat Q</small> || ''Whose dog eats?''
|-
! manner
| '''to mita ina ka''' || <small>manner dog eat Q</small> || ''How does the dog eat?''
|-
! place
| '''mo mita ina ka''' || <small>place dog eat Q</small> || ''Where does the dog eat?''
|-
! reason
| '''nye mita ina ka''' || <small>reason dog eat Q</small> || ''Why does the dog eat?''
|-
! time
| '''ama mita ina ka''' || <small>time dog eat Q</small> || ''When does the dog eat?''
|-
! amount
| '''uku mita ina ka''' || <small>amount dog eat Q</small> || ''How much/many does the dog eat?''
|-
! which
| '''ula mita ina ka''' || <small>any dog eat Q</small> || ''Which dog eats?''
|}


The term “interjection” is used to cover a range of pragmatic, or discourse markers that do not fit well into any other category.  This is because many words and expressions have a pragmatic rather than a semantic meaning.
=Semantic fields and pragmatics=


a – expresses acknowledgement, agreement, or simply that one is listening
=Writing system=
e – marks dispreferreds, ends a digression,
impo – marks a sudden change of topic
ya – vocative marker, polite imperative, expresses commiseration
yali – excuses jostling or interruptions


These can occur either at the beginning or the end of a sentence.
Kala conscripts are many and varied. Rather than multiple pages explaining each of them, [[Kala/writing|'''this''']] page serves as a working list with a consistent example across each script. The most commonly used script is the Hangul adaptation for Kala.


e…nakkan ke ameyo yohatsek
== ''Han Moya'' ==
well queen O America have-seem-NEG
Well... America doesn’t really have a queen.


ke motsa ya’o…a
'''Han Moya''' is an adaptation of [[wp:Hangul|Hangul]] for writing '''Kala'''. It is written horizontally, in lines running from left to right. It can also be written vertically in columns.
O banana five yes
Mm hmm, (you want) five bananas.


impo…taye katso ka
==== consonants ====
so about meal Q
Anyway, what about dinner?


ya kyo’a…nam tsipue
* <font face="gungsuh"><big><big><b>ㄱㄲㄴㄷㄸㄹㅁㅂㅃㅅㅆㅇㅈㅉㅊㅋㅌㅍㅎ</b></big></big></font>
VOC quiet 1pl late
: '''k nk n t nt l m p mp s ns a ts nts ts` k` tl p` h'''
Hey, shut up, we’re late!
: /k~g ᵑk~ⁿg n t~d ⁿt~ⁿd l~ɾ m p~b ᵐp~ᵐb s~ʃ ⁿs~ⁿʃ - ts~t͡ʃ ⁿts~ⁿt͡ʃ tsʰ~t͡ʃʰ kʰ t͡ɬ~tl pʰ h~ɦ/


Other common interjections – of course – include curses, vulgarities, obscenities, etc.
The adaptations of doubled consonants are used word initially to indicate [[wp:Prenasalized_consonant|prenasalization]]. Medial occurrences of nasalized syllables are written across syllables.


kotsa – a spiteful person (“bitch; bastard”)
: Example:
kuna – excrete; expel; defecate (“shit”)
kyosa – sex; copulation; fornicate (“fuck”)
nanka – emphasizing disgust; [interj. of contempt]; (“damn; darn”)
tsaya – damn [general invective]


* <font face="gungsuh"><big><big><b>까바</b></big></big></font> - '''nkapa''' - alcohol; liquor / <font face="gungsuh"><big><big><b>단가</b></big></big></font> - '''tanka''' - eagle; hawk; falcon


* <font face="gungsuh"><big><big><b>감바</b></big></big></font> - '''kampa''' - Cheers! / <font face="gungsuh"><big><big><b>쁘라</b></big></big></font> - '''mpula''' - lamp; lantern; light


==== vowels ====


§ 5) Serial Verbs (uatitsuna)
* <font face="gungsuh"><big><big><b>ㅏ ᅶ ㅐ ㅑ ᅸ ㅓ ㅕ ㅗ ㅛ ㅜ ㅟ ㅠ ㅡ ㅣ</b></big></big></font>
: '''a ao ai ya yao e ye o ao yo ua uai ue u i'''
: /a~a: aʊ̯ aɪ̯ ja~ʲa: jaʊ̯~ʲaʊ̯ e~ɛ je~ʲɛ o~o: jo~ʲo: wa~ʷa: waɪ̯~ʷaɪ̯ we~ʷe: u~u: i~ɪ/


§ 5.1) Comparison (nisanko)
* <font face="gungsuh"><big><big><b>ㅘ</b></big></big></font> This is pronounced /wa/ in Korean because of the order of the vowels; however, because [[wp:List_of_Hangul_jamo|obsolete jamo]] are difficult to type and look junky as images, in Kala, this is used for /aʊ̯/ when typing. It is rarely seen due to the diphthong itself being uncommon.


In Kala the concepts of comparative and superlative degree of an adjective (verb) are merged into a single form, the elative. How this form is understood or translated depends upon context and definiteness. In the absence of comparison, the elative conveys the notion of “greatest”, “supreme.”
=Examples=


tsaka hayo ke nayo tahaka ke mauam tayo yanahu
= Numbers =
house 3s.GEN O 1s.GEN big-AUG O flower.PL 2s.GEN yellow-EXT
His house is bigger than mine. Your flowers are the most yellow.


iyapo ke tsaka tayo pakoha mitala ke yetlam hikyahi
'''Kala''' uses a base 10 number system. The basic numbers are as follows:
PROX-building O home 2sg new-AUG dog-INDEF O DIST-4pl old-DIM
This building is newer than your home. Some dogs are less old (younger) than others.


When comparing the amount of involvement of several participants …:
{| class="wikitable" style="text-align: center; width: 800px;"
|+
|-
! Kala
! number
! English
! Kala
! number
! English
! Kala
! number
! English
|-
| '''ye'o''' || 0 || zero || '''tsa'o''' || 6 || six || '''nya'o''' || 500 || five hundred
|-
| '''na'o''' || 1 || one || '''ka'o''' || 7 || seven || '''tle'o''' || 10<sup>3</sup> || (one) thousand
|-
| '''ta'o''' || 2 || two || '''pa'o''' || 8 || eight || '''mue'o''' || 10<sup>4</sup> || ten thousand
|-
| '''ha'o''' || 3 || three || '''sa'o''' || 9 || nine || '''kye'o''' || 10<sup>5</sup> || (one) hundred thousand
|-
| '''ma'o''' || 4 || four || '''ue'o''' || 10 || ten || '''nte'o''' || 10<sup>6</sup> || (one) million
|-
| '''ya'o''' || 5 || five || '''nye'o''' || 100 || (one) hundred || '''hue'o''' || 10<sup>9</sup> || (one) billion
|}


tsaneya ke ona pa’e naku hayo itsaha
==== Forming Larger Numbers ====
Jane O mother other.than sister 3s.GEN love-AUG
Jane loves her mother more than her sister does.


* '''uena'o''' - eleven / 11
* '''taue'o''' - twenty / 20
* '''nyeka'o''' - one hundred seven / 107
* '''hanyetauetsa'o''' (''long form'') / '''hatatsa'o''' (''short form'') - three hundred twenty six / 326
* '''tsatletauema'o''' - six thousand and twenty four / 6024


Long form numbers are used in formal situations, including financial transactions, especially involving large sums. Short form numbers are used in everyday speech and when calculating basic math.


6.2. Comparison
==== Other Number Forms ====
Comparative adjectives are formed by adding the adverbs plu (‘more’) and min (‘less’). ‘Than’ is ca:
• La cosina es plu calda ca la jardin. – The kitchen is hotter than the garden.
• Esta leto es min comfortosa ca me ia previde. – This bed is less comfortable than I expected.
Superlative adjectives are formed by adding the adverbs la plu (‘most’) and la min (‘least’):
29
• La sol es la ojeto la plu calda en la sistem solal. – The sun is the hottest object in the solar system.
• El ia ave un fia la plu bela en la mundo. – She had a daughter, the most beautiful (of daughters) in the world.
Ordinal numbers can be combined with the superlative construction:
• Vega es la stela sinco la plu briliante en la sielo de note. – Vega is the fifth brightest star in the night sky.
• El ia deveni la om tre la plu rica en la mundo. – He became the third richest man in the world.
Equality comparisons use the combination tan… como… (‘so… as…’):
• La arbor ia es tan alta como un casa. – The tree was as tall as a house.
• On es tan joven como on senti. – You’re as young as you feel.


{| class="wikitable" style="text-align: center; width: 600px;"
|+
|-
! Kala
! number
! English
! ordinal
! multiple
! fractional
|-
| '''na'o''' || 1 || one  || '''kina'o'''<br>first || '''tina'o'''<br>once || -
|-
| '''ueta'o''' || 12 || twelve || '''kiueta'o'''<br>twelfth  || '''tiueta'o'''<br>duodecuple || '''iueta'o'''<br>a twelfth
|-
| '''yauema'o'''<br>'''(yama'o)''' || 54 || fifty four  || '''kiyama'o'''<br>fifty fourth || '''tiyama'o'''<br>54 times || '''iyama'o'''<br>a fifty fourth
|-
| '''nyetsa'o''' || 106 || one hundred (and) six || '''kinyetsa'o'''<br>106<sup>th</sup> || '''tinyetsa'o'''<br>106 times || '''inyetsa'o'''<br>a 106<sup>th</sup>
|-
| '''katle'o''' || 7000 || seven thousand  || '''kikatle'o'''<br>seven thousandth || '''tikatle'o'''<br>7000 times || '''ikatle'o'''<br>1/7000
|}


ha ke tanko hayo kihaku > 3s O team 3s.GEN tall-EXT > She is the tallest on her team.
=== Math Operations ===


'''Kala''' math is fairly basic and relies on particles and verbs to express functions.


Addition uses '''ma''' (''and; also''). There is no specific order to the numbers in the phrase/equation.


* '''ha'o ma ya'o ke pa'o a'''
: <small>3 and 5 O 8 COP</small>
: ''Three plus five is eight.''


akana.conlang.org/wiki/Ronc_Tyu/Serial_verb_constructions#Sequential_events
Subtraction uses '''ma''' (''and; also'') and a negative form of the smaller integer. There is no specific order to the numbers in the phrase/equation.  


* '''tsa'o ma ya'ok ke na'o a'''
: <small>6 and 5-NEG O 1 COP</small>
: ''Six and five-less is one.''


akana.conlang.org/wiki/Ronc_Tyu#Relative_clauses
Multiplication uses '''ma''' (''and; also'') and a multiple form of one of the integers. There is no specific order to the numbers in the phrase/equation.


wikibooks.org/wiki/Japanese/Grammar/Transitivity
* '''ha'o ma tima'o ke ueta'o a'''
: <small>3 and multiple-4 O 12 COP</small>
: ''Three times four is twelve.''


Division uses '''yeka''' (''divide; separate; partition''), or '''ma''' (''and; also'') and a multiple-negative form of one of the integers. There is no specific order to the numbers in the phrase/equation.


Abbreviations
* '''tama'o ma ha'o ke pa'o yeka'''
: <small>24 and 3 O 8 division</small>
: ''Twenty-four divided by three is eight.''


1 first person
=Lexicon=
2 second person
3 third person
4 fourth person
ABIL abilitative mood
AG agentive
ASS assumptive mood
ATT attemptive mood
CONT continuous aspect
DIST distal deixis
DUB dubitative mood
FUT future tense
HORT hortative mood
IMM immediate future tense
MED medial deixis
NEC necessitative mood
NEG negative mood
O (direct) object / topic
PFV perfective aspect
PL plural
PREC precative mood
PREP preparative mood
PROG progressive aspect
PROP propositive mood
PROX proximal deixis
PST past tense
Q question particle
SG singular
VOL volitive mood


links:
=== references ===


akana.conlang.org/wiki/Ronc_Tyu
* akana.conlang.org/wiki/Ronc_Tyu  
en.wikibooks.org/wiki/Na’vi
* en.wikibooks.org/wiki/Na’vi  
frathwiki.com/Kala
* frathwiki.com/Kala  
hungarianreference.com/
* hungarianreference.com/  
japaneselanguageguide.com/grammar/noun.asp
* japaneselanguageguide.com/grammar/noun.asp  
kinezika.info/pdf/ChineseEssentialGrammar.pdf
* kinezika.info/pdf/ChineseEssentialGrammar.pdf  
klingonwiki.net/En/Math
* klingonwiki.net/En/Math  
ossicone.com/conlangs/uskra
* ossicone.com/conlangs/uskra  
pomax.github.io/nrGrammar/
* pomax.github.io/nrGrammar/  
resources.allsetlearning.com/chinese/grammar/
* resources.allsetlearning.com/chinese/grammar/  
rickmor.x10.mx/arabic_morphology.html
* rickmor.x10.mx/arabic_morphology.html  
turkishlanguage.co.uk/
* turkishlanguage.co.uk/  
zompist.com/kitgram.html
* zompist.com/kitgram.html  
zompist.com/wedei.html
* zompist.com/wedei.html
* languagesgulper.com/eng/Quechua.html

Revision as of 16:32, 25 November 2017

Introduction

Kala is a personal conlang (actually more of an artlang), based on my aesthetic preferences, not attached to any conworld or conculture. This language draws on natlangs (natural language), other conlangs, and of course imagination. Kala was started in late 2009. The phonemic inventory is based on Classical Nahuatl while the syllable structure and vowels are based on the strict (C)V structure of Japanese, and the presence of prenasalized stops is influenced by Bantu languages. Kala’s grammar was initially based on Japanese but has changed based on influence from several natural and constructed languages. Many – if not most – of Kala lexemes are derived from or inspired by natural languages. A few have been taken from previous projects or constructed languages such as Ajara (a cipherlang from my youth) and Qatama (a conlang that I abandoned several years ago).

Characteristics

Kala has two parts of speech. Nouns and verbs are content words, while particles (and others) tend to be only functional. Many content words can be used as both nouns and verbs. The best and most common example would be ina /iːˈna/ "food; to eat". Kala is a context-oriented language. In most cases, the more important elements of a phrase are clustered toward the end of the sentence (e.g. verbs and their modifiers). The less important an element is to the understanding of a sentence, the more likely it is to be dropped. Consequently, many Kala sentences end-up consisting solely of a verb (or adjectival verb); more so in conversation than in written Kala, these short phrases are grammatically correct and natural. Here are some examples:

  • muya ka - /muːja gaː/ - do Q - (What are you) doing?
  • ina - /iːna/ - eat - (I am) eating.
  • tamatse - /tamaːˌt͡ʃɛ/ - good-seem - (That looks) good.
  • ueha ka - /weːɦa kaː/ - want Q - (Do you) want (some)?
  • nyasak - /ɲaːʃak/ - thank-NEG - No, thank (you).

Notice that none of the above contains any pronouns, or nouns. Any contextually understood elements may be omitted unless indispensable. There can be considerable divergence from what is grammatical, and what is acceptably idiomatic. The spectrum of formality and grammatical to idiomatic can be seen in the example below:

  • na’etla muyaye1s-P.4s do-PSTI did it. > [grammatical, formal]
  • etla muyayeP.4s do-PST(I) did it. >> It was done. > [grammatical, formal, passive]
  • na muyaye1s do-PSTI did (it). > [grammatical, informal]
  • muyayedo-PST(I) did (it). > [semi-grammatical, idiomatic]
  • muyye – /muːɟɛ/ – do-PST(I) did (it). > [ungrammatical, idiomatic]

Borrowing

See also: Kala etymology

Kala borrows extensively from various natural languages. This is a very small sample of borrowings:

  • patoduck (Anatidae); from Spanish pato
  • kalato speak, talk, converse; from Arabic takallama
  • myontato allow, permit; from Finnish myöntää
  • naI, me; from Arabic ʾanā
  • tsenkaorange; from Chinese chéng
  • uasito take, get, acquire; from Lakota wasichu
  • ato be, exist, yes; from Japanese aru

So, some phrases can contain words from multiple natlangs:

  • ta ke inu uasiye ka
2s O drink take-PST Q
Did you take the drink?

Phonology

Consonants

Consonants
Labial Alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal
Nasal m (m) n (n) ɲ (ny)
Plosive p~b (p) t~d (t) k~g (g) ʔ ( ' )
Affricate ts~t͡ʃ (ts) t͡ɬ~tl (tl)
Continuant s~ʃ (s) l~ɾ (l) h~ɦ (h)
Semivowel j (y) w (u)

The glottal stop is not phonemic but is included in the chart above for completeness. It is only ever intervocalic, meaning it is pronounced between two vowels and/or diphthongs. Where ~ appears, it indicates free variation between phonemes. However, certain sounds change in a predictable way. For example, /h/ becomes [ɦ] when preceded or followed by a front vowel, including when labialized or palatalized. The alveolar affricates are most often /t͡ʃ/ and /t͡ɬ/. The “s” is almost always /ʃ/ unless preceded or followed by a syllable with the onset /t͡ʃ/, in which case “s” becomes /s/. So, sama (sun; star; solar) is /ˈʃaːma/ where sitsa (heat; hot) is /ˈsiːt͡ʃa/ and tsisi (embroider; embroidery) is /t͡ʃiːsi/.

  • Prenasalized: /ᵐp ⁿt ᵑk/
  • Labialized:/pʷ kʷ mʷ nʷ ʃʷ hʷ t͡ʃʷ/
  • Palatalized: /pʲ kʲ mʲ hʲ/

Note: Because of its small phoneme inventory, Kala allows for quite a lot of allophonic variation. For example, /p t k/ may be pronounced [b d ɡ] as well as [p t k], /s l h/ as [ʃ ɾ ɦ], and /t͡s t͡ɬ/ as [t͡ʃ t͡l]; also, vowels may be either long or short.

Vowels

Vowels
Front Back
Close i~ɪ (i) u~u: (u)
Mid e~ɛ (e) o~o: (o)
Open a~a: (a)

Kala has five vowels /i/, /e/, /a/, /o/ and /u/. Each occurs in both stressed and unstressed syllables. Phonetic nasalization occurs for vowels occurring between nasal consonants or when preceding a syllable-final nasal, e.g. tsunka [ˈt͡ʃũᵑka] ('bug').

Diphthongs

Phonetically, Kala has only two diphthongs, both falling; [aɪ̯] and [aʊ̯], but there are five syllables that can be analyzed as rising diphthongs; [wa], [we], [ja], [je], and [jo]. The two triphthongs [waɪ̯] and [jaʊ̯] are very rare but should be noted as possible.

Phonotactics

Kala words are typically made up of open syllables of the type CV (consonant-vowel) with most words having syllables exclusively of this type. There is a limited set of syllables allowed by Kala phonotactics, similar to Japanese or Chinese.

  • /l/ cannot occur word initially (except in loanwords and toponyms):
lupunan = Lebanon
lupusu = Lupus

Syllables

Syllable structure in Kala is typically made up of open syllables of the type CV (consonant-vowel) with most lexemes having syllables exclusively of this type. The exception to this rule are the endings –m (indicating general plural), -n (indicating adverbial or adjectival use), and –k (indicating negative mood). These endings all are word final. In detail a Kala syllable can be analyzed thusly: (N)(C)(u, y)V(a, i) where (N) indicates nasalization, and u and y indicate labialization and palatalization respectively.

Syllable Chart

the 136 basic Kala syllables
a e i o u ua ue ya ye yo ai ao uai yao
p (m)pa (m)pe (m)pi (m)po (m)pu pua pue pya pye pyo pai pao puai pyao
t (n)ta (n)te (n)ti (n)to tai tao
k (n)ka (n)ke (n)ki (n)ko (n)ku kua kue kya kye kyo kai kao kuai kyao
m ma me mi mo mu mua mue mya mye myo mai mao muai myao
n na ne ni no nu nua nue nya nye nyo nai nao nuai nyao
s sa se si so su sua sue sai sao suai
h ha he hi ho hu hua hue hya hye hyo hai hao huai hyao
ts tsa tse tsi tso tsu tsua tsue tsai tsao tsuai
tl tla tle tli tlo tlai tlao
l la le li lo lai lao
- a e i o u ua ue ya ye yo ai ao uai yao

Syllables such as nsa, ntla, or ntsa can occur but usually only in place names or loanwords. The red syllables above occur infrequently and most often as the final syllable of a word.

Collating Order

The collating sequence (alphabetical order) is based on the order established in the Naua script.

Consonants p t k m n s h ts tl l
Vowels a e i o u ua ue ya ye yo

Based on this order, ma would come before ha, etc. Prenasalized syllables are ordered after their non-prenasalized counterparts, i.e. mpa comes after pyo but before ta. To see the collating sequence in action, look through the lexicon.

Stress

Stress generally falls on the penultimate syllable, which means that stress is de facto initial in most lemma given that stems are most often (CVCV). Monosyllabic words are not stressed. So;

  • masa - /ˈmaːsa/ >> masako - /maːˈsako/
  • tliyama - /tɬiːˈjama/ >> tliyamalo - /tɬiːjaˈmalo/
  • kam - /kaːm/ >> kamyo - /ˈkaːmʲo/

Morphology

Kala is a mostly agglutinative language that makes extensive use of compounding, incorporation and derivation. That is, it can add many different prefixes and suffixes to a root until very long words are formed, and a single word can sometimes constitute an entire sentence.

Nouns

Nouns in Kala are inflected only for number. Other relevant distinctions are animacy and possession, but these are not marked on the noun itself. Animacy plays a role both for pronoun choice and for the validity of some syntactic constructions.

Number

In general the plural suffix is not used when the plurality of the noun is clear from context. For example, while the English sentence "there are three dogs" would use the plural "dogs" instead of the singular "dog", the Kala sentence mita ha'o a "dog three exist" keeps the word mita "dog" in its unmarked form, as the numeral makes the plural marker redundant.

Nouns are marked for number; plural and collective:

  • mita - dog - a dog
    • mitam - dog-PL - dogs
    • tlimita - COL-dog - a dog pack / a pack of dogs
    • tlimitam - COL-dog-PL - dog packs / packs of dogs

When the final syllable of a word contains an m, mp, and sometimes a p the plural marking changes to -lo.

  • yama - mountain - a mountain
    • yamalo - mountain-PL - mountains
    • tliyama - COL-mountain - a mountain range / range of mountains
    • tliyamalo - COL-mountain-PL - mountain ranges / ranges of mountains

The collective plural is marked by tli-, derived from tatli, meaning "group; collection; gathering". It is mainly used to indicate collectives of animals, but can also indicate groups of flora, geographic features, and various other groupings. This is called the collective plural (COL).

Gender

Gender is not normally marked but can be with the endings -na and -ta to mark the feminine and masculine, respectively or nouns such as naka, tlaka, nahi, or tahi (the woman, the man, the girl, the boy), etc. A gender neutral suffix, -nta may be used when the gender is unknown or ambiguous.

  • uma - horse - a horse
    • umana - horse-FEM - mare
    • umata - horse-MASC - stallion

Pronouns

Kala agent pronouns are often omitted when the person is obvious from context. There are four persons in Kala. The 4th being inanimate, or indefinite. The pronoun na'am is used as the 1st person plural exclusive, meaning "We, but not you." The 3rd person plural is irregular, all other pronoun decline regularly. Pronouns do not inflect for gender; if gender is significant, one can use words like naka, tlaka, nahi, tahi (the woman, the man, the girl, the boy), etc.

Personal pronouns:

  • na - 1st person
  • ta - 2nd person
  • ha - 3rd person
  • tla - 4th person ("it", "one") (used for inanimate nouns)

Modifiers:

Other pronouns include:

  • tlokua - everyone, everybody
  • kola - someone, somebody; whomever, anyone, anybody
  • tlok - no one, nobody
  • nokua - everything
  • nola - something; whatever, anything
  • nok - nothing


nkalo
Agent Patient Reflexive Possessive Reciprocal
1sg na ena na'i nayo -
2sg ta eta ta'i tayo -
3sg ha eha ha'i hayo -
4sg tla etla tla'i tlayo -
1pl
1pl exclusive
nam
na'am
enam
ena'am
nami
na'ami
namyo
na'amyo
nanku
na'anku
2pl tam etam tami tamyo tanku
3pl kam ekam kami kamyo kanku
4pl tlam etlam tlami tlamyo tlanku

Reflexives and Reciprocals

Kala has reflexive and reciprocal pronoun forms.

  • na'i sepaye
1s.REFL injure-PST
I hurt myself.
  • ha'i tlela
3s.REFL bathe
She bathes herself.
  • kanku ke onta nayo itsa
3pl.RECP O parent 1s.GEN love
My parents love each other.
  • na'anku amyapak
1pl.EXCL.RECP like-ABIL-NEG
We (but not you) are not able to like each other.


Determiners & Demostratives

The demonstratives can be prefixed to any noun to show deixis. Kala makes a three-way distinction. Typically there is a distinction between proximal or first person (objects near to the speaker), medial or second person (objects near to the addressee), and distal or third person (objects far from both).

  • itla (i-) - this (near me)
  • uatla (ua-) - that (near you)
  • yetla (ye-) - that (over there)

Examples:

  • imitami - PROX-dog-few - These few dogs
  • yemitampa - DIST-dog-many - Those many dogs (over there)
  • uamitali - MED-dog-each - Each dog (each of those dogs) (near you)

Quantifiers follow the noun that modify.

  • kua (-kua) - all; every; whole
  • oli (-li) - each; every
  • ula (-la) - whatever; any; some
  • mi (-mi) - few; little
  • nke (-k) - none
  • mpa (-mpa) - many; much; a lot
  • maha - more; plus
  • ohi - less; fewer

Correlatives

uatse
Proximal
i-
Medial
ua-
Distal
ye-
Inclusive
-kua
Negative
-k
Indefinite
-la
mo
(place)
hina
here
uana
there
yemua
over there
mokua
everywhere
mok
nowhere
mola
somewhere; anywhere
ko
(person)
iko
this person
uako
that person
yeko
that person
(over there)
tlokua
everyone
tlok
no one
kola
someone; anyone
uku
(amount)
iku
this much
uaku
that much
- kua
all; every
ok
none
ula
some; any
ama
(time)
ima
now, at present
uama
then; at that time
- kuama
always
amak
never
tlama
sometime; anytime
so
(kind, type)
iso
this kind
so'o
that kind
yeso
that kind
(over there)
sokua
all kinds
sok
no kind (at all)
sola
some/any kind
no
(thing)
itla
this
uatla
that
yetla
that
(over there)
nokua
everything
nok
nothing; none
nola
something; anything
to
(manner, way)
yoto
thus; like this; this way
uato
that way
ato
that way
(over there)
tokua
every way
tok
no way
tola
somehow; anyway

Verbs

Kala relies on analytic serial verb constructions, and can therefore get by with very little verbal morphology. Each verb has at most two possible forms: the active and the stative. Passivity is marked on the subject thus verbs are unmarked and must be analyzed based on surrounding morphology. Active verbs solely denote actions and occurrences and never states in Kala. Stative verbs are the words that modify nouns in an attributive and often adjectival way. They often express a state like a quality or result. Verbs can be marked with several suffixes to add or change meaning. The modals and tense affixes can be added in different order to a verb to create a new meaning; their placement is not always fixed. The negative, adverbial, and plural endings are always final, while other affixes can be varied, but in general they should be ordered:

STEM-(SIZE/IMPORTANCE)-(MOOD)-(ASPECT)-(TENSE)-(NEGATIVE)

Example:

Verb Stem Size/Importance Mood Aspect Tense Negative
empa -hi -pa -nko -ye -k
run DIM ABIL PROG PST NEG
  • na empahipankoyek
1SG run-DIM-able-PROG-PST-NEG
I was not able to keep jogging.

Tense

Kala has three simple tenses; past, present, and future. Present tense is unmarked. However, past (-ye) and future (-tli) tenses can be modified to include immediate future ("is about to..."), distant future ("will...in a long while"), recent past ("just ..."), and remote past ("...a long while ago"). These distinctions are made with the augmentative and diminutive endings -ha and -hi.

eme
Kala gloss English
remote past kamahi hinayeha
or -hai
town-DIM be.here-REM There was a village here (long ago).
(before the lifetime of the speaker)
past naka mita anyaye woman dog see-PST The woman saw the dog.
recent past ota namyo akyayehi
or -hye
father 1pl.GEN wake-REC Our father just woke.
(action just finished)
present mita tahi yatsi dog boy bite The dog bites the boy.
future naka tahi tlepatli woman boy teach-FUT The woman will teach the boy.
immediate future na tlelatlihi
or -tlai
1s bathe-FUT I'll bathe soon.
(within the day)
distant future panam opuatliha rain-PL end-FUT The rains will end.
(months from now)
  • The present tense can show immediacy by using the adverb ima, "now; at this time":
ima mita ina - now dog eat - The dog is eating right now.
  • If a temporal adverb is used, the tense suffix may be omitted:
yomaye nam ina - yesterday 1pl eat - We ate yesterday.

Aspect

There are four aspects in Kala. The progressive, also called the continuous [CONT], this is used to express an incomplete action or state in progress at a specific time. It is marked with -nko, from nkoso - "to continue; proceed; progress". The perfective aspect indicates that an action is completed [PFV]. It is often translated by the English present perfect (have done some-thing). It is marked with -pua, from opua - "to end; finish; complete". The inchoative aspect refers to the beginning of a state [INCH]. It is marked with -mu, from mula - "to begin; start; initiate". The frequentative aspect refers to a repeated action [FREQ]. It is marked with -nua, from nua - "frequent; often; regular".

Kala gloss English
Continuous na ke niye pukunko 1s O undergarment wear-CONT I am wearing underclothes.
Frequentative tlaka ke apua tlatonua man O song recite-FREQ The man recites the song repetitively.
Inchoative nahi yotimu girl play-INCH The girl begins to play.
Perfective kam inapua 3pl eat-PFV They have eaten.

Mood

Besides various aspects, Kala also marks moods other than realis: irrealis, imperative, hortative, and negative. These are also expressed by suffixes on the verb and typically follow aspectual marking where it is expressed by a suffix. As is common throughout natural and constructed languages, the indicative mood is unmarked. Subjunctive, conditional, and imperative moods are marked lexically, by various particles, and as such are covered in detail in the “particles” of this grammar.

Kala gloss English
Abilitative na mokuyepak 1s sleep-PST-ABIL-NEG I was unable to sleep.
Assumptive naka hinaho woman be.here-ASS The woman must be here. (I assume) (also used as "assertive")
Attemptative neko ke panya matapya cat O mouse kill-ATT The cat is trying to kill the mouse.
Dubitative ha tsakahueke 3s home-LOC-DUB I guess he is at home. lit: He is at home, supposedly.
Hortative yalakya walk-HORT Let's go!
Necessitative mita inahe dog eat-NEC The dog needs to eat.
Negative naku nayo hinak sister 1s-GEN be.here-NEG My sister is not here.
Permissive ta ke hina simamyok 2s O here sit-PERM-NEG You are not allowed to sit here.
Precative ke asi yetate O salt give-PREC Will you please pass me the salt?
Preparative tahi mokusue boy sleep-PREP The boy is ready to sleep.
Propositive ta mokune 2s sleep-PROP You should sleep.
Volitive otsokai ka'e moli yalaue wolf-red toward forest go-VOL Redwolf wants/intends to go to the forest.


The negative mood (always marked finally) is indicated by the suffix –k or –nke (when the last syllable contains /k/).

  • mita inayek - dog eat-PST-NEG - The dog did not eat.
  • mita mokunke - dog sleep-NEG - The dog does not sleep.

Adjectives

Kala does not have morphologically distinct adjectives. Stative verbs are the words that modify nouns in an predicative and often adjectival way. They often express a state like a quality or result. In the simplest form, the adjective simply appears after the noun, in verbal position. Many statements that would be phrased as adjectival predicates in English are preferably expressed with stative intransitive verbs in Kala, requiring no copula. (For simplicity, such verbs are glossed without “be”.) This leaves open to interpretation many phrases.

  • mita inya - dog hungry
    • The dog hungers.
    • The dog is hungry.
    • The hungry dog.
    • A hungry dog.
  • tsaka ketlahi - house red-DIM
    • The house is a little red.
    • The light-red house.
    • A pale red house.
  • taki saua - coat wet
    • The coat is wet.
    • The wet coat.
    • A wet coat.
  • umalo tahaku - horse-PL big-extreme
    • The horses are extremely large.
    • The very big horses.


Comparison

In Kala the concepts of comparative and superlative degree of an predicative adjective (verb) are merged into a single form, the elative. How this form is understood or translated depends upon context and definiteness. In the absence of comparison, the elative conveys the notion of “greatest”, “supreme.” The comparative is made by using the augmentative or diminutive ending on the verb.

  • tsaka hayo ke nayo tahaka
house 3sg.POSS O 1sg.POSS big-AUG
His house is bigger than mine.
  • ke mauam tayo yanaha
O flower.PL 2sg.POSS yellow-AUG
Your flowers are the most yellow.
  • iyapo ke tsaka tayo pakoha
PROX-building O home 2sg new-AUG
This building is newer than your home.


Equivalence

Equivalence is indicated with either kue (as, like), or mya (as...as).

  • tsaka hayo kue nayo ketla
house 3s.GEN as 1s.GEN be.red
His house is red like mine.
  • tsaka hayo mya nayo ketla
house 3s.GEN as.X.as 1s.GEN be.red
His house is as red as mine.


Like verbs, adjectives can be used as nouns. For example, aya means "beautiful", but ayako means "a beautiful one" or "a beauty." An adjective can be made into an abstract noun by adding -n (-ity, -ness, -ship, -hood). In this way aya becomes ayan, meaning "beauty". This can also be used with nouns: ona (mother) becomes onan (motherhood).

Relative

In a relative clause, the verb has the suffix -tle (or -le if the final syllable contains /tl/) added to it. The order of the words in relative clauses remains the same as in regular clauses. The use of participles in Kala is rather different than in English and at first sight is difficult to understand. This is mainly due to the fact that the relative pronouns who, what, which, where are not used in Kala as in English.

  • yalapa - to be able to walk produces: yalapatle - who/which/that can walk
  • yalapak - to not be able to walk produces: yalapanketle - who/which/that can't walk

This nominalizes the verb in some cases, and makes it possible for it to be either the subject or the object.

  • na ke tlaka nya inama talatle unya
1sg O man for eat-time come-REL know
I know the man who is coming to lunch.
  • ke naka patlole pako
O woman sweep-REL young
The woman who is sweeping is young.

The relative suffix is most often in the final position. In some cases, it may be followed by the negative -k.

  • itsaka na sutahuetle
PROX-house 1sg reside-LOC-REL
This is the house in which I live.
  • itsaka na sutahueyetlek
PROX-house 1sg reside-LOC-PST-REL-NEG
This is the house in which I did not live.


Adverbs

Adverbs tell us when, how, why or where the action happens. They modify a verb, a noun, an adjective, another adverb or a complete sentence. They also can provide us information about manner, quantity, frequency, time, or place. Kala does not have morphologically distinct adverbs. Adverbs can be formed from all adjectives (or stative verbs) by adding -n to the root. Since this rule is regular, it is not generally indicated in grammatical examples or in the lexicon.

  • aya - beautiful >> ayan - beautifully
  • tama - good >> taman - well
  • poyo - rich >> poyon - richly
  • tsipue - slow >> tsipuen - slowly (this can also be marked on the main verb with -tsue)
  • tlaki - fluent >> tlakin - fluently

Many adverbs (mostly temporal) do not derive from verbs:

  • yomaye - yesterday
  • iyoma - today
  • yomali - every day
  • kuama - always
  • ima - now

Temporal adverbs always precede the phrase they modify.

  • yomuali na ka'e hakyo yala
morning-each 1s to school go
I go to school every morning.

Other adverbials can be marked on the verb.

  • ona kamyo ma'a siku kupayetsua
mother 3pl.GEN with accident die-PST-almost
Their mother almost died in the accident.

Prepositions

Kala does not have prepositions (or postpositions) as a distinct part of speech. Instead, many locative verbs can be used as adpositionals, in which case they precede the noun they modify. There is one general locative (-hue) which is affixed to nouns (and occasionally verbs) to indicate the sense of “at; in; on”. Here are some common verbs used as adpositions:

  • pahe - against; touching
  • pa'e - apart from; other than; except for
  • paye - beyond; exceeding; farther than
  • pue - after; back; behind; rear
  • tahe - below; beneath; under
  • ka'e - to; towards; at [moving toward]
  • kaye - around; encircling; surrounding
  • mahe - around; approximate; close to
  • ma'a - with [accompanied by / furnished with]
  • ma'e - before; in front
  • maye - between; among
  • nahe - in [located inside of]; internal
  • nyaue - outside of; exterior to
  • sahe - across; opposite; other side
  • saye - along; following [a line]
  • hue / -hue - at [in the same location as] [LOC]
  • tsa'e - across; through
  • ua'e - above; over / on
  • uaye - from [moving out of or away from]
  • ya'e - near; close to
  • yomo - to the right of
  • yoso - to the left of


  • na ke ito yamahue anyapa
1sg O tree hill-LOC see-ABIL
I can see a tree on the hill.
  • ntahim nyaue tsaka yoti
child-PL outside.of house play
The children are playing outside of the house.

Many of these take the motive suffix -la.

  • mita ke tsaka nahelaye
dog O house go.into-PST
The dog went into the house.
  • taku nayo ke ito ua'ela
brother 1s.GEN O tree go.up
My brother is climbing the tree.

Particles

Conjunctions

Words and phrases may be coordinated in Kala with the following words:

  • pa - although; even though; even if
  • ku - and; also [clause level]
  • ma - and; also; too; as well
  • ehe / (me) - but; yet; however
  • impo - therefore; as a result; so; consequently; thus
  • ua - or; other; else
  • ue - either X or Y
  • uenke / (uek) - neither X nor Y
  • yatli - if X then Y
  • yema - both X and Y
  • yetli - if it were not; if not X then Y >> X yatli Y
  • ha'ena itsa ehe hinak
3s-P.1s love but be.here-NEG
She loves me but is not here.
  • aye na tala ku matsu
PST 1s come CONJ conquer
I came, I conquered.


Interjections

There are a few particles, usually appearing at the beginning of the sentence, with a pragmatic meaning. These typically precede phrases they modify.

morpheme indicates gloss example
a acknowledgement, agreement, or that one is listening yes; hm mm; yeah a ta inaue
Mm hmm...You want to eat.
e filler or pause during conversation uh, er, well e na uamek
Well, I'm not sure.
yali excuses jostling or interruptions excuse me yali itla tayo ka
Excuse me, is this yours?

Derivational morphology

Because Kala has only two main parts of speech (content and functional words), new words formed by derivation should be analyzed based on context. Functional words can rarely be used to form new words, but this is typically to form extensions of functions, or new functions.

Compounding

New nouns are usually created through head-initial compounding, using both nominal and verbal stems as the second, dependent element of the compound. The resulting lexical entries usually behave as single phonological words, which, however, have four full syllables: kuatlatloha "grass snake". Compounding of more than two elements is not common.

  • kayapusu - "earthquake" > kaya - earth + pusu - vibrate
  • asuaseka - "leather" > asua - skin + seka - dry

There are also numerous affixes used to form new meanings. A few examples are;

  • tiyasu - "bakery" > tiya - bread + -su - market; shop
  • onyomo - "school" > onyo - learn + -mo - place; location
  • kuhasa - "kitchen" > kuha - cook + -sa - room; chamber
  • pyetampu - "egg-shaped" > pyeta - egg + -mpu - shape; form

Derivation

Verbalization

Causative verbs (as well as achievement verbs) can be formed from other verbs by adding -mya (from muya - "do, make, cause") or -la (from ela - "become; change into; turn into"). This type of derivation is fairly common; however, verbs created in this way are syntactically defective and tend to appear only in serial verb constructions.

  • tinamya - "bend" < tina - be bent
  • pitamya - "hollow out" < pita - be hollow; void
  • enomya - "annoy, bother" < eno - be angry
  • tsipuela - "slow down" < tsipue - be slow
  • kyolola - "speed up" < kyolo - be quick

Intensive verbs can be formed from other verbs by adding -mpa (from mpa - "many; much; very"), or more commonly -hu (from kyohu - "be drastic; extreme; aggressive").

  • ketsahu - "dismiss, reject, repudiate" < ketsa - doubt
  • amyampa - "fall in love with" < amya - be fond of; like; prefer (of people)

Adjective-like stative verbs which name an associated quality may be formed from nouns by -n (from no - "thing" (-ish, -ly, -ous)).

  • timan - "be cruel, be bloody" < tima - blood
  • amyan - "be welcoming, be hospitable" < amya - be fond of

Nominalization

Agentive

Nouns referring to a human subject of a verb (usually in a habitual sense) can be formed with the agentive suffix -ko (from ko - "individual; person"). This suffix changes to -tlo when a velar stop is present in the preceding syllable.

  • kitlako - "craftsman" < kitla - create; invent; make-up
  • sutako - "inhabitant (of)" < suta - live; reside; dwell; inhabit; settle
  • yekatlo - "unmarried young adult" < yeka - be separate, be on one's own
  • makatlo - "musician" < maka - music; play ~; tune
  • tsaniko - "storyteller" < tsani - recite, tell (a story)

Instrumental

Instrument nouns and names for tools and other inanimates can be derived from verbs or from other nouns by adding the suffix -nyo (from mayo - "device; equipment; tool").

  • hitanyo - "atlatl (spear-thrower)" < hita - throw; cast; expel
  • amonyo - "handle (for carrying)" < amo - transport; carry
  • kusunyo - "clasp, brooch, fibula" < kusu - squeeze
  • toponyo - "lock" < topo - door; gate

Locative

Location nouns can be formed from both nouns and verbs by several suffixes. These indicate specific places where either something happens, or something resides there are a few affixes which modify both verbs and nouns.

-mo (from mo - "location; place; site"). This suffix is used to form the general idea of where something happens or resides.

  • tanamo - "battlefield; boxing ring; wrestling mat, etc." < tana - fight; combat
  • uelomo - "bicycle-place; bike path; bike rack, etc." < uelo - bicycle; bike
  • inamo - "eat-place; dining room; restaurant" [This can also mean food-place; pantry, etc.] < ina - food; eat
  • onyomo - "learn-place; school" < onyo - learn; study

-su (from suku - "market; shop; store"). This suffix is used to specify a business where items are produced and/or sold.

  • tiyasu - "bread-shop; bakery" < tiya - bread
  • inasu - "food-market; grocery store; restaurant" < ina - food; eat
  • uelosu - "bicycle-shop" < uelo - bicycle; bike

-kyo (from hakyo - "college; school; university"). This suffix is used to specify a location where students learn. This can also be used to indicate a school of thought, or ideology.

  • tanakyo - "dojo; martial arts training academy; etc." < tana - fight; combat
  • kuhakyo - "culinary-school; chef’s academy" < kuha - cook; prepare food
  • tsiyakyo - "liberalism" < tsiya - freedom; liberty
  • ya'akyo - "medical-school" < ya'a - medicine; drug; cure

-sa (from sala - "chamber; room; section"). This is more specific than -mo and used primarily for spaces inside buildings.

  • kuhasa - "cook-room; kitchen" < kuha - cook; prepare food
  • mokusa - "sleep-room; bedroom" < moku - sleep; rest
  • inasa - "eat-room; dining room" < ina - food; eat
  • onyosa - "learn-room; classroom" < onyo - learn; study

Diminutive

Diminutive nouns and endearment terms can be formed from verbs and other nouns by adding the suffix -hi (from ahi - "few; small"). This becomes -ki after a syllable that contains a glottal fricative.

  • mukuhi - "blade" < muku - knife
  • umahi - "foal" < uma - horse; equine
  • mitahi - "puppy" < mita - dog; canine
  • ohuaki - "indulgence" < ohua - luxurious; extravagant

Augmentative

Augmentative nouns can be formed from verbs and other nouns by adding the suffix -ha (from taha - "big; large; grand"). This becomes -ka after a syllable that contains a glottal fricative.

  • kamaha - "city" < kama - village; town
  • ohaka - "dislocate one's jaw" < oha - yawn; open one's mouth
  • mosaha - "epic; novel" < mosa - book; letter; scroll
  • tiniha - "hurricane" < tini - spiral; whorl

Honorific

Honorific nouns can be formed from other nouns by prefixing o-.

  • omasa - "stag" < masa - deer; cervine
  • okama - "capital" < kama - town; village

Syntax

Kala has an extremely regular grammar, with very few exceptions to its rules. Sentences are made up of one or more phrases. Each phrase consists of a verb (optionally followed by modifying particles) and a subject (optionally followed by modifying particles). The subject, if understood, can be omitted at the end of an utterance: pana ("It is raining.") pana! ("Rain!") An utterance can be anything from an interjection to a story.

The basic structure of a Kala sentence is: AGENT--PATIENT--VERB (or SOV)

The agent is the person or thing doing the action described by the verb; The patient is the recipient of that action. The importance of word order can be seen by comparing the following sentences:

  • mita tlaka anya
dog man see
The dog sees the man.
  • tlaka mita anya
man dog see
The man sees the dog.

In both sentences, the words are identical, the only way to know who is seeing whom is by the order of the words in the sentence.

The use of the object marker ke indicates the recipient of the action.

  • naka ke mita itsa
woman O dog love
The woman loves the dog.

Kala lacks morphological adjectives and instead uses predicative verbs.

  • ke tsaka taha
O house be.big
The big house / The house is big

Kala lacks morphological adverbs, verbs modified with the adverbial ending -n tend to precede the verb phrase they modify.

  • tsumun nam yokone
cautious-ADV 1pl swim-SUG
We should swim cautiously.

Kala lacks morphological prepositions and instead uses locational and relational verbs.

  • mita ke yempa tahe
dog O table be.under
The dog is under the table.

Clauses

Relative clauses (or adjective clauses) function like adjectives. Relative clauses follow the noun or noun phrase that they modify:

  • naka ke na itsatle te ameyo
woman O 1s love-REL from America
The woman (that) I love comes from America.
  • mayo ke na kitlayetle muyak
tool O 1s create-PST-REL do-NEG
The tool (that) I built doesn't function.
  • na ke ta yani unyak / na ke yani tayo unyak
1s O 2s mean know-NEG / 1s O meaning 2s.GEN know-NEG
I don't understand what you mean.

Subordinate clauses rely on conjunctions and other particles.

  • eya ta ke mpeka inaye yatli ta pasala
maybe 2s O toad eat-PST therefore 2s nauseous-become
If you ate the toad (which you might have), you might get sick.
  • naye na tasa ke masa okyohue anyaye
while 1s hunt O deer clearing-LOC see-PST
While hunting, I saw a deer in a clearing.

Questions

There are two types of questions: Polar, those which may be answered "yes" or "no," and those which require explanations as answers.

Polar Questions

Any statement can become a polar question by adding the interrogative particle ka at the end of the sentence.

  • mita ina
dog eat
The dog eats.
  • nta'i moku
baby sleep
The baby is sleeping. / The baby sleeps.
  • ta ke tlo'o anyaye
2SG O elephant see-PST
You saw the elephant.
  • tekatlo eta ke ya'a yetaye
heal-AG P.2SG O medicine give-PST
The doctor gave you the medicine.
  • mita ina ka
dog eat Q
Does the dog eat?
  • nta'i moku ka
baby sleep Q
Is the baby sleeping?
  • ta ke tlo'o anyaye ka
2SG O elephant see-PST Q
Did you see the elephant?
  • tekatlo eta ke ya'a yetaye ka
heal-AG P.2SG O medicine give-PST Q
Did the doctor give you the medicine?

Content questions

Questions that give a list of possible answers are formed like polar questions, with the conjunction ue ‘or’ introducing each alternative (which must appear in the form of a noun phrase).

  • ta ke nkapa ue maya inuue ka
2SG O beer or.EXCL water drink-VOL Q
Do you want to drink beer or water?
  • uala ta ke sinka mataye ue empa ma koma ka
truly 2sg O lion kill-PST or.EXCL flee CONJ hide Q
Did you really kill the lion, or did you run away and hide?

Open content questions are most easily formed with the correlatives, such as ko ‘person’, mo ‘place’, to ‘manner’, etc. These correlatives always appear clause-initially:

  • ko ta ka
person 2sg Q
Who are you?
  • itla ka
this Q
What is this?
  • to kihu ka
manner weather Q
What's the weather like?

The other type contains a question word and is followed by ka:

kanyo
Kala gloss English
object ke mita ina ka O dog eat Q What does the dog eat?
person ko ina ka person eat Q Who eats?
possession koyo mita ina ka person-POSS dog eat Q Whose dog eats?
manner to mita ina ka manner dog eat Q How does the dog eat?
place mo mita ina ka place dog eat Q Where does the dog eat?
reason nye mita ina ka reason dog eat Q Why does the dog eat?
time ama mita ina ka time dog eat Q When does the dog eat?
amount uku mita ina ka amount dog eat Q How much/many does the dog eat?
which ula mita ina ka any dog eat Q Which dog eats?

Semantic fields and pragmatics

Writing system

Kala conscripts are many and varied. Rather than multiple pages explaining each of them, this page serves as a working list with a consistent example across each script. The most commonly used script is the Hangul adaptation for Kala.

Han Moya

Han Moya is an adaptation of Hangul for writing Kala. It is written horizontally, in lines running from left to right. It can also be written vertically in columns.

consonants

  • ㄱㄲㄴㄷㄸㄹㅁㅂㅃㅅㅆㅇㅈㅉㅊㅋㅌㅍㅎ
k nk n t nt l m p mp s ns a ts nts ts` k` tl p` h
/k~g ᵑk~ⁿg n t~d ⁿt~ⁿd l~ɾ m p~b ᵐp~ᵐb s~ʃ ⁿs~ⁿʃ - ts~t͡ʃ ⁿts~ⁿt͡ʃ tsʰ~t͡ʃʰ kʰ t͡ɬ~tl pʰ h~ɦ/

The adaptations of doubled consonants are used word initially to indicate prenasalization. Medial occurrences of nasalized syllables are written across syllables.

Example:
  • 까바 - nkapa - alcohol; liquor / 단가 - tanka - eagle; hawk; falcon
  • 감바 - kampa - Cheers! / 쁘라 - mpula - lamp; lantern; light

vowels

  • ㅏ ᅶ ㅐ ㅑ ᅸ ㅓ ㅕ ㅗ ㅛ ㅜ ㅟ ㅠ ㅡ ㅣ
a ao ai ya yao e ye o ao yo ua uai ue u i
/a~a: aʊ̯ aɪ̯ ja~ʲa: jaʊ̯~ʲaʊ̯ e~ɛ je~ʲɛ o~o: jo~ʲo: wa~ʷa: waɪ̯~ʷaɪ̯ we~ʷe: u~u: i~ɪ/
  • This is pronounced /wa/ in Korean because of the order of the vowels; however, because obsolete jamo are difficult to type and look junky as images, in Kala, this is used for /aʊ̯/ when typing. It is rarely seen due to the diphthong itself being uncommon.

Examples

Numbers

Kala uses a base 10 number system. The basic numbers are as follows:

Kala number English Kala number English Kala number English
ye'o 0 zero tsa'o 6 six nya'o 500 five hundred
na'o 1 one ka'o 7 seven tle'o 103 (one) thousand
ta'o 2 two pa'o 8 eight mue'o 104 ten thousand
ha'o 3 three sa'o 9 nine kye'o 105 (one) hundred thousand
ma'o 4 four ue'o 10 ten nte'o 106 (one) million
ya'o 5 five nye'o 100 (one) hundred hue'o 109 (one) billion

Forming Larger Numbers

  • uena'o - eleven / 11
  • taue'o - twenty / 20
  • nyeka'o - one hundred seven / 107
  • hanyetauetsa'o (long form) / hatatsa'o (short form) - three hundred twenty six / 326
  • tsatletauema'o - six thousand and twenty four / 6024

Long form numbers are used in formal situations, including financial transactions, especially involving large sums. Short form numbers are used in everyday speech and when calculating basic math.

Other Number Forms

Kala number English ordinal multiple fractional
na'o 1 one kina'o
first
tina'o
once
-
ueta'o 12 twelve kiueta'o
twelfth
tiueta'o
duodecuple
iueta'o
a twelfth
yauema'o
(yama'o)
54 fifty four kiyama'o
fifty fourth
tiyama'o
54 times
iyama'o
a fifty fourth
nyetsa'o 106 one hundred (and) six kinyetsa'o
106th
tinyetsa'o
106 times
inyetsa'o
a 106th
katle'o 7000 seven thousand kikatle'o
seven thousandth
tikatle'o
7000 times
ikatle'o
1/7000

Math Operations

Kala math is fairly basic and relies on particles and verbs to express functions.

Addition uses ma (and; also). There is no specific order to the numbers in the phrase/equation.

  • ha'o ma ya'o ke pa'o a
3 and 5 O 8 COP
Three plus five is eight.

Subtraction uses ma (and; also) and a negative form of the smaller integer. There is no specific order to the numbers in the phrase/equation.

  • tsa'o ma ya'ok ke na'o a
6 and 5-NEG O 1 COP
Six and five-less is one.

Multiplication uses ma (and; also) and a multiple form of one of the integers. There is no specific order to the numbers in the phrase/equation.

  • ha'o ma tima'o ke ueta'o a
3 and multiple-4 O 12 COP
Three times four is twelve.

Division uses yeka (divide; separate; partition), or ma (and; also) and a multiple-negative form of one of the integers. There is no specific order to the numbers in the phrase/equation.

  • tama'o ma ha'o ke pa'o yeka
24 and 3 O 8 division
Twenty-four divided by three is eight.

Lexicon

references

  • akana.conlang.org/wiki/Ronc_Tyu
  • en.wikibooks.org/wiki/Na’vi
  • frathwiki.com/Kala
  • hungarianreference.com/
  • japaneselanguageguide.com/grammar/noun.asp
  • kinezika.info/pdf/ChineseEssentialGrammar.pdf
  • klingonwiki.net/En/Math
  • ossicone.com/conlangs/uskra
  • pomax.github.io/nrGrammar/
  • resources.allsetlearning.com/chinese/grammar/
  • rickmor.x10.mx/arabic_morphology.html
  • turkishlanguage.co.uk/
  • zompist.com/kitgram.html
  • zompist.com/wedei.html
  • languagesgulper.com/eng/Quechua.html